SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.147 issue2Management of hip fractures in the elderly. A national survey among Chilean orthopaedic surgeonsEnvironmental risk factors in the development and evolution of inflammatory bowel disease author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Journal

Article

Indicators

Related links

  • On index processCited by Google
  • Have no similar articlesSimilars in SciELO
  • On index processSimilars in Google

Share


Revista médica de Chile

Print version ISSN 0034-9887

Rev. méd. Chile vol.147 no.2 Santiago Feb. 2019

http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/s0034-98872019000200206 

Comunicación Breve

Marijuana use associated with worse verbal learning and delayed recall in a sample of young adults

El consumo de marihuana se asocia a una menor capacidad de aprendizaje verbal y memoria tardía en adultos jóvenes

Natalia Laspada1  a

Erin Delker1  be

Estela Blanco1  cef

Pamela Encina2  bg

Gabriela Caballero2  bg

Jorge Delva3  dh

Raquel Burrows2  j

Betsy Lozoff4  ij

Sheila Gahagan1  ej

1Department of Pediatrics, Division of Child Development and Community Health, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA, USA

2Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology, Unversidad de Chile, Av. El Líbano 5524, Santiago, Chile

3Dean School of Social Work, Director and Paul Farmer Professor for Innovation in Social Work and Health, Boston University, Boston, MA, 02215, USA

4Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, 300 N. Ingalls Bldg. 1063NE, Ann Arbor, MI, USA

ABSTRACT

Background:

There is concern about the cognitive consequences of marijuana consumption.

Aim:

To assess the influence of current and past marijuana use and frequency on verbal learning and memory in a sample of adults aged 21 years old.

Material and Methods:

Marijuana use was assessed using a clinician administered interview in 654 participants (56% females), who reported frequency of use, age of first use and whether its use led to problems in their lives. The CogState International Shopping List was administered to assess learning and memory.

Results:

Seventy percent reported ever using marijuana, 46% consuming during the past year and 27% during the past 30 days. The latter scored significantly lower on delayed recall. Current and frequent use were significantly associated with lower accuracy in verbal learning and memory.

Conclusions:

In this cohort of adults aged 21 years old, marijuana use was prevalent and related to worse verbal memory.

Key words: Cannabis; Memory; Verbal Learning; Young Adult; Marijuana Use

RESUMO

Antecedentes:

Existe preocupación acerca de los efectos cognitivos del consumo de marihuana.

Objetivo:

Estudiar el efecto de consumo de marihuana presente o pasado en la capacidad de aprendizaje verbal y memoria en una muestra de adultos de 21 años.

Material y Métodos:

El consumo de marihuana fue evaluado mediante una entrevista médica en 654 adultos de 21 años (56% mujeres), quienes informaron acerca de la frecuencia de consumo, edad de comienzo y si el consumo les ha causado problemas en sus vidas. Se les administró el Cogstate International Shopping List para evaluar aprendizaje y memoria.

Resultados:

El 70% informó haber consumido marihuana alguna vez, 46% la usó durante el último año y el 27% en los últimos 30 días. Estos últimos tuvieron un menor puntaje en memoria tardía. El consumo actual y frecuente se asoció a una menor precisión en la capacidad de aprendizaje verbal y memoria.

Conclusiones:

En esta cohorte de adultos de 21 años, el consumo de marihuana fue prevalente y relacionado a una menor memoria verbal.

Palabras clave: Cannabis; Memoria; Aprendizaje Verbal; Adulto Joven; Uso de la marihuana

Marijuana use is increasing in Chile and worldwide, and several countries support decriminalization of the substance. In 2015, Chilean Law 20,000 allowed, for the first time, a small amount of marijuana cultivation, provided proper authorization, for personal use1. Among South American countries, Chile reported the highest lifetime marijuana use (28%), with adolescent use doubling in the last decade from 14.8% (2001) to 30.6% (2013)2. Furthermore, comprehensive national surveys conducted by SENDA (Servicio Nacional para la Prevención y Rehabilitación del consumo de Dorogas y Alcohol) (2016) reflect systematic increase in the prevalence of marijuana use from 2010 to 2016. In particular, young adults aged 19-25 experienced a statistically significant increase in reported lifetime marijuana use, rising from 24.0% in 2014 to 33.8% in 20163. Given the relative commonality of marijuana use among the young adult population, it is important to understand possible consequences and associations of marijuana use. In particular, our intention was to test for neurocognitive associations of marijuana use within a Chilean young adult sample.

Prior studies have demonstrated negative cognitive outcomes for marijuana users410, including early onset users9, with effects persisting even after periods of forced abstinence6. In a Chilean sample, marijuana use has been related to deficits in fluid intelligence10. Cognitive functions affected include task accuracy, psychomotor speed, sequencing ability, sustained attention, and verbal memory47. In contrast, a recent meta-analysis found that the supposed negative cognitive outcomes of marijuana use may be overstated or due to acute exposure11. We assessed cognitive outcomes related to any, frequent, early onset, and problematic marijuana use in a sample of Chilean young adults.

Materials and Methods

Participants were part of a Chilean cohort studied since infancy in either a preventative trial of iron-deficiency anemia or a neuromaturation study12. Data were collected from healthy 21-year-olds originally recruited in infancy from four low-to middle-income communities in Santiago, Chile12,13. Of 1,790 infants from the original studies, 654 participated in an evaluation of substance use and neurocognitive testing at 21 years. The study was approved by Institutional Review Boards at the Institute for Nutrition and Food Technology, University of Chile, University of Michigan, and University of California San Diego. All participants gave informed consent.

Study variables

Marijuana use was assessed via clinician-administered survey. Participants indicating any marijuana use received questions regarding frequency in the past 30 days and past year. Three or more marijuana uses in the past 30 days was considered frequent use. Problematic marijuana use was ≥1 problems reported in relationships, school, or health measured on a 14-item questionnaire(14; see appendix A).

Verbal learning (International Shopping List [ISL]) and memory (ISL+ Recall [ISLR]) were assessed with the CogState-International Shopping List, which has reliability across translations, as suggested by Lim et al.15. The CogState neurocognitive battery has determined validity for measuring cognitive functioning16. The ISL assesses immediate and delayed recall of a 12-item list. Immediate recall is measured as the sum of 3 trials. Delayed recall is measured in one trial. Higher scores indicate better verbal learning and memory.

Covariates from infancy (socioeconomic status [SES], Mother's IQ, sex, randomization group) and young adulthood (age, education [high school graduation yes/no], “binge” drinking, lifetime cigarette use) were considered as potential confounding variables. SES was assessed using the Graffar index17; higher scores indicate lower SES. Binge drinking was defined as consuming ≥ 4 or 5 drinks on one occasion for females and males, respectively.

Statistical analyses

Univariate and bivariate analyses were used for descriptive statistics. Multivariable generalized linear models tested group differences in learning/memory scores. We compared mean cognitive scores of (1) participants reporting marijuana use over the past year or 30 days to never users; (2) frequent marijuana users to non-frequent or never-users; and (3) problem marijuana users to non-problem or never-users. Analyses adjusted for sex, age at substance use assessment, education, binge drinking, and cigarette use18. We utilized SAS version 9.4 and SPSS version 23 software.

Results

Participants, as described in Table 1, were 21 (SD = 0.2) years old on average, 85.5% high school graduates, and 55.6% female. Marijuana use was endorsed by 69.5% (lifetime), 45.8% (past year), and 27.0% (past 30 days).

Table 1 Participant characteristics according to lifetime marijuana use 

Yes (n, %) or Mean (SD) No (n, %) or Mean (SD) p-value
Infancy
Socioeconomic status, (range 9-43)b (10) 27.3 (SD = 6.5) 27.8 (SD = 6.0) 0.404
Mother's IQ, (range 51-110) 83.9 (SD = 9.7) 84.5 (SD = 9.2) 0.487
Sex
Male (222, 76.3) (69, 23.7) 0.001
Female (232, 63.9) (131, 36.1)
Iron supplementation
Yes (138, 30.0) (322, 70.0) 0.927
No (51, 30.9) (114, 69.1)
Iron deficiency anemia
Yes (19, 23.5) (62, 76.5) 0.154
No (170, 31.3) (374, 68.8)
Young Adult
Age of cognitive assessment, yr 21.0 (SD = 0.3) 21.0 (SD = 0.3) 0.830
Age of drug assessment, yr 20.9 (SD = 0.2) 21.0 (SD = 0.2) 0.076
Graduate secondary school
Yes (379, 67.8) (180, 32.2) 0.029
No (75, 79.0) (20, 21.1)
Lifetime binge drinking
Yes (138, 95.8) (6, 4.2) 0.000
No (316, 62.0) (194, 38.0)
Lifetime cigarette use
Yes (390, 85.0) (69, 15.0) 0.000
No (64, 32.8) (131, 67.2)

aGroup differences tested with chi-square (categorical variables) and independent samples t-test (continuous variables). Mean (SD) presented for continuous variables, (n, %) presented for categorical variables.

bHigher scores indicate more socioeconomic disadvantage.

In adjusted multivariable analyses, we found lower mean memory scores for both past 30-days (p = 0.001) and past-year (p = 0.015) marijuana users, compared to non-users (Table 2). Problematic users had worse scores than non-problematic users on the memory test (p = 0.048) but not on the learning test. Furthermore, there was some indication that early onset users -defined as participants who reported using marijuana for the first time at or prior to age 169- scored worse on the learning (p = 0.135) and memory (p = 0.068) tests when compared with late onset users and never users, but these findings did not achieve statistical significance. Past 30-day frequent users had significantly lower mean learning (p = 0.049) and memory (p = 0.001) scores compared to non-frequent or non-users. We also tested higher levels of use (≥ 6 uses in past 30 days; ≥ 40 uses in past year). More frequent use was consistently associated with lower scores on both tests.

Table 2 Marijuana use and verbal learning and memory scores in young adulthood*  

Verbal learning p value Memory p value
12-Month marijuana use
Yes 26.2 (0.2) 0.735 9.4 (0.1) 0.015
No 26.3 (0.2) -- 9.8 (0.1) --
30-Day marijuana use
Yes 25.9 (0.3) 0.130 9.2 (0.1) 0.001
No 26.4 (0.2) -- 9.7 (0.1) --
12-Month frequent use (≥40 uses)
Yes 24.9 (0.4) 0.000 8.8 (0.2) <0.001
No 26.5 (0.2) -- 9.7 (0.1) --
30-Day frequent use (≥ 3 uses)
Yes 25.6 (0.4) 0.049 9.1 (0.2) 0.001
No 26.4 (0.2) -- 9.7 (0.1) --
Problematic Use (≥1 problem)
Yes 26.1 (0.3) 0.471 9.4 (0.1) 0.048
No 26.4 (0.2) -- 9.7 (0.1) --
Age of onset of marijuana use
Early (≤16 yr) 25.9 (0.3) 0.135 9.3 (0.1) 0.068
Late (>16 yr) 26.3 (0.2) -- 9.6 (0.1) --
Never Use 26.7 (0.3) -- 9.8 (0.1) --

*All models are adjusted for sex, age at substance use assessment, education, binge drinking, and cigarette use.

Discussion

We assessed the association between marijuana use and cognition in this sample of Chilean young adults. Participants who reported marijuana use over the last year or past 30 days performed worse than never-users on a delayed recall task measuring verbal memory. Frequent marijuana use was also associated with worse performance for verbal learning and memory. Assessing varying levels of exposure, we found significant negative effects of marijuana use even for individuals who reported using “at least once” in the past year -relatively lenient criteria compared to other studies. As we altered the cut-off points to focus on more intense marijuana use in the exposed group, the magnitude of the effect increased.

Previous studies found marijuana use to be associated with poorer cognition, yet a large meta-analysis suggests these conclusions may have been overstated. Limitations of previous studies included small sample size (4-6, 8) and varying definitions of marijuana use. Some studies had stringent criteria (4-6 uses/week), while others assessed infrequent users (e.g. 10 joints in past year). Furthermore, there has been an overreliance on defining problematic marijuana use only by frequency of use, rather than by marijuana use causing problems in daily life19. Given recent changes in Chilean marijuana legislation, shifting attitudes towards marijuana use, and increasingly prevalent use among Chileans, this study provides important information on the potential adverse effects of such use.

This study has several limitations. Participants cannot be considered representative of the Santiago population. Results should be replicated in other settings to determine generalizability. Furthermore, attrition was a limitation, as this study included only 654 of the original 1,790 infancy cohort participants. However, current study participants did not differ significantly from the original cohort regarding most baseline characteristics, with the exception of supplementation group. Participants that were randomized to high and low iron supplementation in infancy were more likely to be included in this analysis compared to infants randomized to no iron supplementation. Another limitation is that the measures used to quantify exposure (marijuana use) and outcome (verbal memory performance) were not specifically validated for a Chilean population. However, there is likely cross-cultural validity15 of verbal memory tasks, such as the CogState ISL. Participants retrospectively reported timing and frequency of marijuana use, thus risking recall bias. Furthermore, some participants may have been experiencing acute effects of marijuana at the time of evaluation, as no abstinence period was enforced20. In addition, we analyzed the effect of marijuana use on only one cognitive measure, verbal recall ability, and we were unable to adjust for the level of effort during the evaluation. Observed associations may vary across cognitive domains. Further understanding of these nuances will help elucidate plausible mechanisms. Another limitation is the cross-sectional nature of the analysis. These analyses will be strengthened by further follow-up of participants to test persistence of observed associations. Additionally, the field could benefit from understanding the predictors of marijuana use as well as the effect of marijuana use on later cognitive and functional outcomes21,22.

Strengths of this study include the relatively large sample size and use of a valid and reliable assessment of verbal learning and memory (CogState-ISL). Marijuana use was defined consistently, and problematic use was based on a validated 14-item questionnaire (see Appendix A). Although participants were not assessed for clinical psychopathology diagnosis or Substance Use Disorder, our study in a non-clinical, non-U.S. sample is relevant to more typical substance use. Considering the continued uncertainty surrounding the short-term and long-term cognitive harms of marijuana, more research is warranted, and this study adds to this research.

In this study of 21-year-olds from Santiago, Chile, marijuana use correlated with poorer cognitive outcomes. A cross-sectional association between increased marijuana use and worse cognitive outcomes implies a need to investigate the relationship between marijuana use and cognitive changes over time. In this era of liberalization of marijuana, scientific research on the effects of marijuana on cognitive, behavioral, psychological, and functional outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood is particularly timely. Such research should expand beyond clinical samples to increase the likelihood of external validity.

Funding: This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (R01HD033487) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01DA021181).

Appendix A

Problematic marijuana use items

The following questions ask the participant (marijuana user) if consuming marijuana has caused any of the following problems during his/her life:

  1. You have behaved in some way that you later regretted. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  2. Has caused problems with your parents. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  3. Has caused problems with your husband/wife or girlfriend/boyfriend. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  4. Has caused problems with your friends. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  5. Has caused problems with your teachers or bosses. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  6. Has caused you to get together with people who have a bad influence. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  7. Has caused problems in school or work. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  8. Has caused you to have less interest in activities where you used to have more interest. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  9. Has caused emotional instability. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  10. Has caused you to have less energy. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  11. Has interfered with your ability to think well. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  12. Has caused other psychological problems. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  13. Has caused problems for your health. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

  14. Has caused problems with the police. (1 = No, 2 = Yes)

Acknowledgements:

We would like to acknowledge the participants and their families for making this study possible. We also acknowledge the contributions of Professor Bruno Giordani at the University of Michigan for his consultation on neuropsychological instruments for this study. This work was supported by Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Development (R01HD033487) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01DA021181-05).

References

1. Ministerio del Interior y Seguridad Pública Sustituye la ley N° 19.366, que sanciona el tráfico ilícito de estupefacientes y sustancias psicotrópicas. https://www.leychile.cl/N?i=235507&f=2015-10-22&p= (octubre, 2015) 2005. [ Links ]

2. Informe del uso de drogas en las Américas. Washington: OAS. Recuperado de http://www.cicad.oas.org/oid/pubs/Uso_de_Drogas_en_Americas2011_Esp.pdf (junio, 2014) 2011. [ Links ]

3. Ministerio del Interior y Seguridad Pública. Décimo Segundo Estudio de Drogas en Población General. Chile: Observatorio Chileno de Drogas. http://www.senda.gob.cl/wp-content/uploads/2017/12/InformeENPG2016.pdf (diciembre, 2017) 2016. [ Links ]

4. Becker MP, Collins PF, Luciana M. Neurocognition in college-aged daily marijuana users. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2014; 36 (4): 379-98. [ Links ]

5. Lisdahl KM, Price JS. Increased marijuana use and gender predict poorer cognitive functioning in adolescents and emerging adults. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2012; 18 (4): 678-88. [ Links ]

6. Medina KL, Hanson KL, Schweinsburg AD, Cohen-Zion M, Nagel BJ, Tapert SF. Neuropsychological functioning in adolescent marijuana users: subtle deficits detectable after a month of abstinence. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13 (5): 807-20. [ Links ]

7. Winward JL, Hanson KL, Tapert SF, Brown SA. Heavy alcohol use, marijuana use, and concomitant use by adolescents are associated with unique and shared cognitive decrements. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2014; 20 (8): 784-95. [ Links ]

8. Filbey FM, Aslan S, Calhoun VD, Spence JS, Damaraju E, Caprihan A, et al. Long-term effects of marijuana use on the brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2014; 111 (47): 16913-18. [ Links ]

9. Schuster RM, Hoeppner SS, Evins AE, Gilman JM. Early onset marijuana use is associated with learning inefficiences. Neuropsychology 2016; 30 (4): 405-15. [ Links ]

10. Huepe D, Roca M, Canales-Johnson A, Rivera-Rei ÁA, Zamorano L, Concepción A, et al. Fluid intelligence and psychosocial outcome: From logical problem solving to social adaptation. PLoS One 2011; 6 (9): e24858. [ Links ]

11. Scott JC, Slomiak ST, Jones JD, Rosen AFG, Moore TM, Gur RC. Association of cannabis with cognitive functioning in adolescents and young adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2018; 75 (6): 585-95. [ Links ]

12. Lozoff B, De Andraca I, Castillo M, Smith JB, Walter T, Pino P. Behavioral and developmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy full-term infants. Pediatrics 2003; 112 (4): 846-54. [ Links ]

13. Roncagliolo M, Garrido M, Walter T, Peirano P, Lozoff B. Evidence of altered central nervous system development in infants with iron deficiency anemia at 6 mo: delayed maturation of auditory brainstem responses. Am J Clin Nutr 1998; 68 (3): 683-90. [ Links ]

14. Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE, O’Malley PM, Johnston LD, Bachman JG. Adolescents’ reported reasons for alcohol and marijuana use as predictors of substance use and problems in adulthood. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2011; 72 (1): 106-16. [ Links ]

15. Lim YY, Prang KH, Cysique L, Pietrzak RH, Synder PJ, Maruff P. A method for cross-cultural adaptation of a verbal memory assessment. Behav Res Methods 2009; 41 (4) : 1190-200. [ Links ]

16. Maruff P, Thomas E, Cysique L, Brew B, Collie A, Snyder P, et al. Validity of the CogState brief battery: relationship to standardized tests and sensitivity to cognitive impairment in mild traumatic brain injury, schizophrenia, and AIDS dementia complex. Arch Clin Neuropsych 2009; 24 (2): 165-78. [ Links ]

17. Graffar M. Une méthode de classification sociale d’échantillons de population. Courrier 1956; 6: 455-9. [ Links ]

18. Schuster RM, Crane NA, Mermelstein R, González R. Tobacco may mask poorer episodic memory among young adult cannabis users. Neuropsychology 2015; 29 (5) : 759-66. [ Links ]

19. Ashbridge M, Duff C, Marsh DC, Erikson PG. Problems with the identification of ‘problematic’ cannabis use: examining the issues of frequency, quantity, and drug use environment. Eur Addict Res 2014; 20 (5): 254-67. [ Links ]

20. Hanson KL, Winward JL, Schweinsburg AD, Medina KL, Brown SA, Tapert SF. Longitudinal study of cognition among adolescent marijuana users over three weeks of abstinence. Addict Behav 2010; 35 (11): 970-6. [ Links ]

21. McKetin R, Parasu P, Cherbuin N, Eramudugolla R, Anstey KJ. A longitudinal examination of the relationship between cannabis use and cognitive function in mid-life adults. Drug Alcohol Depend 2016; 169: 134-40. [ Links ]

22. Tait RJ, Mackinnon A, Christensen H. Cannabis use and cognitive function: 8-year trajectory in a young adult cohort. Addiction 2011; 106 (2): 2195-203. [ Links ]

Received: March 27, 2017; Accepted: January 02, 2019

Correspondence to: Sheila Gahagan, MD, MPH Department of Pediatrics, Division of Child Development and Community Health, University of California, San Diego. 4305 University Avenue, Suite 590. San Diego, CA, USA 92105. Fax: 619-681-0666 sgahagan@ucsd.edu

a

Psychology and Public Health pre-graduate student

b

Psychologist.

c

Epidemiologist.

d

Social worker.

e

MPH.

f

MA.

g

BS.

h

PhD.

i

MS.

j

MD.

Creative Commons License This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.