Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-11T08:00:09.629Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Studying the relationship between intelligence quotient and schizophrenia polygenic scores in a family design with first-episode psychosis population

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2024

Nancy Murillo-García
Affiliation:
Research Group on Mental Illnesses, Valdecilla Biomedical Research (IDIVAL), Santander, Spain Department of Molecular Biology, School of Medicine, University of Cantabria, Santander, Spain
Sergi Papiol
Affiliation:
Department of Falkai, Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, Munich, Germany Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Health Institute Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Luis Manuel Fernández-Cacho
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, Marqués de Valdecilla University Hospital, Santander, Spain Faculty of Nursing, University of Cantabria, Santander, Spain
Mar Fatjó-Vilas
Affiliation:
Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Health Institute Carlos III, Madrid, Spain FIDMAG Sisters Hospitallers Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain Departament de Biologia Evolutiva, Ecologia i Ciències Ambientals, Facultat de Biologia, Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Rosa Ayesa-Arriola*
Affiliation:
Research Group on Mental Illnesses, Valdecilla Biomedical Research (IDIVAL), Santander, Spain Department of Molecular Biology, School of Medicine, University of Cantabria, Santander, Spain Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Health Institute Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Rosa Ayesa Arriola; Email: rayesa@humv.es

Abstract

Background

The intelligence quotient (IQ) of patients with first-episode psychosis (FEP) and their unaffected relatives may be related to the genetic burden of schizophrenia (SCZ). The polygenic score approach can be useful for testing this question.

Aim

To assess the contribution of the polygenic risk scores for SCZ (PGS-SCZ) and polygenic scores for IQ (PGS-IQ) to the individual IQ and its difference from the mean IQ of the family (named family-IQ) through a family-based design in an FEP sample.

Methods

The PAFIP-FAMILIES sample (Spain) consists of 122 FEP patients, 131 parents, 94 siblings, and 176 controls. They all completed the WAIS Vocabulary subtest for IQ estimation and provided a DNA sample. We calculated PGS-SCZ and PGS-IQ using the continuous shrinkage method. To account for relatedness in our sample, we performed linear mixed models. We controlled for covariates potentially related to IQ, including age, years of education, sex, and ancestry principal components.

Results

FEP patients significantly deviated from their family-IQ. FEP patients had higher PGS-SCZ than other groups, whereas the relatives had intermediate scores between patients and controls. PGS-IQ did not differ between groups. PGS-SCZ significantly predicted the deviation from family-IQ, whereas PGS-IQ significantly predicted individual IQ.

Conclusions

PGS-SCZ discriminated between different levels of genetic risk for the disorder and was specifically related to patients’ lower IQ in relation to family-IQ. The genetic background of the disorder may affect neurocognition through complex pathological processes interacting with environmental factors that prevent the individual from reaching their familial cognitive potential.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Psychiatric Association

Introduction

Intelligence quotient (IQ) is a quantitative estimate of an individual’s general cognitive ability [Reference Wechsler1]. Patients experiencing a first-episode psychosis (FEP) tend to have lower IQs than healthy controls [Reference Kendler, Turkheimer, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2,Reference Khandaker, Barnett, White and Jones3]. It has also been described that these IQ deficits precede the onset of psychosis, probably due to neurodevelopmental impairments [Reference Cosway, Byrne, Clafferty, Hodges, Grant and Abukmeil4,Reference Jones, Rodgers, Murray and Marmot5]. While cognitive abilities aggregate in families, FEP patients tend to perform worse on cognitive tasks than their first-degree relatives, indicating a deviation from familial cognitive aptitude [Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Aguilera, Campanera, Fatjó-Vilas, Guitart and Miret6Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Mezuk, Sundquist and Sundquist10]. Accordingly, IQ and specific neuropsychological functions have been largely investigated as endophenotypic traits of psychosis that may enhance preventive measures and early intervention [Reference Bertisch, Mesen-Fainardi, Martin, Pérez-Vargas, Vargas-Rodríguez and Delgado11Reference Zhang, Zhang, Qin and Tan14].

Both IQ and psychosis are highly heritable, with heritability estimates ranging from 40% to 70% [Reference Haworth, Wright, Luciano, Martin, de Geus and van Beijsterveldt15,Reference Willoughby, McGue, Iacono and Lee16] and 60% to 80% [Reference Owen, Sawa and Mortensen17,Reference Hilker, Helenius, Fagerlund, Skytthe, Christensen and Werge18], respectively. The polygenic score (PGS) method is useful for estimating an individual’s genetic make-up for such complex phenotypes [Reference Genç, Schlüter, Fraenz, Arning, Metzen and Nguyen19,Reference Martin, Daly, Robinson, Hyman and Neale20]. On the one hand, it is possible to calculate PGS-IQ based on the results of large-scale genome-wide studies that have characterised the genetic architecture of intelligence [Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois and de Leeuw21]. PGS-IQ is strongly correlated with crystallised intelligence and accounts for up to 5.1% of the variance in general cognitive ability [Reference Loughnan, Palmer, Thompson, Dale, Jernigan and Chieh Fan22]. On the other hand, polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia (PGS-SCZ) can be calculated leveraging the results of genome-wide studies on this disorder [Reference Trubetskoy, Pardiñas, Qi, Panagiotaropoulou, Awasthi and Bigdeli23,24]. PGS-SCZ explain between 2.4% and 7.3% of the variance in SCZ on the liability scale [Reference Trubetskoy, Pardiñas, Qi, Panagiotaropoulou, Awasthi and Bigdeli23,24] and is increased in FEP patients compared to controls [Reference Ferraro, Quattrone, La Barbera, La Cascia, Morgan and Jb25,Reference Harrisberger, Smieskova, Vogler, Egli, Schmidt and Lenz26]. There may be a certain degree of association between these two PGSs, given that numerous genetic variants have been identified as contributing factors to intelligence and SCZ [Reference Hill, Davies, Liewald, McIntosh and Deary27,Reference Murillo-García, Barrio-Martínez, Setién-Suero, Soler, Papiol and Fatjó-Vilas28]. Similarly, PGS discriminating SCZ from bipolar disorder was found to be specifically related to intelligence [Reference Ohi, Nishizawa, Sugiyama, Takai, Kuramitsu and Hasegawa29].

We hypothesised that i) FEP patients would have higher PGS-SCZ and lower PGS-IQ than first-degree relatives and healthy controls, and ii) PGS-SCZ would be negatively associated with IQ and the patient’s IQ deviation from the mean score of their family (named family-IQ), suggesting that genetic predisposition to SCZ is related to worse general cognitive ability. We also expected a positive association of PGS-IQ with IQ.

Our primary aim was to test whether the genetic risk for SCZ, as determined by PGS-SCZ, might be associated with IQ and contributed to patient-specific differences from their family-IQ in a sample of FEP patients, their first-degree relatives, and healthy controls. Second, we also aimed to examine to what extent PGS-IQ predicts intelligence and deviation from family-IQ.

Methods

Sample

Participants were drawn from PAFIP-FAMILIES, a family-based study carried out in Cantabria, Spain, from January 2018 to March 2021, funded by the ISCIII (FIS PI17/00221). All participants were of European ancestry. We recruited first-degree relatives of a cohort of FEP patients previously enrolled in the Cantabria Program for Early Intervention in Psychosis (PAFIP) [Reference Ayesa-Arriola, Ortíz-García de la Foz, Martínez-García, Setién-Suero, Ramírez and Suárez-Pinilla30,Reference Rodríguez-Sánchez, Setién-Suero, Suárez-Pinilla, Mayoral Van Son, Vázquez-Bourgon and Gil López31]. The local institutional review committee (CEIm Cantabria) approved both projects (PAFIP and PAFIP-FAMILIES) under international research ethics standards and all participants gave their written informed consent. The initial sample consisted of 133 FEP patients, 146 parents, 98 siblings, and 202 controls [Reference Murillo-García, Díaz-Pons, Fernández-Cacho, Miguel-Corredera, Martínez-Barrio and Ortiz-García de la Foz32].

FEP patients

The PAFIP program was carried out at the University Hospital Marqués de Valdecilla (Santander, Spain) from 2001 to 2018. FEP patients were referred from the inpatient unit, outreach mental health services, and healthcare centres in the region. Inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) 15–60 years of age; 2) living within the recruitment area; 3) experiencing an FEP; 4) no prior treatment with antipsychotic medication or if previously treated, a total lifetime of antipsychotic treatment of <6 weeks; and 5) criteria for brief psychotic disorder, schizophreniform disorder, SCZ, or not otherwise specified psychosis according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV). The exclusion criteria included meeting the DSM-IV criteria for drug or alcohol dependence, having an intellectual disability, and having a history of neurological disease or head injury.

First-degree relatives

We contacted the parents and siblings of the eligible patients (those with neuropsychological data and DNA samples) and invited them to participate in the study. Inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) age over 15 years, 2) good domain of the Spanish language, and 3) ability to give informed consent in writing. Exclusion criteria included a history of psychiatric diagnosis related to psychotic illness spectrum, organic brain pathology, and intellectual disability or substance use disorders according to DSM-V criteria.

Controls

Controls were retrieved from the PAFIP program, which recruited healthy individuals through advertisements from the local community. They met the same inclusion and exclusion criteria as first-degree relatives. The psychiatric history of controls and relatives was screened by the abbreviated version of the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History [Reference Andreasen33], a semi-structured psychiatric interview that inquires about the presence of clinical symptoms for mania, depression, and positive, disorganised, and negative dimensions of psychosis.

Phenotypic data

Sociodemographic data

We recorded the sex, age, and completed years of formal education of all participants. Cannabis consumption was recorded for FEP patients, siblings, and controls.

Clinical data

We obtained clinical data from patients at baseline through medical records and interviews. The age at psychosis onset was defined as the age when the emergence of the first continuous psychotic symptom occurred. Duration of untreated illness was defined as the time from the first nonspecific symptom related to psychosis. Duration of untreated psychosis was established as the time from the first continuous psychotic symptom to initiation of antipsychotic drug treatment. Patients were randomly assigned to treatment with olanzapine, risperidone, or haloperidol [Reference Gómez-Revuelta, Pelayo-Terán, Juncal-Ruiz, Vázquez-Bourgon, Suárez-Pinilla and Romero-Jiménez34]. Positive symptoms were assessed by the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms [Reference Andreasen35], and negative symptoms by the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms [Reference Andreasen36]. Functioning was rated by the Global Assessment of Functioning [Reference Hall37]. Diagnoses were confirmed through the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I) conducted by an experienced psychiatrist within 6 months of the baseline visit.

Estimation of IQ

Expert neuropsychologists administered the WAIS-III Vocabulary subtest [Reference Wechsler1] to estimate the IQ of all participants. This subtest has adequate properties as a proxy measure for crystallised intelligence in the general population and FEP [Reference Lezak, Howieson, Loring and Fischer38]. Crystallised intelligence is defined as knowledge acquired throughout life, including vocabulary, general information, culture, and specific skills [Reference Cattell39]. It represents the stored information and strategies that individuals draw on to solve common problems [Reference Ellingsen and Ackerman40]. Crystallised intelligence is more stable than fluid intelligence [Reference de Oliveira, Nitrini, Yassuda and Brucki41]; thus, the Vocabulary subtest would enable the estimation of cognitive abilities before the onset of psychosis in the FEP sample. This subtest is associated with educational attainment and the linguistic knowledge of one’s native language [Reference de Oliveira, Nitrini, Yassuda and Brucki41]. We have previously used Vocabulary as a proxy measure for premorbid intelligence, showing utility in studying the IQ of FEP patients [Reference Ayesa-Arriola, Setién-Suero, Neergaard, Belzunces, Contreras and van Haren42].

To estimate a proxy of the potential IQ of FEP patients, we calculated a “family-IQ” for each family. This score represents the mean IQ of all family members, including the FEP patient themself. We included patients in the estimation because 42% of our families consisted of only the proband and one other member (see Figure 1). See the details of family-IQ estimated from unaffected relatives only in the Supplementary Material.

Figure 1. Conformation of the families participating in this study.

Note: Each family was formed by a FEP patient and at least one first-degree relative, either a parent or sibling. All participants completed the same neuropsychological battery and provided a DNA sample that allowed the calculation of polygenic scores. *There was one family with nine members, one with six members, and five with five members.

Deviation from family-IQ was determined by calculating the difference between the individual and family scores. Positive deviations indicate that an individual’s IQ is above their family-IQ, while negative deviations indicate that it is below their family-IQ.

Genotyping and PGSs estimation

DNA was extracted from venous blood samples at baseline. Samples and data from patients included in this study were provided by the Biobank Valdecilla (PT20/00067), integrated into the Spanish Biobank Network and they were processed following standard operating procedures with the appropriate approval of the Ethical and Scientific Committees. The genotyping was performed at the Centro Nacional de Genotipado (Human Genotyping laboratory, CeGen) using the Global Screening Array v.3.0 panel (Illumina).

The quality control process was performed using PLINK 1.9. Single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) with a minor allele frequency of less than 0.01, missing data exceeding 0.02, or exhibiting deviation from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium were removed. Participants were excluded if there were discrepancies in sex information or detected heterozygosity. A set of SNPs meeting high-quality criteria (HWE p > 0.001, MAF > 0.01) and subjected to linkage disequilibrium pruning was employed to assess relatedness. We confirmed the participants’ recorded relationships, in which PI-HAT values around 0.50 were considered to indicate first-degree relatives. Ancestry outliers were identified through principal component analysis based on 1000 Genomes Project European reference populations and subsequently removed (see Supplementary Figure S1). The final dataset comprised 525 participants and 492,348 SNPs. Genetic imputation was carried out in the Michigan Imputation Server using Minimac4 and individuals from the Haplotype Reference Consortium (HRC; Version r1.1) as the reference dataset. Genetic variants with MAF > 0.01 were kept. After imputation, 6,910,431 SNPs were available for downstream analyses.

We calculated PGS for each participant using the latest publicly available summary statistics for SCZ [Reference Trubetskoy, Pardiñas, Qi, Panagiotaropoulou, Awasthi and Bigdeli23] and IQ [Reference Savage, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, Bryois and de Leeuw21] by the method of polygenic continuous shrinkage (PGS-CS) [Reference Ge, Chen, Ni, Feng and Smoller43]. PGS-CS shrinks the effect sizes towards the population mean, thereby attenuating the influence of variants with unstable or exaggerated effects. This regularisation technique provides more reliable and interpretable PGS estimates, enhancing their predictive power and generalizability across different populations or cohorts. PGS was then calculated in PLINK 1.9 using imputed dosage data in this cohort.

After obtaining the PGS in our sample, we corrected it by their first five ancestry principal components. The aim was to control for their possible influence on our results. We regressed the effect of the principal components on the PGS using a linear model. Finally, we kept the residuals as the corrected PGS and standardised them.

Statistical analysis

We performed statistical analysis in R [44]. To take into account that our sample was related, we carried out linear mixed models (LMMs) using the “lme4” package.

(1) $$ {Y}_{ij}={\beta}_0+{\beta}_1X+{\upsilon}_i+{\varepsilon}_{ij}. $$

In Equation 1), $ Y $ represents the dependent variable. The subscripts $ i $ and $ j $ on the $ Y $ indicate that each observation $ j $ is nested within cluster $ i $ , in this case, the family. $ {\beta}_0 $ is the overall intercept. $ {\beta}_1X $ represents the vector of fixed effects. $ {\upsilon}_i $ is the random effect of family code. $ \varepsilon $ is the error of the model. We adjusted the p-values by false discovery rate (FDR) and considered those equal to or less than 0.05 as significant.

Between-group comparisons were performed using separate LMMs, one for each dependent variable (IQ, deviation from family-IQ, PGS-SCZ, PGS-IQ, and sociodemographic) according to Equation (1). These models included the grouping variable as a fixed effect (FEP patient, sibling, parent, or control) and the family code as a random effect. We covariated IQ comparisons by sex, age, and years of education. Post hoc comparisons were conducted with Bonferroni correction and effect sizes were estimated using beta standardised coefficients.

Then, we performed the main analyses, consisting of four LMMs according to Equation 1), which were fitted to families without controls. All four models included the same covariates (sex, age, and years of education) and random effect (family code). The first and second models tested the predictive effect of PGS-SCZ on IQ and deviation from family-IQ, respectively. The third and fourth models tested the predictive effect of PGS-IQ on IQ and deviation from family-IQ, respectively.

We tested the potential effect of antipsychotic medication (chlorpromazine-equivalent dose at baseline) on patients’ IQ and found no significant results (p = 0.585). Therefore, the antipsychotic variable was excluded from the main analyses.

Results

Descriptive statistics and between-group comparisons

Of all subjects with PGS estimates, five were removed from the LMM analyses because they could not be nested within families (e.g., a dyad whose family member was removed in QC becomes incomplete). The final sample consisted of 344 relatives and 176 controls. Figure 1 displays the distribution of the 121 families included in the LMMs.

There was a higher proportion of men in the FEP and control groups compared to siblings and parents (p < 0.001). FEP patients were significantly younger than all other groups and had higher rates of cannabis use than controls and siblings (p < 0.050). Siblings were significantly older than controls and had completed more years of education than the other participants had (p < 0.001).

Table 1 shows post hoc comparisons between groups. After correcting for covariates, parents had significantly higher IQs than patients (p = 0.024) and controls (p = 0.018). FEP patients deviated more from family-IQ (p < 0.001) than their relatives. The FEP patients had significantly higher PGS-SCZ than all other groups (p < 0.001), and their parents had significantly higher PGS-SCZ than controls (p = 0.023) (Figure 2). PGS-IQ was not different between groups.

Table 1. Between-group comparisons using linear mixed model analysis

Abbreviations: DUI, duration of untreated illness; DUP, duration of untreated psychosis; GAF, global assessment of functioning; IQ, intelligence quotient; NA, not available; SANS, scale for the assessment of negative symptoms, SAPS, scale for the assessment of positive symptoms.

Notes: All post hoc comparisons were Bonferroni corrected and significant at p < 0.001 except when indicated. *IQ was covariated with age and years of education.

a Controls were used as the reference category in the models (intercept). Therefore, the effect sizes of the other groups represent their differences from the controls.

b Siblings were used as the reference category in the models (intercept).

Figure 2. Violin plots of IQ, PGS-SCZ, and PGS-IQ according to the group of participants.

Note: The IQs shown in the first plot are without corrections for age and years of education. After introducing the former covariates, parents had higher IQs than FEP patients (p = 0.024) and controls (p = 0.018). Regarding PGS-SCZ, FEP patients had higher scores than all other groups (p < 0.001). No significant differences were found for PGS-IQ.

Predictive effect of the PGSs on IQ and deviation from family-IQ

PGS-SCZ was not associated with IQ (β = −0.08, SE = 0.04, p = 0.53, pFDR = 0.63). However, PGS-SCZ significantly predicted IQ deviation from family-IQ (β = −0.17, SE = 0.05, pFDR = 0.003) (see the results detailed in Table 2).

Table 2. The predictive effect of PGS-SCZ on IQ and deviation from family-IQ using linear mixed models

Overall model 1: Wald = 194.86, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.46.

Overall model 2: Wald = 115.98, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.26.

PGS-IQ significantly predicted the individual IQ (β = 0.13, SE = 0.04, pFDR = 0.003) but showed a trend towards significance in predicting the deviation from family-IQ (β = 0.08, SE = 0.04, pFDR = 0.073) (see the results detailed in Table 3).

Table 3. The predictive effect of PGS-IQ on IQ and deviation from family-IQ using linear mixed models

Overall model 1: Wald = 204.07, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.46.

Overall model 2: Wald = 104.02, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.24.

Discussion

Through a family-based design, we add data on the association of the polygenic background of SCZ and IQ with general cognitive performance. We report, as expected, that PGS-SCZ is increased in FEP patients as compared to their relatives and controls. Our data also show that PGS-SCZ significantly predicts the individual’s deviation from the mean IQ of their relatives, whereas PGS-IQ is more predictive of the individual’s IQ.

Between-group differences in IQ, PGS-SCZ, and PGS-IQ

FEP patients had higher PGS-SCZ than other groups, with first-degree relatives having intermediate scores. This supports the efficacy of the PGS method in discerning varying levels of genetic predisposition to psychosis. While previous research indicates that PGS-SCZ can differentiate between FEP patients and controls [Reference Ferraro, Quattrone, La Barbera, La Cascia, Morgan and Jb25,Reference Harrisberger, Smieskova, Vogler, Egli, Schmidt and Lenz26], our findings suggest that it can also detect genetic risk variation within families. Although FEP patients showed PGS-IQ similar to other groups, their IQ scores were lower, suggesting unachieved cognitive potential. In addition, FEP patients showed a negative deviation from their family-IQ of 6.84 points on average. This is consistent with previous research describing a strong correlation between deviation from family cognitive ability and risk of SCZ [Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Mezuk, Sundquist and Sundquist10]. Such deviation is aligned with the well-reported cognitive impairments associated with SCZ [Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Aguilera, Campanera, Fatjó-Vilas, Guitart and Miret6], bringing at the same time new questions about the aetiological mechanisms underlying the intra-family differences. Thus, deviation from familial aptitude emerges as an important marker of neurodevelopmental processes predisposing to psychosis [Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Mezuk, Sundquist and Sundquist10].

We found that unaffected siblings have a lower PGS-SCZ than the proband, implying a slightly reduced genetic predisposition to SCZ. Siblings had similar IQs to controls, and their performance aligned with their family cognitive profile. Previous research consistently shows that siblings tend to perform better than the proband in cognitive domains such as executive functions and memory [Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Aguilera, Campanera, Fatjó-Vilas, Guitart and Miret6,Reference Murillo-García, Díaz-Pons, Fernández-Cacho, Miguel-Corredera, Martínez-Barrio and Ortiz-García de la Foz32,Reference Kuha, Tuulio-Henriksson, Eerola, Perälä, Suvisaari and Partonen45Reference Scala, Lasalvia, Seidman, Cristofalo, Bonetto and Ruggeri47]. Siblings had higher educational attainment and lower cannabis use rates (Table 1), which may be protective factors that increase cognitive reserve against psychosis [Reference Ayesa-Arriola, de la, Murillo-García, Vázquez-Bourgon, Juncal-Ruiz and Gómez-Revuelta48,Reference Magdaleno Herrero, Murillo García, Yorca Ruiz, Neergaard, Crespo Facorro and Ayesa Arriola49].

Parents in our sample were found to have higher IQs than the other participants, including the healthy controls. This finding contrasts with previous evidence showing IQ deficits among first-degree relatives of FEP patients [Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Aguilera, Campanera, Fatjó-Vilas, Guitart and Miret6,Reference Cella, Hamid, Butt and Wykes7,Reference McIntosh, Harrison, Forrester, Lawrie and Johnstone9,Reference van Os, Marsman, van Dam and Simons50,Reference de Zwarte, Brouwer, Agartz, Alda, Alonso-Lana and Bearden51]. The discrepancy in results may be related to the neuropsychological measure used in our study. We estimated crystallised intelligence, which tends to increase with age [Reference Hartshorne and Germine52] and is strongly influenced by education [Reference Rindermann, Flores-Mendoza and Mansur-Alves53]. As parents in our sample are the oldest, age may have contributed to their IQ advantage.

Relationship between PGS-SCZ and deviation from family-IQ

Our research shows that PGS-SCZ can predict deviation from family-IQ, but it does not have any direct relationship with IQ. These findings converge with some previous studies showing no connection between genetic risk of SCZ and intelligence [Reference Engen, Lyngstad, Ueland, Simonsen, Vaskinn and Smeland54,Reference van, van der, Islam, Gülöksüz, Rutten and Simons55]. However, others have reported a direct correlation between higher PGS-SCZ and low intelligence in individuals at high risk of psychosis [Reference He, Jantac Mam-Lam-Fook, Chaignaud, Danset-Alexandre, Iftimovici and Gradels Hauguel56], with SCZ [Reference Ohi, Nishizawa, Sugiyama, Takai, Kuramitsu and Hasegawa29], and in controls [Reference McIntosh, Gow, Luciano, Davies, Liewald and Harris57,Reference Shafee, Nanda, Padmanabhan, Tandon, Alliey-Rodriguez and Kalapurakkel58]. Conflicting findings in the literature may be due to differences in neuropsychological measures and sample variation. An alternative explanation is that genetic risk for SCZ may influence longitudinal intellectual trajectories rather than cross-sectional IQ scores. Although the literature on FEP is limited, some insights can be drawn from studies of the general population. Germine et al. [Reference Germine, Robinson, Smoller, Calkins, Moore and Hakonarson59] described that PGS-SCZ was associated with reduced speed of emotion identification and verbal reasoning in childhood. McIntosh et al. [Reference McIntosh, Gow, Luciano, Davies, Liewald and Harris57] found that high PGS-SCZ was associated with greater cognitive decline. Therefore, this evidence suggests that genetic liability for SCZ may be related to specific cognitive domains at key life stages. These trajectories need to be explored in the FEP population, as long-term factors such as antipsychotic medication or disease progression may influence their cognitive outcomes.

Concerning intellectual family deviation, our findings indicate that an increase of one standard deviation in PGS-SCZ may lead to roughly 0.17 standard deviations of negative deviation from family-IQ. Following Kendler et al. [Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Mezuk, Sundquist and Sundquist10], we interpret that the genetic liability for SCZ indirectly influences intelligence by disrupting neurodevelopment and preventing the achievement of cognitive potential. In this regard, it could be suggested that increased genetic susceptibility to SCZ in FEP patients may shape developmental trajectories and/or make individuals more sensitive to environmental insults [Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin60,Reference Martin, Robinson, Reutens and Mowry61], leading to the onset of psychosis. This interpretation is based on existing evidence of a common genetic susceptibility between SCZ and neurodevelopmental disorders [Reference Owen and O’Donovan62,Reference Singh, Walters, Johnstone, Curtis, Suvisaari and Torniainen63], which, when combined with environmental risk factors [Reference Guloksuz, Pries, Delespaul, Kenis, Luykx and Lin60,Reference Schmitt, Falkai and Papiol64], can increase the likelihood of impaired cognitive development from an early age.

Relationship between PGS-IQ and IQ

We confirmed a strong association between PGS-IQ and IQ. This association has been previously reported in the general population [Reference Genç, Schlüter, Fraenz, Arning, Metzen and Nguyen19,Reference Loughnan, Palmer, Thompson, Dale, Jernigan and Chieh Fan22], and our study replicates it in the FEP population [Reference Ferraro, Quattrone, La Barbera, La Cascia, Morgan and Jb25,Reference Richards, Pardiñas, Frizzati, Tansey, Lynham and Holmans65]. As expected, polymorphic genetic factors explain a small percentage of the variance in IQ, suggesting that there is a very large amount of variability associated not only with other sources of genomic variability but also with environmental factors.

As PGS-IQ showed a trend towards predicting deviation from family-IQ (p = 0.073), the evidence for this relationship remains unclear. Deviation from family cognition may not solely reflect the risk of SCZ. It is also possible that a lower genetic predisposition to intelligence contributes to this deviation. Further research on IQ in FEP, particularly investigating indirect parental genetic effects, could provide more clarity [Reference Kong, Thorleifsson, Frigge, Vilhjálmsson, Young and Thorgeirsson66,Reference Pingault, Barkhuizen, Wang, Hannigan, Eilertsen and Corfield67]. Research has shown a robust effect of genetic nurture on education, influenced by parental education and socioeconomic status [Reference Wang, Baldwin, Schoeler, Cheesman, Barkhuizen and Dudbridge68,Reference Harden, Turkheimer and Loehlin69]. This pathway could be homologous to IQ, although this needs to be verified in future studies.

Strengths and limitations

The strength of this study lies in the use of neuropsychological and genetic data from FEP patients and their unaffected first-degree relatives. However, some limitations should also be acknowledged. First, the modest sample size of the study, especially when analysing subgroups, and the incomplete families with only sibling pairs limit the study of genetic transmission. In this regard, beyond larger samples future studies would also benefit from including both first-degree relatives of controls and affected and non-affected first-degree relatives of patients. Second, IQ estimation focuses on crystallised intelligence, and the results may not generalise to other types of intelligence such as fluid intelligence. Third, the inclusion of participants of European ancestry may limit the generalisation to diverse populations. Finally, potential biases may also arise from voluntary participation and the exclusion of relatives with a history of psychiatric diagnosis, which may result in a sample with preserved cognitive function. Further studies involving two or more people with psychosis in the same family may be relevant for studying populations at high risk of SCZ.

Conclusions

Based on a family-based design in an FEP population, we confirmed that the polygenic risk for SCZ is increased in the probands, whereas the first-degree relatives score is intermediate between patients and controls. This validates the polygenic background as a discernible marker of genetic risk variation within families. Additionally, our results indicated that the genetic load for SCZ significantly predicts the deviation from the family-IQ, explaining that FEP patients underperformed in the IQ test compared to their relatives. The genetic risk for SCZ may modulate cognition by shaping developmental trajectories and making individuals more sensitive to environmental insults, therefore, preventing individuals from reaching the familial cognitive potential. Further research is needed to determine the potential contribution of genetic liability for intelligence to the unrealised cognitive potential of FEP patients.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2024.24.

Acknowledgements

Thanks to all participants of PAFIP-FAMILIES. The authors thank the Biobank Valdecilla (PT20/00067), integrated into the Spanish Biobank Network, for its collaboration.

Competing of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Funding support

The PAFIP-FAMILIES project was funded by the ISCIII (FIS PI17/00221). Genotyping was performed at the Human Genotyping laboratory (CeGen), supported by grant PT17/0019, of the PE I+D+i 2013-2016, funded by ISCIII and FEDER. Rosa Ayesa-Arriola and Mar Fatjó-Vilas were funded by Miguel Servet contracts from ISCIII (CP18/00003 and CP20/00072). Nancy Murillo-Garcia was funded by a predoctoral contract from IDIVAL (PREVAL20/05) and a scientific exchange grant from EMBO (SEG9790). Additional sources of funding include ISCIII (PI20/00066, PI14/00639, PI14/00918, MS18-Ayuda) and IDIVAL (INNVAL20/02, INNVAL23/21).

References

Wechsler, D. The Wechsler adults intelligence scale. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp. Harcourt; 1997.Google Scholar
Kendler, KS, Turkheimer, E, Ohlsson, H, Sundquist, J, Sundquist, K. Family environment and the malleability of cognitive ability: A Swedish national home-reared and adopted-away cosibling control study. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112(15):4612–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, GM, Barnett, JH, White, IR, Jones, PB. A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophr Res noviembre de. 2011;132(2-3):220–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosway, R, Byrne, M, Clafferty, R, Hodges, A, Grant, E, Abukmeil, SS, et al. Neuropsychological change in young people at high risk for schizophrenia: Results from the first two neuropsychological assessments of the Edinburgh high risk study. Psychol Med septiembre de. 2000;30(5):1111–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, P, Rodgers, B, Murray, R, Marmot, M. Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet Lond Engl. 1994;344(8934):1398–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrantes-Vidal, N, Aguilera, M, Campanera, S, Fatjó-Vilas, M, Guitart, M, Miret, S, et al. Working memory in siblings of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res. 2007;95(1-3):70–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cella, M, Hamid, S, Butt, K, Wykes, T. Cognition and social cognition in non-psychotic siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2015;20(3):232–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Zwarte, SMC, Brouwer, RM, Tsouli, A, Cahn, W, Hillegers, MHJ, Hulshoff Pol, HE, et al. Running in the family? Structural brain abnormalities and IQ in offspring, siblings, parents, and co-twins of patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2019;45(6):1209–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, AM, Harrison, LK, Forrester, K, Lawrie, SM, Johnstone, EC. Neuropsychological impairments in people with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder and their unaffected relatives. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci. 2005;186:378–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, KS, Ohlsson, H, Mezuk, B, Sundquist, JO, Sundquist, K. Observed cognitive performance and deviation from familial cognitive aptitude at age 16 years and ages 18 to 20 years and risk for schizophrenia and bipolar illness in a Swedish National Sample. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(5):465–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertisch, H, Mesen-Fainardi, A, Martin, MV, Pérez-Vargas, V, Vargas-Rodríguez, T, Delgado, G, et al. Neuropsychological performance as endophenotypes in extended schizophrenia families from the Central Valley of Costa Rica. Psychiatr Genet. 2009;19(1):4552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, RC, Braff, DL, Calkins, ME, Dobie, DJ, Freedman, R, Green, MF, et al. Neurocognitive performance in family-based and case-control studies of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res abril de. 2015;163(1-3):1723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, NS, Badcock, JC, Clark, ML, Knowles, EEM, Cadby, G, Melton, PE, et al. Assessment of cognition and personality as potential endophenotypes in the Western Australian family study of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(4):908–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Z, Zhang, R, Qin, P, Tan, L. Cognitive dysfunction and negative symptoms in patients with schizophrenia and their first-degree relatives from simplex and multiplex families. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2018;14:3339–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haworth, CMA, Wright, MJ, Luciano, M, Martin, NG, de Geus, EJC, van Beijsterveldt, CEM, et al. The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15(11):1112–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, EA, McGue, M, Iacono, WG, Lee, JJ. Genetic and environmental contributions to IQ in adoptive and biological families with 30-year-old offspring. Intelligence. 2021;88:101579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, MJ, Sawa, A, Mortensen, PB. Schizophrenia. Lancet. 2016;388(10039):8697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilker, R, Helenius, D, Fagerlund, B, Skytthe, A, Christensen, K, Werge, TM, et al. Heritability of schizophrenia and schizophrenia spectrum based on the nationwide Danish twin register. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83(6):492–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genç, E, Schlüter, C, Fraenz, C, Arning, L, Metzen, D, Nguyen, HP, et al. Polygenic scores for cognitive abilities and their association with different aspects of general intelligence—A deep phenotyping approach. Mol Neurobiol. 2021;58(8):4145–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, AR, Daly, MJ, Robinson, EB, Hyman, SE, Neale, BM. Predicting polygenic risk of psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86(2):97109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, JE, Jansen, PR, Stringer, S, Watanabe, K, Bryois, J, de Leeuw, CA, et al. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nat Genet. 2018;50(7):912–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loughnan, RJ, Palmer, CE, Thompson, WK, Dale, AM, Jernigan, TL, Chieh Fan, C. Intelligence polygenic score is more predictive of crystallized measures: Evidence from the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study. Psychol Sci. 2023;09567976231160702.Google ScholarPubMed
Trubetskoy, V, Pardiñas, AF, Qi, T, Panagiotaropoulou, G, Awasthi, S, Bigdeli, TB, et al. Mapping genomic loci implicates genes and synaptic biology in schizophrenia. Nature. 2022;604(7906):502–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium . Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511(7510):421–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferraro, L, Quattrone, D, La Barbera, D, La Cascia, C, Morgan, C, Jb, K, et al. First-episode psychosis patients who deteriorated in the premorbid period do not have higher polygenic risk scores than others: A cluster analysis of EU-GEI data. Schizophr Bull. 2023;49(1):218–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrisberger, F, Smieskova, R, Vogler, C, Egli, T, Schmidt, A, Lenz, C, et al. Impact of polygenic schizophrenia-related risk and hippocampal volumes on the onset of psychosis. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6(8):e868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, WD, Davies, G, CHARGE Cognitive Working Group, Liewald, DC, McIntosh, AM, Deary, IJ. Age-dependent pleiotropy between general cognitive function and major psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;80(4):266–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murillo-García, N, Barrio-Martínez, S, Setién-Suero, E, Soler, J, Papiol, S, Fatjó-Vilas, M, et al. Overlap between genetic variants associated with schizophrenia spectrum disorders and intelligence quotient: A systematic review. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2022;47(6):E393–408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohi, K, Nishizawa, D, Sugiyama, S, Takai, K, Kuramitsu, A, Hasegawa, J, et al. Polygenic risk scores differentiating schizophrenia from bipolar disorder are associated with premorbid intelligence in schizophrenia patients and healthy subjects. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2021;24(7):562–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayesa-Arriola, R, Ortíz-García de la Foz, V, Martínez-García, O, Setién-Suero, E, Ramírez, ML, Suárez-Pinilla, P, et al. Dissecting the functional outcomes of first episode schizophrenia spectrum disorders: A 10-year follow-up study in the PAFIP cohort. Psychol Med. 2021;51(2):264–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez-Sánchez, JM, Setién-Suero, E, Suárez-Pinilla, P, Mayoral Van Son, J, Vázquez-Bourgon, J, Gil López, P, et al. Ten-year course of cognition in first-episode non-affective psychosis patients: PAFIP cohort. Psychol Med. 2022;51(4):770–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murillo-García, N, Díaz-Pons, A, Fernández-Cacho, LM, Miguel-Corredera, M, Martínez-Barrio, S, Ortiz-García de la Foz, V, et al. A family study on first episode of psychosis patients: Exploring neuropsychological performance as an endophenotype. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2022;145(4):384–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, NC. Comprehensive assessment of symptoms and history (CASH). Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa College of Medicine; 1987.Google Scholar
Gómez-Revuelta, M, Pelayo-Terán, JM, Juncal-Ruiz, M, Vázquez-Bourgon, J, Suárez-Pinilla, P, Romero-Jiménez, R, et al. Antipsychotic treatment effectiveness in first episode of psychosis: PAFIP 3-year follow-up randomized clinical trials comparing haloperidol, olanzapine, risperidone, aripiprazole, quetiapine, and ziprasidone. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020;23(4):217–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, NC. The Scale for the assessment of positive symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City: University of Iowa; 1984.Google Scholar
Andreasen, NC. The scale for the assessment of negative symptoms (SANS). Iowa City: University of Iowa; 1983.Google Scholar
Hall, RCW. Global assessment of functioning: A modified scale. Psychosomatics. 1995;36(3):267–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, MD, Howieson, DB, Loring, DW, Fischer, JS. Neuropsychological assessment. USA: Oxford University Press; 2004.Google Scholar
Cattell, RB. Abilities: their structure, growth, and action. Oxford, England: Houghton Mifflin; 1971. xxii, 583pp.Google Scholar
Ellingsen, VJ, Ackerman, PL. Fluid–crystallized theory of intelligence. In: The encyclopedia of adulthood and aging [Internet]. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2015 [cited 21 Dec. 2023]. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002/9781118521373.wbeaa022Google Scholar
de Oliveira, MO, Nitrini, R, Yassuda, MS, Brucki, SMD. Vocabulary is an appropriate measure of premorbid intelligence in a sample with heterogeneous educational level in Brazil. Behav Neurol. 2014;2014:875960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayesa-Arriola, R, Setién-Suero, E, Neergaard, KD, Belzunces, ÀA, Contreras, F, van Haren, NEM, et al. Premorbid IQ subgroups in first episode non affective psychosis patients: Long-term sex differences in function and neurocognition. Schizophr Res. 2018;197:370–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, T, Chen, CY, Ni, Y, Feng, YC, Smoller, JW. Polygenic prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):1776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. [Internet]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2021. Available from: https://www.R-project.org (accessed December 10, 2022).Google Scholar
Kuha, A, Tuulio-Henriksson, A, Eerola, M, Perälä, J, Suvisaari, J, Partonen, T, et al. Impaired executive performance in healthy siblings of schizophrenia patients in a population-based study. Schizophr Res. 2007;92(1-3):142–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chu, AOK, Chang, WC, Chan, SKW, Lee, EHM, Hui, CLM, Chen, EYH. Comparison of cognitive functions between first-episode schizophrenia patients, their unaffected siblings and individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis. Psychol Med. 2019;49(11):1929–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scala, S, Lasalvia, A, Seidman, LJ, Cristofalo, D, Bonetto, C, Ruggeri, M. Executive functioning and psychopathological profile in relatives of individuals with deficit v. non-deficit schizophrenia: A pilot study. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2014;23(1):8597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayesa-Arriola, R, de la, Foz VOG, Murillo-García, N, Vázquez-Bourgon, J, Juncal-Ruiz, M, Gómez-Revuelta, M, et al. Cognitive reserve as a moderator of outcomes in five clusters of first episode psychosis patients: A 10-year follow-up study of the PAFIP cohort. Psychol Med. 2023;53(5):18911905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magdaleno Herrero, R, Murillo García, N, Yorca Ruiz, Á, Neergaard, K, Crespo Facorro, B, Ayesa Arriola, R. Biomarkers as Proxies for Cognitive Reserve: the role of high density lipoprotein cholesterol in first episode of psychosis. Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment. 2023. doi:10.1016/j.rpsm.2023.03.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Os, J, Marsman, A, van Dam, D, Simons, CJP, GROUP Investigators. Evidence that the impact of childhood trauma on IQ is substantial in controls, moderate in siblings, and absent in patients with psychotic disorder. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(2):316–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Zwarte, SMC, Brouwer, RM, Agartz, I, Alda, M, Alonso-Lana, S, Bearden, CE, et al. Intelligence, educational attainment, and brain structure in those at familial high-risk for schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2020;43(1):414–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartshorne, JK, Germine, LT. When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(4):433–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rindermann, H, Flores-Mendoza, C, Mansur-Alves, M. Reciprocal effects between fluid and crystallized intelligence and their dependence on parents’ socioeconomic status and education. Learn Individ Differ. 2010;20:544–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engen, MJ, Lyngstad, SH, Ueland, T, Simonsen, CE, Vaskinn, A, Smeland, O, et al. Polygenic scores for schizophrenia and general cognitive ability: Associations with six cognitive domains, premorbid intelligence, and cognitive composite score in individuals with a psychotic disorder and in healthy controls. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10(1):416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van, Os J, van der, Steen Y, Islam, MA, Gülöksüz, S, Rutten, BP, Simons, CJ, et al. Evidence that polygenic risk for psychotic disorder is expressed in the domain of neurodevelopment, emotion regulation and attribution of salience. Psychol Med. 2017;47(14):2421–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, Q, Jantac Mam-Lam-Fook, C, Chaignaud, J, Danset-Alexandre, C, Iftimovici, A, Gradels Hauguel, J, et al. Influence of polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia and resilience on the cognition of individuals at-risk for psychosis. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11(1):518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, AM, Gow, A, Luciano, M, Davies, G, Liewald, DC, Harris, SE, et al. Polygenic risk for schizophrenia is associated with cognitive change between childhood and old age. Biol Psychiatry 2013;73(10):938–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shafee, R, Nanda, P, Padmanabhan, JL, Tandon, N, Alliey-Rodriguez, N, Kalapurakkel, S, et al. Polygenic risk for schizophrenia and measured domains of cognition in individuals with psychosis and controls. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;8(1):78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Germine, L, Robinson, EB, Smoller, JW, Calkins, ME, Moore, TM, Hakonarson, H, et al. Association between polygenic risk for schizophrenia, neurocognition and social cognition across development. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6(10):e924–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guloksuz, S, Pries, L, Delespaul, P, Kenis, G, Luykx, J, Lin, BD, et al. Examining the independent and joint effects of molecular genetic liability and environmental exposures in schizophrenia: results from the EUGEI study. World Psychiatry [Internet]. 2019 [cited 20 Sept 2023]. Available from: https://consensus.app/details/results-suggest-etiopathogenesis-schizophrenia-guloksuz/ff10d9c2ec33573f9c6bb9d5a43f1eb9/CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, AK, Robinson, G, Reutens, D, Mowry, B. Cannabis abuse and age at onset in schizophrenia patients with large, rare copy number variants. Schizophr Res. 2014;155(1-3):21–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, MJ, O’Donovan, MC. Schizophrenia and the neurodevelopmental continuum: Evidence from genomics. World Psychiatry 2017;16(3):227–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, T, Walters, J, Johnstone, M, Curtis, D, Suvisaari, J, Torniainen, M, et al. The contribution of rare variants to risk of schizophrenia in individuals with and without intellectual disability. Nat Genet [Internet]. 2017 [cited 20 Sept 2023]. Availabe from: https://consensus.app/details/together-results-show-variants-contribute-risk-singh/ab8224d4f7c15eeaa18716853dfdf36f/CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, A, Falkai, P, Papiol, S. Neurodevelopmental disturbances in schizophrenia: Evidence from genetic and environmental factors. J Neural Transm Vienna Austria. 2023;130(3):195205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, AL, Pardiñas, AF, Frizzati, A, Tansey, KE, Lynham, AJ, Holmans, P, et al. The relationship between polygenic risk scores and cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2020;46(2):336–44.Google ScholarPubMed
Kong, A, Thorleifsson, G, Frigge, M, Vilhjálmsson, B, Young, AI, Thorgeirsson, T, et al. The nature of nurture: Effects of parental genotypes. Science. 2017;359:424–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pingault, JB, Barkhuizen, W, Wang, B, Hannigan, LJ, Eilertsen, EM, Corfield, E, et al. Genetic nurture versus genetic transmission of risk for ADHD traits in the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;28:1731–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, B, Baldwin, JR, Schoeler, T, Cheesman, R, Barkhuizen, W, Dudbridge, F, et al. Robust genetic nurture effects on education: A systematic review and meta-analysis based on 38,654 families across 8 cohorts. Am J Hum Genet. 2021;108(9):1780–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harden, K, Turkheimer, E, Loehlin, J. Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents’ cognitive aptitude. Behav Genet. 2007;37:273–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Conformation of the families participating in this study.Note: Each family was formed by a FEP patient and at least one first-degree relative, either a parent or sibling. All participants completed the same neuropsychological battery and provided a DNA sample that allowed the calculation of polygenic scores. *There was one family with nine members, one with six members, and five with five members.

Figure 1

Table 1. Between-group comparisons using linear mixed model analysis

Figure 2

Figure 2. Violin plots of IQ, PGS-SCZ, and PGS-IQ according to the group of participants.Note: The IQs shown in the first plot are without corrections for age and years of education. After introducing the former covariates, parents had higher IQs than FEP patients (p = 0.024) and controls (p = 0.018). Regarding PGS-SCZ, FEP patients had higher scores than all other groups (p < 0.001). No significant differences were found for PGS-IQ.

Figure 3

Table 2. The predictive effect of PGS-SCZ on IQ and deviation from family-IQ using linear mixed models

Figure 4

Table 3. The predictive effect of PGS-IQ on IQ and deviation from family-IQ using linear mixed models

Supplementary material: File

Murillo-García et al. supplementary material

Murillo-García et al. supplementary material
Download Murillo-García et al. supplementary material(File)
File 214.7 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.