Neurocognitive performance in family-based and case-control studies of schizophrenia

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.049Get rights and content

Abstract

Background

Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia (SZ) are established and the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) investigated such measures as endophenotypes in family-based (COGS-1) and case-control (COGS-2) studies. By requiring family participation, family-based sampling may result in samples that vary demographically and perform better on neurocognitive measures.

Methods

The Penn computerized neurocognitive battery (CNB) evaluates accuracy and speed of performance for several domains and was administered across sites in COGS-1 and COGS-2. Most tests were included in both studies. COGS-1 included 328 patients with SZ and 497 healthy comparison subjects (HCS) and COGS-2 included 1195 patients and 1009 HCS.

Results

Demographically, COGS-1 participants were younger, more educated, with more educated parents and higher estimated IQ compared to COGS-2 participants. After controlling for demographics, the two samples produced very similar performance profiles compared to their respective controls. As expected, performance was better and with smaller effect sizes compared to controls in COGS-1 relative to COGS-2. Better performance was most pronounced for spatial processing while emotion identification had large effect sizes for both accuracy and speed in both samples. Performance was positively correlated with functioning and negatively with negative and positive symptoms in both samples, but correlations were attenuated in COGS-2, especially with positive symptoms.

Conclusions

Patients ascertained through family-based design have more favorable demographics and better performance on some neurocognitive domains. Thus, studies that use case-control ascertainment may tap into populations with more severe forms of illness that are exposed to less favorable factors compared to those ascertained with family-based designs.

Introduction

Methods of ascertainment are pivotal across biomedical research and are an important consideration in the research design. In genetic studies, the utility and statistical approach of family-based and unrelated case-controls studies has been discussed (e. g. Hiekkalinna et al., 2012). The incorporation of endophenotypes to genetic investigations of schizophrenia (SZ) has grown significantly with neurocognitive measures (Gur et al., 2007a, Gur et al., 2007b, Lee et al., 2015--in this issue, Nuechterlein et al., 2015--in this issue, Stone et al., 2015--in this issue) and neurophysiological measures (Swerdlow et al., 2014, Light et al., 2015--in this issue, Turetsky et al., 2015--in this issue) playing key roles. Family-based designs enable testing the endophenotype criteria (Braff et al., 2007, Braff, 2015--in this issue, Gottesman and Gould, 2003) and, when sufficiently powered, allow for the examination of heritability, association with the disease phenotype and co-segregation within families (Glahn et al., 2014, Greenwood et al., 2007, Greenwood et al., 2011, Greenwood et al., 2013).

Several meta-analyses have reported that adult relatives of probands with SZ show intermediate deficits in neurocognitive measures including executive functions, such as working memory and attention, verbal fluency and sensori-motor speed (Faraone et al., 2001, Kremen and Hoff, 2004, Sitskoorn et al., 2004, Snitz et al., 2006). Similar deficits have also been observed in younger relatives (Niemi et al., 2003, Seidman et al., 2006, Keshavan et al., 2010, Agnew-Blais and Siedman, 2013). The neurocognitive domains implicated in family-based studies are similar to deficits observed in case-control studies (Gur et al., 2001b). Yet, direct evaluation of these complementary ascertainment strategies applying the same measures has not been conducted. The Penn computerized neurocognitive battery (CNB) used in the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) provides a unique opportunity to evaluate effects of ascertainment methods—family-based (COGS-1) vs. case control (COGS-2)—with the same neurocognitive battery across the participating sites.

The CNB, developed in concert with functional neuroimaging studies (Gur et al., 2010), has been validated in healthy participants and people with SZ (Gur et al., 2001a, Gur et al., 2001b) and is sensitive to the effects of age and sex (Gur et al., 2012, Irani et al., 2012). The battery, which provides measures of performance accuracy and response time, was applied in three independent large-scale family-based genetic studies. The Multiplex Multigenerational Investigation of Schizophrenia (MGI; Gur et al., 2007a) reported that probands demonstrated greatest impairment relative to healthy controls, with intermediate performance of family members. Liability for SZ affected the speed–accuracy tradeoff differently for specific neurocognitive domains. Significant heritability estimates were obtained for accuracy of verbal, facial, and spatial memory and spatial and emotion processing. For speed, estimates of heritability were significant for abstraction and mental flexibility, attention, face memory, and spatial and sensorimotor processing. The results of the Project among African-Americans to Explore Risks for Schizophrenia (PAARTNERS) revealed that patients with SZ exhibited less accuracy and speed in most neurocognitive domains than their relatives, who were impaired relative to HCS in most domains. Significant heritabilities were observed for most neurocognitive domains, with the highest for accuracy of abstraction and mental flexibility, verbal memory, face memory, spatial processing, and emotion processing and for speed of attention (Calkins et al., 2010).

In COGS-1 all of the measures applied from the Penn CNB (Abstraction and Mental Flexibility, Face Memory, Spatial Memory, Spatial Processing, Sensorimotor Dexterity, and Emotion Recognition) were significantly heritable with heritability estimates ranging from 24% for Spatial Memory to 55% for Spatial Processing (Greenwood et al., 2007). These heritabilities are in the same range as the heritability of SZ itself in the COGS-1 families (Light et al., in press). Furthermore, we noted sex differences in familiality effects with male probands' performance predictive of performance of their unaffected relatives (Calkins et al., 2013). The subsequent application of the CNB in the case-control design of COGS-2 enabled evaluation of the pattern of performance of individuals with SZ, compared to HCS, ascertained in family-based and case-control designs. We noted that in some endophenotypic measures in COGS-1 probands were less impaired than observed in other samples of patients with SZ (Greenwood et al., 2007). The major ascertainment difference between the samples is that patients recruited for COGS-1 required the availability of parents and siblings while COGS-2 permitted participation of patients regardless of family availability (Swerdlow et al., 2015--in this issue). This difference likely affects multiple demographic characteristics related to age, education, socioeconomic status as well as severity of illness, favoring COGS-1. We hypothesized that while the profile of impairment would be similar, probands in the COGS-1 family-based ascertainment would perform better than those ascertained as cases in COGS-2.

Section snippets

Participants

Details on the COGS-1 and COGS-2 samples' ascertainment, inclusion and exclusion criteria and clinical assessment are provided elsewhere in this issue (Braff et al.; Swerdlow et al.). Briefly, COGS-1, a family-based design, and COG-2, a case-control design, included probands 18–65 years old who met DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia based on established diagnostic procedures. COGS-1 required that both biological parents were available for genotyping, and that at least one full sibling,

Performance comparisons on the computerized neurocognitive battery

The means and standard deviations, as well as p values for Student's t-tests and effect sizes comparing patients and controls within each sample and between samples are presented in the bottom portion of Table 1. The z-scores of patients compared to their respective control groups are illustrated in Fig. 1.

As can be seen in Table 1, both COGS-1 patients and COGS-2 patients are impaired relative to their respective controls, although the effect sizes range from small to moderate in COGS-1, with

Discussion

The Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) allowed for the comparison of neurocognitive performance deficits between individuals with schizophrenia ascertained through a family-based sampling (COGS-1) and those ascertained through case-control sampling (COGS-2) with their differing ascertainment strategies as discussed above and in this issue (cf. Swerdlow et al., 2015--in this issue). The results indicated very similar neurocognitive deficit profiles, for COGS-1 and COGS-2

Role of funding source

This work was supported by collaborative R01 grants from the National Institute of Mental Health. COGS-1 and COGS-2: MH065571 UCSD, MH065707 UCLA, MH065554 MSSM, MH065578 Penn, MH065558 Washington; COGS-1 Only: MH065588 Colorado, MH065562 Harvard; Seidman and Stone are on a subcontract for COGS-2; COGS-2 Only: MH86135 Stanford.

Contributors

All authors significantly contributed to study design, data collection and reviewed and approved this manuscript.

Conflict of interest

Dr. Green has been a consultant to AbbVie, Biogen, DSP, EnVivo/Forum and Roche, and he is on the scientific advisory board of Mnemosyne. He has received research funds from Amgen. Dr. Lazzeroni is an inventor on a patent application filed by Stanford University on genetic polymorphisms associated with depression. Dr. Light has served as a consultant for Astellas, Forum, and Neuroverse. Dr. Nuechterlein has received unrelated research support from Janssen Scientific Affairs, Genentech, and Brain

Acknowledgments

We thank the research staff who administered the CNB, Allison Mott CNB Implementation Manager, University of Pennsylvania and the patients, families and healthy volunteers who participated in the study.

References (43)

  • L. Niemi et al.

    Childhood developmental abnormalities in schizophrenia: evidence from high-risk studies

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2003)
  • K.H. Nuechterlein et al.

    Attention/vigilance in schizophrenia: performance results from a large multi-site study of the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS)

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2015)
  • M.M. Sitskoorn et al.

    Cognitive deficits in relatives of patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2004)
  • W.S. Stone et al.

    California Verbal Learning Test-II performance in schizophrenia as a function of ascertainment strategy: Comparing the first and second phases of the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS)

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2015)
  • N.R. Swerdlow et al.

    Deficient prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia detected by the multi-site Consortium on the Genetics in Schizophrenia

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2014)
  • N.R. Swerdlow et al.

    Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) assessment of endophenotypes for schizophrenia: an introduction to this special issue of Schizophrenia Research

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2015)
  • B.I. Turetsky et al.

    The utility of P300 as a schizophrenia endophenotype and predictive biomarker: Clinical and socio-demographic modulators in COGS-2

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2015)
  • J. Agnew-Blais et al.

    Neurocognition in youth adults under age 30 at familial risk for schizophrenia: a quantitative and qualitative review

    Cogn. Neuropsychiatry

    (2013)
  • N.C. Andreasen

    Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS)

    (1984)
  • N.C. Andreasen

    Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS)

    (1984)
  • D.L. Braff et al.

    Deconstructing schizophrenia: an overview of the use of endophenotypes in order to understand a complex disorder

    Schizophr. Bull.

    (2007)
  • Cited by (31)

    • Familial risk of psychosis in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Impact on clinical characteristics, comorbidity and treatment response

      2022, Journal of Psychiatric Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      As dysfunction in the cortico-striato-thalamic circuitry is postulated to be a key mechanism in both disorders, there is also substantial overlap in the endophenotypes identified between them. For example, many studies in OCD have reported working memory deficits, which is considered a key endophenotype in schizophrenia (Abramovitch et al., 2015; Gur et al., 2015). Aberrations in certain neurophysiological measures such as evoked potentials (P300, mismatch negativity, etc.) are common to both disorders.

    • Comparison of two cognitive screening measures in a longitudinal sample of youth at-risk for psychosis

      2022, Schizophrenia Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Cognitive impairment is most evident in patients with chronic symptoms of psychosis, however, multiple studies report cognitive deficits in first-episode psychosis patients (FEP) (Sawada et al., 2017; Saykin, 1994; Simon et al., 2007), in those at clinical high-risk (CHR) for developing psychosis (Bolt et al., 2019; De Herdt et al., 2013; Gur et al., 2015a; Lencz et al., 2006; Reichenberg, 2005; Zhang et al., 2021a), and in those with sub-threshold psychosis spectrum (PS) symptoms (Gur et al., 2014; Gur et al., 2015b).

    • Deviation from expected cognitive ability is a core cognitive feature of schizophrenia related to neurophysiologic, clinical and psychosocial functioning

      2020, Schizophrenia Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Further, deviation scores accounted for the largest shared variance when predicting illness duration (R2 = 0.012 vs. 0.003) (see Fig. 3). Cognitive impairment is a key characteristic of schizophrenia due to its prevalence, severity, and relationship to major domains of neurophysiologic, clinical, and psychosocial function (Green et al., 2004; Gur et al., 2015; Hill et al., 2013; Hochberger et al., 2016; Light et al., 2012; Perry et al., 2000; Rissling et al., 2014; Swerdlow et al., 2015; Thomas et al., 2017). Neuropsychological assessment based on population-based norms remains the gold standard for evaluating current cognitive functioning in schizophrenia and other disorders (Crawford, 2005; Kessels and Hendriks, 2015; Lezak, 1995).

    • Performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: A comparison between US and Israeli cohorts

      2016, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      This is also true in the 22q11DS field and there is an on-going effort to harmonize assessments across multiple sites through the International 22q11.2 Brain Behavior Consortium. Notably, because PCNB has been used in other relevant populations including clinical high risk for psychosis and family members of schizophrenia (R.C. Gur et al., 2014, 2015), use of PCNB in 22q11DS should allow a comparison across multiple at-risk populations. In the present study, we have administered a Hebrew translation of the PCNB to a 22q11DS cohort in Tel Aviv (TLV), Israel and compared their neurocognitive profile to one of the largest 22q11DS cohorts based in Philadelphia (PHL), United States.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text