Next Article in Journal
Current Consumer Perceptions of Animal Welfare across Different Farming Sectors on the Island of Ireland
Previous Article in Journal
Gene-Set Enrichment Analysis for Identifying Genes and Biological Activities Associated with Growth Traits in Dromedaries
Previous Article in Special Issue
Emotions and Dog Bites: Could Predatory Attacks Be Triggered by Emotional States?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Context-Dependent Gestural Laterality: A Multifactorial Analysis in Captive Red-Capped Mangabeys

by
Juliette Aychet
1,*,
Noémie Monchy
1,
Catherine Blois-Heulin
1,† and
Alban Lemasson
1,2,†
1
Univ Rennes, Normandie Univ, CNRS, EthoS (Éthologie Animale et Humaine)—UMR 6552, 35380 Paimpont, France
2
Institut Universitaire de France, CEDEX 05, 75231 Paris, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Animals 2022, 12(2), 186; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12020186
Submission received: 6 December 2021 / Revised: 6 January 2022 / Accepted: 10 January 2022 / Published: 13 January 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Advanced Research in Animal Communication)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

In vertebrates, each of the two hemispheres of the brain controls the opposite side of the body. Consequently, lateralized actions in animals may reflect that one hemisphere processes particular functions underlying the behavior. In humans, for instance, the left-brain hemisphere is specialized for language functions. In several species of non-human primates, communicative gestures are preferentially produced with the right hand. It has been hypothesized that the human language lateralization has common evolutionary origins with a left-hemispheric specialization for gestures in African-Asian primates. Therefore, it is particularly interesting to describe the gestural laterality of non-human primates to understand the evolutionary history of intentional communication. Previous studies reported different factors affecting the gestural laterality of great apes, but this has rarely been investigated in more distant species of monkeys. In the present study, we observed the manual gestures produced by captive mangabeys. We found that, although monkeys were not lateralized when considering their gestures, on the whole, they preferentially gesture with their right hand in contexts of aggression or submission, and that the position of their receiver also affected the hand used. As for chimpanzees, gorillas, or humans, the gestural laterality of mangabeys depends on the context in which they communicate.

Abstract

Catarrhine primates gesture preferentially with their right hands, which led to the hypothesis of a gestural origin of human left-hemispheric specialization for language. However, the factors influencing this gestural laterality remain understudied in non-hominoid species, particularly in intraspecific contexts, although it may bring valuable insights into the proximate and ultimate causes of language lateralization. We present here a preliminary investigation of intraspecific gestural laterality in catarrhine monkeys, red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus). We described the spontaneous production of brachio-manual intentional gestures in twenty-five captive subjects. Although we did not evidence any significant gestural lateralization neither at the individual- nor population-level, we found that mangabeys preferentially use their right hands to gesture in negative social contexts, such as aggressions, suggesting an effect of emotional lateralization, and that they adapt to the position of their receiver by preferentially using their ipsilateral hand to communicate. These results corroborate previous findings from ape studies. By contrast, factors related to gesture form and socio-demographic characteristics of signaler and receiver did not affect gestural laterality. To understand better the relationships between gestural laterality and brain lateralization from an evolutionary perspective, we suggest that the gestural communication of other monkey species should be examined with a multifactorial approach.

1. Introduction

Humans exhibit a left-hemispheric specialization for language functions, particularly through Broca’s and Wernicke’s brain areas, which results in a right-biased lateralization for verbal and non-verbal communicative behaviors [1,2,3,4,5,6,7]. Great apes also exhibit neuroanatomical asymmetries in favor of the left hemisphere in brain regions homologous to Broca’s area (in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, bonobos, Pan paniscus, and gorillas, Gorilla gorilla [8]), and functional association of these regions with gestural communication have been evidenced in chimpanzees [9,10,11]. A communicative gesture can be defined as a non-locomotor and mechanically ineffective brachio-manual signal, that is directed to a recipient and lead to a voluntary response from the latter [12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21], although it has to be noted that this operational definition may vary depending on the authors [22]. Population-level right-handedness for the production of communicative gestures has been evidenced in several catarrhine primates (e.g., chimpanzees [23,24,25,26], gorillas [27,28,29], Tonkean macaques, Macaca tonkeana [30,31], olive baboons, Papio anubis [31,32,33]). Moreover, gestural laterality is dissociated from hand preference observed in non-communicative actions in numerous species (e.g., in chimpanzees [26], Campbell’s mona monkeys, Cercopithecus campbelli, and red-capped mangabeys, Cercocebus torquatus [34], Tonkean macaques [30], and olive baboons [35]), which suggests different neuronal control for communicative and non-communicative gestures. These neuroanatomical and behavioral similarities between language lateralization and gesture production in several primate species, together with their phylogenetical proximity with Homo sapiens [36,37,38], led to the hypothesis that the brain lateralization for language control is inherited from a left-hemispheric specialization for intentional gestures in the common ancestor of humans and other catarrhine primates [39,40,41,42,43,44,45]. Thus, studying gestural laterality in primates seems particularly relevant to elucidate the proximate and ultimate causes of language lateralization.
Different hypotheses have been formulated concerning the emergence of manual laterality. Notably, the “postural origin theory” [46,47] proposes that manual laterality results from adaptations of the right hand for complex tasks in terrestrial species, free from postural constraints implied by arboreal locomotion. Some authors also hypothesized that manual laterality evidenced in captive non-human primates was the by-product of experimental biases and captivity (“artefactual hypothesis”) [48,49,50]. Looking specifically at communicative gestures, different factors might influence manual laterality in primates. Firstly, gestural laterality may depend on factors related to the interactional context, such as its emotional valence. Prieur and colleagues [26,29,51] evidenced that chimpanzees and gorillas preferentially use their right hands to gesture in aggression contexts, which may be explained by the hemispheric lateralization for emotions in vertebrates [52,53]. The relative position of the signaler and receiver during the interaction can also affect gestural laterality, as shown in chimpanzees and gorillas [26,29,54], presumably because of gesture directionality (which may imply a preferential use of the ipsilateral hand), and to asymmetrical emotional signals from recipient which could drive gesture production [26,55]. Secondly, gesture characteristics may affect hand preference, such as the type of gesture, as shown in chimpanzees and gorillas [25,26,29,54,56,57], or the sensory modality on which it is delivered (i.e., visual only, acoustic or tactile [26,54]). Moreover, in line with the “postural origin theory”, it can be hypothesized that the hand used to gesture depends on whether hands are engaged for signaler posture stability. This postural effect on laterality has notably been evidenced for manual tasks in red-capped mangabeys [58] and grey-cheeked mangabeys (Lophocebus albigena) [59]. Thirdly, sociodemographic characteristics of the signaler and receiver may affect gestural laterality. Although no or slight effects of dominance and kinship have been found in chimpanzees and gorillas [26,29,60], the age of the signaler affects gestural laterality in several species, as right-handedness increases with age (e.g., in chimpanzees [25,26,56]; gorillas [29,51]; olive baboons [32]).
To understand how all these factors may affect gestural laterality, it seems judicious to observe non-human primate gestures occurring in intraspecific contexts. Prieur and colleagues [26,29] were the first to adopt a multifactorial approach to characterize intraspecific gestural laterality, in chimpanzees and gorillas. To our knowledge, such method has never been adopted to study catarrhine monkeys’ gestural communication. Yet, right-handedness for communicative gestures have been evidenced in cercopithecid species (Tonkean macaques [30,31]; Campbell’s mona monkeys and red-capped mangabeys [34]), including for intraspecific gestures (olive baboons [32,33]), suggesting that it could be investigated beyond the great ape clade. Moreover, our understanding of the causes of gestural laterality, and, thus, to a greater extent our understanding of language evolutionary origins, benefits from comparisons between primates with different social systems and ecologies [61].
We present here a preliminary investigation of intraspecific gestural laterality in captive red-capped mangabeys, a semi-terrestrial monkey [62] originating from West African rainforest coastal regions [63,64,65,66]. They naturally live among multi-male, multi-female social groups, from 10 to 25 individuals [63,64], and exhibit linear hierarchy in captivity [67], with aggression and affiliation patterns both observed in despotic and relaxed macaque societies [68]. Recent results evidenced that captive red-capped mangabeys produce intentional body signals as part of their intraspecific communication, and, notably, manual gestures [69]. Experimental assessment of manual laterality in human-directed communication brought to light lateralized individuals for pointing gestures, and a dissociation of hand preference for communicative compared to non-communicative manual acts [34]. However, mangabeys’ intraspecific gestural laterality has never been studied. Our first aim here was to test which proximate factors would affect hand preference in red-capped mangabey gestures, by taking into account similar factors as those tested in multifactorial analyses in chimpanzees and gorillas [26,29], for comparison purposes. We tested the effects of the gesture production characteristics (sensory modality involved, signaler posture), of the interactional context (emotional valence of context, relative positions of signalers and receivers), and socio-demographic characteristics (age and sex of the signaler, relationship between signaler and receiver in terms of difference of age and sex, dominance, and kinship). Secondly, we tested whether captive red-capped mangabeys are lateralized at the individual and population-level for their intraspecific gestures. Based on previous findings in catarrhine apes and monkeys [25,26,28,32], we expected to find an overall right-hand bias in mangabey manual gesturing.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects and Housing Conditions

We observed twenty-five captive red-capped mangabeys housed at the Station Biologique de Paimpont (University of Rennes 1, France). Subjects were 10 females and 15 males, including 3 juveniles, 14 middle-aged, and 8 old adults (Table 1). They lived in social groups, from two to 13 individuals, which have been stable from at least 8 months at the moment of data collection. Mangabeys were housed in outdoor-indoor enclosures of different sizes (from 8 to 26.4 m2 for indoor enclosures, 14.7 to 37.2 m2 for outdoor enclosures, and height from 2.5 m to 4.4 m) and could move in and out freely, using connecting tunnels. The temperature of indoor enclosures was set at 22 °C. Enclosures were enriched with perches made of wood and metal, on which swinging chains and hessian ribbons were attached. The floor of the indoor enclosures was covered with straw and sawdust, while the floor of the outdoor enclosures was covered with cement or bark. Feeding occurred twice a day, with fresh fruits and vegetables in the morning and monkey chows in the afternoon. Water was available ad libitum.

2.2. Data Collection

2.2.1. Focal Observations

We analyzed observational data collected from the end of January to the end of June 2018 for the purpose of describing red-capped mangabeys’ intraspecific signaling (see Reference [70]). We recorded focal videos for each monkey (“individual focal sampling method” [71]) using a JVC Full HD GZ-RX615 camcorder (JVCKENWOOD Europe B.V.). An observation session corresponded to 15 min of recording (mean session duration ± S.E. = 15.67 ± 0.10 min), and each individual was observed during 8 sessions. We, thus, totalized more than 2 h (125.33 ± 0.75 min) of recording per individual, and 52.22 h of observations overall. Sessions were scheduled randomly, and then equilibrated so that all monkeys were observed at all times of day (from 9:00 a.m. to 6:00 p.m.) and at all feeding periods (before or during the first feeding/between two feedings/after or during the second feeding). BORIS v.6.0.6 software [72] was used to point each event of manual gesture and each social interaction involving the focal individual. We then coded parameters and behaviors associated to each gesture occurrence (i.e., the signaler and receiver identities, the type and characteristics of gesture, the social context, and relative position of interactants).

2.2.2. Brachio-Manual Gestures

We recorded every instance of unimanual gestures produced by the focal individuals, in intraspecific dyadic context. A brachio-manual gesture was defined as any movement of one arm or hand that was: (i) nonlocomotor and “mechanically ineffective”, though not excluding gestures that implied contact with a substrate or the recipient [15,73,74]; (ii) physically directed to a receiver, as shown by the signaler’s head or body orientation and/or physical contact between the signaler and the receiver [12,15,54,56,69,70,75,76], and/or preceded by “audience checking” within the 5 s before the gesture [17,70]; (iii) adapted to the receiver attention, i.e., produced in front of a visually attentive recipient (the signaler being more or less 45° in front of the receiver’s face), except for tactile and audible gestures [15,69,70,76,77,78]; and (iv) followed by a voluntary response of the receiver within 5 s [70,76,79,80].

2.2.3. Emotional Valence of Context

The social contexts of gesture production were determined based on the signaler and receiver’s behavior within the 5 s before and after the signal [70,81]. Six different contexts were distinguished, and then categorized according to their assumed emotional valence (Table 2). Aggression and submission contexts were assumed to be “negative”, in contrast to affiliation, grooming, social play and socio-sexual contexts that were qualified as “positive or neutral”. We note that the latter contexts may involve ambiguous behaviors, falling within “positive” contexts, while possibly eliciting negative emotions in interactants.

2.2.4. Relative Positions of Interactants

For each gesture instance, we also recorded the relative positions of the signaler and the receiver in each other’s visual field [26,29,51,54,82]. The receiver position was coded as “front” if it was positioned more or less 60° in front of either signaler’s face [83], and “right” or “left” depending on whether it was in the signaler’s right or left visual field. The position of the signaler was similarly coded from the receiver’s point of view.

2.2.5. Gesture Characteristics and Signaler’s Posture

For each gesture occurrence, we noted the hand used (left or right), the type of gesture produced [69,70], and the sensory modality on which it could be perceived. Gestures were classified as “visual” if they were silent and distant brachio-manual movements, “audible” if they provoked a sound, and “tactile” if they involved a physical contact with the receiver [16,69,70,77,84,85]. According to the “postural” theory on gestural laterality origins [46,47], signaler posture may constrain the availability of hands to gesture and, thus, be a determining factor shaping gestural laterality. To test for this effect in red-capped mangabeys, we categorized the signaler’s posture when gesturing, depending on whether one hand was “engaged” in posture as a support (subject walking or standing quadrupedally, standing bipedally with hand on substrate, climbing or hanging on cage) or both hands were “free” (subject sitting, laying, jumping, or standing bipedally without other support).

2.2.6. Signaler and Receiver Relationships

In order to assess the effect of social factors on the hand used to gesture, we characterized the relationship between the signaler and the receiver in terms of dominance and kinship. Dominance was deduced from avoidance behaviors [86], i.e., turning away from another individual, avoiding an individual by changing direction, avoiding contact by moving a body part away from the proximity of another individual, or fleeing an individual by walking or running in opposed direction [87]. All avoidance behaviors involving focal subjects were analyzed, and, when at least 3 occurrences were observed for a dyad, we computed the percentage of avoidance of subject A from subject B over the total number of these behaviors between A and B [88]. Subject A was considered subordinate to subject B if this percentage was above 50%. Moreover, kinship was known for all individuals in the colony, and the signaler and the receiver were considered as “kin” if they were first-degree relatives (parents or siblings). Finally, we noted whether the signaler and the receiver were of same age or not (based on age categories: juveniles, middle aged, and old adults) and of same sex or not.

2.3. Statistical Analyses

We used R v.3.6.2 [89] for all statistical analyses. All tests were two-tailed, and alpha-level was set at 0.05.
Firstly, we tested which factors affected hand use in red-capped mangabey gestural communication, among contextual factors (valence of social context, signaler and receiver positions in each other’s visual field), gesture production characteristics (gesture sensory modality, signaler posture), and socio-demographic factors (dominance and kinship between the signaler and receiver, signaler age category and sex). To do so, we used a Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM) of the binomial family to analyze the hand used to produce gestures (right or left), depending on the above-cited variables, and including the identity of the signaler as a random effect (R package {lme4}). The model quality was verified by checking for the absence of data overdispersion, the independence of model residuals, and the absence of fixed factors collinearity (R packages {RVAideMemoire} and {performance}).
Secondly, we tested the presence of an overall gestural laterality in our captive red-capped mangabeys. To assess gestural laterality at the individual level, for each individual that produced at least 6 manual gestures, we performed binomial tests to compare the proportion of right- and left-hand gestures to a theoretical proportion of 0.5. For each of these subjects, a handedness index (HI) was also computed using the following formula: HI = (R − L)/(R + L), where R corresponds to the number of gestures produced with the right hand and L with the left hand. This index, between −1 and 1, is commonly used to assess laterality [90], as its sign reveals the potential bias direction: toward the right if it is positive, and the left if it is negative. We tested whether HI differed from null values using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. The strength of individuals’ hand preference was estimated by the absolute value of HI (ABSHI). Gestural laterality was assessed at the population level based on the proportion of lateralized individuals in the population and the average ABSHI value. Moreover, we compared the number of gestures that mangabeys produced with the right or left hand using a Wilcoxon signed rank test.

3. Results

3.1. Manual Gestures

We recorded 275 occurrences of unimanual gestures produced in intraspecific dyadic contexts by captive red-capped mangabeys. Twenty of the 25 captive mangabeys produced manual gestures, and 10 different gestures were observed (Table 3), of which 7 have been previously described as intentional gestures [69].

3.2. Factors Affecting Hand Use in Gestural Communication

Contextual factors affected the hand they used to communicate (GLMM Binomial, detailed comparison results in Table 4). Particularly, we found a significant effect of the emotional valence of context in which the gestures were produced (GLMM Binomial, Type II Wald Chi-square test: X21 = 6.383, p = 0.011) with more right-hand gestures in “negative” contexts than in others (Figure 1). Moreover, the relative receiver and signaler’s positions had a significant effect on the hand used to gesture (receiver position: X22 = 29.233, p < 0.001; signaler position: X22 = 7.068, p = 0.029). Signalers adapted the side used to communicate to the position of the receivers, using more the right hand when the receivers were in their right visual field and conversely (Figure 2a). Moreover, they used less the right hand when they gestured in the right visual field of receivers than when they were in front of them (Figure 2b). Interestingly, we found no effect of the sensory modality of gestures on hand use (X22 = 3.148, p = 0.207), nor of the signaler posture (X21 = 1.373, p = 0.241) or socio-demographic factors (Dominance: X22 = 0.500, p = 0.779; Kinship: X21 = 0.005, p = 0.941; Age difference: X21 = 1.670, p = 0.196; Sex difference: X21 = 0.065, p = 0.799; Signaler’s age: X22 = 0.718, p = 0.698; Signaler’s sex: X21 = 0.246, p = 0.620).

3.3. Gestural Laterality at the Individual and Population Level

None of the red-capped mangabeys were significantly lateralized for their manual gestures (Binomial test, Table 5). Thus, there was no laterality bias at the population-level. The HI did not overall differ from null values (Wilcoxon signed rank test: N = 11, V = 30, p = 0.823), and the mean ABSHI value was low (ABSHI ± S.E. = 0.24 ± 0.04), as mangabeys produced as much manual gestures with the right than with the left hand (Wilcoxon signed rank test: N = 20, V = 98, p = 0.320).

4. Discussion

We present here a preliminary assessment of captive red-capped mangabey intraspecific gestural laterality. We found a significant effect of interactional context factors on the hand used by red-capped mangabeys to communicate, corroborating previous findings from ape studies and suggesting an effect of emotional lateralization on gestural communication. On the contrary, factors related to gesture production and socio-demographic characteristics of the signaler and the receiver did not affect mangabey gestural laterality. None of the individuals we observed were significantly lateralized when looking at their overall gesture production; thus, no population-level bias was observed, which we discuss with regards of our sample size.
We found that contextual factors affected the hand used by red-capped mangabeys to communicate with their conspecifics. As found in chimpanzees and gorillas [26,29,51], mangabeys used their right hand more to gesture in contexts associated with negative emotional states (i.e., aggression and submission contexts). At first sight, these findings may seem to contradict theories on brain asymmetries for emotion processing, which would predict a left-hand preference for negative contexts [52]. The “right hemisphere theory” indeed proposes that the right hemisphere controls perception and expressions of emotions [91,92,93], and the “valence theory” suggests that the right and left hemispheres, respectively, control negative emotions, frequently associated with withdrawal behaviors, and positive emotions, frequently associated with approaches [94,95,96]. Prieur and colleagues hypothesized that the observed right-hand bias of apes in negative contexts originates from the same mechanisms that underlie left prefrontal brain region activation in humans during negative events, such as aggressions [97]. Taking the perspective that aggression contexts involve not only negative emotions (e.g., anger) but also motivation for “approaching” behaviors [97], this seems actually in accordance with the “approach/withdrawal” theory on brain asymmetries [52,97,98,99], which could explain results on chimpanzees, gorillas, and red-capped mangabey gestures. Aside from the effect of the context valence, we found that gestural laterality was significantly affected by the relative positions of the signaler and the receiver. Red-capped mangabeys preferentially used their ipsilateral hand to gesture toward conspecifics, probably to more efficiently direct their signal. This is in accordance with observation of captive chimpanzees and gorillas [26,29,54], who preferentially performed visual and tactile gestures with their right hand when their recipient was in their right visual field. These results highlight the importance of taking into account the receivers’ positions when studying intraspecific gestural laterality of primates, or to control this parameter in experimental conditions, given its substantial effect on hand use.
Factors related to the gesture production in itself did not affect gestural laterality, since we found no effect of the sensory modality involved (visual only, acoustic or tactile) nor of the signaler posture while gesturing. Experimental studies brought to light an effect of red-capped mangabeys’ postures for manual tasks, and this was particularly true for complex tasks [58]. We can hypothesize that this postural effect is not found for communicative gestures because they are less demanding in terms of movement precision, compared for instance to bimanual manipulation tasks (“task complexity hypothesis” [100]). Interestingly, socio-demographic characteristics of the signaler and the receiver had no effect on gestural laterality. The signaler’s sex did not affect the hand used to gesture, as found in apes and olive baboons [28,32,33,35,60]. Although right-hand preference has been found to increase with age in apes and olive baboons [25,26,29,32,51,56], we did not find this effect in red-capped mangabeys. This is not surprising regarding the absence of lateralized individuals in our population, but these aspects should be further investigated with an increased sample size. Finally, the absence of dominance or kinship effect on mangabey gestural laterality was in accordance with the results obtained from ape studies [29,60]. One study showed an effect of social hierarchy on gestural laterality of primates, describing more right-handedness in subordinate captive chimpanzees than in other individuals [26], and this was hypothesized to result from the higher level of psychological stress in these individuals, in line with the effect of negative context on right-handedness. We only analyzed here the effect of dominance relationships determined at the dyadic level, but future studies with larger samples within a same social group may permit to test the effect of mangabeys’ individual hierarchical ranks on gestural laterality. A previous study evidenced an effect of social hierarchy on social laterality in captive mangabeys: the individuals that were approached more frequently from their left side were the ones with higher social ranks [83]. As we show here that monkeys’ relative positions affect the hand used to gesture, we may hypothesize an indirect effect of mangabeys’ social ranks on gestural laterality. Social status has been shown to be related to behavioral lateralization in diverse species (e.g., in humans [101,102] but also in more evolutionary distant species, such as geckos, Ptyodactylus guttatus [103]), which may reflect some social benefits conferred by lateralization at the individual level.
Looking at the production of brachio-manual gesture as a whole, we found no significant gestural lateralization at the individual nor population level in captive red-capped mangabeys. Given that population-level right handedness for communicative gesture was found in several catarrhine monkey species [30,31,32,33], we hypothesize that the absence of gestural lateralization found here results from our sample size, which is the main limit on research on behavioral laterality [104]. On one hand, the small number of data points per subjects could hide existing individual biases [27]. On the other hand, the number of subjects may be not sufficient to reveal a population-level bias [105,106]. A previous study evidenced right-handed mangabeys for human-directed pointing gestures, yet this was found only for the individuals with high referential communication abilities, suggesting substantial inter-individual variability for this trait [34]. Present results should be completed by measures on an increased sample, to confirm or contradict this hypothesis. This should also permit to investigate the effect of subjects’ characteristics (such as age, sex, or hierarchical rank) on gestural lateralization at the individual level. Moreover, it should permit to analyze independently the different types of gestures produced by mangabeys, as gesture type could affect the direction and strength of laterality [25,26,29,54,56,57].

5. Conclusions

This first investigation of captive red-capped mangabey gestural laterality corroborates previous findings from ape gesture studies, particularly regarding the effect of interactional context on hand use, which suggests that emotional lateralization affects manual preference in non-human primate gestural communication. We did not highlight any bias at the individual or population level, possibly because of our sample size. Thus, these promising results should be completed with further observational studies of mangabey gestures, and by similar investigations in other monkey genera, with the view to understand the evolutionary roots of modern human language and handedness.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani12020186/s1, Table S1: Analyzed data on red-capped mangabeys’ gestural laterality.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.A., C.B.-H. and A.L.; Methodology, J.A., C.B.-H. and A.L.; Formal Analysis, N.M. and J.A.; Investigation, J.A.; Writing—original draft, J.A.; Writing—review & editing, all authors; Visualization: J.A.; Supervision: C.B.-H. and A.L.; Project administration: C.B.-H. and A.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was purely observational and did not imply any modification of the living conditions of the captive mangabeys housed at the Station Biologique de Paimpont (University of Rennes 1, France), where animal facilities and animal care procedures are regularly monitored by the responsible local authorities (Housing agreement for research D35-211-18, delivered by the “Direction Départementale de la Cohésion Sociale et de la Protection des Populations” (DDCSPP)).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are openly available as a supplementary table (Supplementary Table S1).

Acknowledgments

We are very thankful to Serenella D’Ingeo, Angelo Quaranta and Marcello Siniscalchi for the invitation to contribute to this special issue. We would like to thank also Arnaud Rossard and Philippe Bec for taking care of the monkeys at the Station Biologique de Paimpont, and for their flexibility regarding our observation schedule.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Broca, P. Sur le Siège de la Faculté du Langage Articulé. Bull. Soc. D’anthropologie Paris 1865, 6, 377–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Wernicke, C. Der Aphasische Symptomencomplex: Eine Psychologische Studie auf Anatomischer Basis; Cohn & Weigert: Breslau, Poland, 1874. [Google Scholar]
  3. Kimura, D. Manual Activity during Speaking—I. Right-Handers. Neuropsychologia 1973, 11, 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Grossi, G.; Semenza, C.; Corazza, S.; Volterra, V. Hemispheric Specialization for Sign Language. Neuropsychologia 1996, 34, 737–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Lieberman, P. On the Nature and Evolution of the Neural Bases of Human Language. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2002, 119, 36–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Cooper, D.L. Broca’s Arrow: Evolution, Prediction, and Language in the Brain. Anat. Rec. 2006, 289B, 9–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Vigneau, M.; Beaucousin, V.; Hervé, P.Y.; Duffau, H.; Crivello, F.; Houdé, O.; Mazoyer, B.; Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. Meta-Analyzing Left Hemisphere Language Areas: Phonology, Semantics, and Sentence Processing. NeuroImage 2006, 30, 1414–1432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Cantalupo, C.; Hopkins, W.D. Asymmetric Broca’s Area in Great Apes. Nature 2001, 414, 505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Taglialatela, J.P.; Cantalupo, C.; Hopkins, W.D. Gesture Handedness Predicts Asymmetry in the Chimpanzee Inferior Frontal Gyrus. Neuroreport 2006, 17, 923–927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Taglialatela, J.P.; Russell, J.L.; Schaeffer, J.A.; Hopkins, W.D. Communicative Signaling Activates ‘Broca’s’ Homolog in Chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 2008, 18, 343–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Meguerditchian, A.; Gardner, M.J.; Schapiro, S.J.; Hopkins, W.D. The Sound of One-Hand Clapping: Handedness and Perisylvian Neural Correlates of a Communicative Gesture in Chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2012, 279, 1959–1966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Tomasello, M.; George, B.L.; Kruger, A.C.; Jeffrey, M.; Farrar; Evans, A. The Development of Gestural Communication in Young Chimpanzees. J. Hum. Evol. 1985, 14, 175–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Liebal, K.; Müllr, C.; Pika, S. Gestural Communication in Nonhuman and Human Primates; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands; Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cartmill, E.A.; Byrne, R.W. Orangutans Modify Their Gestural Signaling According to Their Audience’s Comprehension. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 1345–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Pollick, A.S.; De Waal, F.B. Ape Gestures and Language Evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 8184–8189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Pika, S. Gestures of Apes and Pre-Linguistic Human Children: Similar or Different? First Lang. 2008, 28, 116–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Hobaiter, C.; Byrne, R.W. The Gestural Repertoire of the Wild Chimpanzee. Anim. Cogn. 2011, 14, 745–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Bard, K.; Dunbar, S.; Maguire-Herring, V.; Veira, Y.; G Hayes, K.; McDonald, K. Gestures and Social-Emotional Communicative Development in Chimpanzee Infants. Am. J. Primatol. 2014, 76, 14–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gupta, S.; Sinha, A. Not Here, There! Possible Referential Gesturing during Allogrooming by Wild Bonnet Macaques, Macaca Radiata. Anim. Cogn. 2016, 19, 1243–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Tomasello, M.; Call, J. Thirty Years of Great Ape Gestures. Anim. Cogn. 2018, 22, 461–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Fröhlich, M.; Müller, G.; Zeiträg, C.; Wittig, R.M.; Pika, S. Gestural Development of Chimpanzees in the Wild: The Impact of Interactional Experience. Anim. Behav. 2017, 134, 271–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bourjade, M.; Cochet, H.; Molesti, S.; Guidetti, M. Is Conceptual Diversity an Advantage for Scientific Inquiry? A Case Study on the Concept of “Gesture” in Comparative Psychology. Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2020, 54, 805–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Fletcher, A.W. Clapping in Chimpanzees: Evidence of Exclusive Hand Preference in a Spontaneous, Bimanual Gesture. Am. J. Primatol. 2006, 68, 1081–1088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Meguerditchian, A.; Vauclair, J.; Hopkins, W.D. Captive Chimpanzees Use Their Right Hand to Communicate with Each Other: Implications for the Origin of the Cerebral Substrate for Language. Cortex 2010, 46, 40–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Hobaiter, C.; Byrne, R.W. Laterality in the Gestural Communication of Wild Chimpanzees. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2013, 1288, 9–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Prieur, J.; Pika, S.; Barbu, S.; Blois-Heulin, C. A Multifactorial Investigation of Captive Chimpanzees’ Intraspecific Gestural Laterality. Anim. Behav. 2016, 116, 31–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hopkins, W.D.; Pika, S.; Liebal, K.; Bania, A.; Meguerditchian, A.; Gardner, M.; Schapiro, S.J. Handedness for Manual Gestures in Great Apes. Dev. Primate Gesture Res. 2012, 6, 93–111. [Google Scholar]
  28. Prieur, J.; Pika, S.; Barbu, S.; Blois-Heulin, C. Gorillas Are Right-Handed for Their Most Frequent Intraspecific Gestures. Anim. Behav. 2016, 118, 165–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Prieur, J.; Pika, S.; Barbu, S.; Blois-Heulin, C. A Multifactorial Investigation of Captive Gorillas’ Intraspecific Gestural Laterality. Laterality Asymmetries Body Brain Cogn. 2017, 23, 538–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Meunier, H.; Fizet, J.; Vauclair, J. Tonkean Macaques Communicate with Their Right Hand. Brain Lang. 2013, 126, 181–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Meunier, H.; Fagard, J.; Maugard, A.; Briseño, M.; Fizet, J.; Canteloup, C.; Defolie, C.; Vauclair, J. Patterns of Hemispheric Specialization for a Communicative Gesture in Different Primate Species. Dev. Psychobiol. 2013, 55, 662–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Meguerditchian, A.; Vauclair, J. Baboons Communicate with Their Right Hand. Behav. Brain Res. 2006, 171, 170–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Meguerditchian, A.; Molesti, S.; Vauclair, J. Right-Handedness Predominance in 162 Baboons (Papio anubis) for Gestural Communication: Consistency across Time and Groups. Behav. Neurosci. 2011, 125, 653–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Maille, A.; Chapelain, A.; Déruti, L.; Bec, P.; Blois-Heulin, C. Manual Laterality for Pointing Gestures Compared to Grasping Actions in Guenons and Mangabeys. Anim. Behav. 2013, 86, 705–716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Meguerditchian, A.; Vauclair, J. Contrast of Hand Preferences between Communicative Gestures and Non-Communicative Actions in Baboons: Implications for the Origins of Hemispheric Specialization for Language. Brain Lang. 2009, 108, 167–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Raaum, R.L.; Sterner, K.N.; Noviello, C.M.; Stewart, C.-B.; Disotell, T.R. Catarrhine Primate Divergence Dates Estimated from Complete Mitochondrial Genomes: Concordance with Fossil and Nuclear DNA Evidence. J. Hum. Evol. 2005, 48, 237–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Chatterjee, H.J.; Ho, S.Y.; Barnes, I.; Groves, C. Estimating the Phylogeny and Divergence Times of Primates Using a Supermatrix Approach. BMC Evol. Biol. 2009, 9, 259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Perelman, P.; Johnson, W.E.; Roos, C.; Seuánez, H.N.; Horvath, J.E.; Moreira, M.A.M.; Kessing, B.; Pontius, J.; Roelke, M.; Rumpler, Y.; et al. A Molecular Phylogeny of Living Primates. PLoS Genet. 2011, 7, e1001342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Meguerditchian, A.; Vauclair, J.; Hopkins, W.D. On the Origins of Human Handedness and Language: A Comparative Review of Hand Preferences for Bimanual Coordinated Actions and Gestural Communication in Nonhuman Primates. Dev. Psychobiol. 2013, 55, 637–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hewes, G.W.; Andrew, R.J.; Carini, L.; Choe, H.; Gardner, R.A.; Kortlandt, A.; Krantz, G.S.; McBride, G.; Nottebohm, F.; Pfeiffer, J.; et al. Primate Communication and the Gestural Origin of Language [and Comments and Reply]. Curr. Anthropol. 1973, 14, 5–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Corballis, M.C. From Mouth to Hand: Gesture, Speech, and the Evolution of Right-Handedness. Behav. Brain Sci. 2003, 26, 199–208; discussion 208–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Corballis, M.C. The Gestural Origins of Language. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2010, 1, 2–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Vauclair, J. Lateralization of Communicative Signals in Nonhuman Primates and the Hypothesis of the Gestural Origin of Language. Interact. Stud. 2004, 5, 365–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Prieur, J.; Barbu, S.; Blois-Heulin, C.; Lemasson, A. The Origins of Gestures and Language: History, Current Advances and Proposed Theories. Biol. Rev. 2019, 95, 531–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Prieur, J.; Lemasson, A.; Barbu, S.; Blois-Heulin, C. History, Development and Current Advances Concerning the Evolutionary Roots of Human Right-Handedness and Language: Brain Lateralisation and Manual Laterality in Non-Human Primates. Ethology 2019, 125, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. MacNeilage, P.F.; Studdert-Kennedy, M.G.; Lindblom, B. Primate Handedness Reconsidered. Behav. Brain Sci. 1987, 10, 247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. MacNeilage, P.F. Present Status of the Postural Origins Theory. In Special Topics in Primatology; The Evolution of Hemispheric Specialization in Primates; Hopkins, W.D., Ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2007; Volume 5, pp. 58–91. [Google Scholar]
  48. Warren, J.M. Handedness and Laterality in Humans and Other Animals. Physiol. Psychol. 1980, 8, 351–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. McGrew, W.C.; Marchant, L.F. On the Other Hand: Current Issues in and Meta-Analysis of the Behavioral Laterality of Hand Function in Nonhuman Primates. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1997, 104, 201–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. McGrew, W.C.; Marchant, L.E. Ethological Study of Manual Laterality in the Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Behaviour 2001, 138, 329–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Prieur, J.; Barbu, S.; Blois-Heulin, C.; Pika, S. Captive Gorillas’ Manual Laterality: The Impact of Gestures, Manipulators and Interaction Specificity. Brain Lang. 2017, 175, 130–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Demaree, H.A.; Everhart, D.E.; Youngstrom, E.A.; Harrison, D.W. Brain Lateralization of Emotional Processing: Historical Roots and a Future Incorporating “Dominance”. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2005, 4, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Goursot, C.; Düpjan, S.; Puppe, B.; Leliveld, L.M.C. Affective Styles and Emotional Lateralization: A Promising Framework for Animal Welfare Research. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 237, 105279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Prieur, J.; Pika, S.; Barbu, S.; Blois-Heulin, C. Intraspecific Gestural Laterality in Chimpanzees and Gorillas and the Impact of Social Propensities. Brain Res. 2017, 1670, 52–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Wallez, C.; Schaeffer, J.; Meguerditchian, A.; Vauclair, J.; Schapiro, S.J.; Hopkins, W.D. Contrast of Hemispheric Lateralization for Oro-Facial Movements between Learned Attention-Getting Sounds and Species-Typical Vocalizations in Chimpanzees: Extension in a Second Colony. Brain Lang. 2012, 123, 75–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Hopkins, W.D.; Leavens, D.A. Hand Use and Gestural Communication in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 1998, 112, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Hopkins, W.D.; Wesley, M.J. Gestural Communication in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The Influence of Experimenter Position on Gesture Type and Hand Preference. Laterality Asymmetries Body Brain Cogn. 2002, 7, 19–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Blois-Heulin, C.; Guitton, J.S.; Nedellec-Bienvenue, D.; Ropars, L.; Vallet, E. Hand Preference in Unimanual and Bimanual Tasks and Postural Effect on Manual Laterality in Captive Red-Capped Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus). Am. J. Primatol. 2006, 68, 429–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Blois-Heulin, C.; Bernard, V.; Bec, P. Postural Effect on Manual Laterality in Different Tasks in Captive Grey-Cheeked Mangabey (Lophocebus albigena). J. Comp. Psychol. 2007, 121, 205–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Hopkins, W.D.; Russell, J.L.; Cantalupo, C.; Freeman, H.; Schapiro, S.J. Factors Influencing the Prevalence and Handedness for Throwing in Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 2005, 119, 363–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Meguerditchian, A. Pour une large approche comparative entre primates dans les recherches sur les origines de l’Homme: L’exemple de la latéralité manuelle. BMSAP 2014, 26, 166–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Mitani, J.C.; Call, J.; Kappeler, P.M.; Palombit, R.A.; Silk, J.B. (Eds.) The Evolution of Primate Societies; The University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA; London, UK, 2012; ISBN 978-0-226-53172-4. [Google Scholar]
  63. Jones, C.; Sabater-Pi, J. Comparative Ecology of Cercocebus Albigena and Cercocebus Torquatus in Rio Muni, West Africa. Folia Primatol. 1968, 9, 99–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Gautier-Hion, A.; Colyn, M.; Gautier, J.-P. Histoire Naturelle des Primates D’Afrique Centrale; ECOFAC: Libreville, Gabon, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  65. Cooke, C.A. The Feeding, Ranging and Positional Behaviors of Cercocebus torquatus, the Red-Capped Mangabey, in Sette Cama Gabon: A Phylogenetic Perspective. Ph.D. Dissertation, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  66. Orimaye, O.J. Density and Abundance of the Red-Capped Mangabey (Cercocebus torquatus) in Omo Biosphere Reserve and Idanre Forest Reserve, South Western Nigeria. MOJ Proteom. Bioinform. 2017, 5, 63–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Dolado, R.; Beltran, F.S. Dominance Hierarchy and Spatial Distribution in Captive Red-Capped Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus): Testing Hemelrijk’s Agent-Based Model. Interact. Stud. 2011, 12, 461–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Dolado, R.; Beltran, F.S. Emergent Patterns of Social Organization in Captive Cercocebus torquatus: Testing the GrooFiWorld Agent-Based Model. J. Biosci. 2012, 37, 777–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Schel, A.M.; Bono, A.; Aychet, J.; Pika, S.; Lemasson, A. Intentional Gesturing in Red-Capped Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus). In Animal Cognition; under Review; UniversitéParis Nanterre: Nanterre, France, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  70. Aychet, J.; Blois-Heulin, C.; Lemasson, A. Sequential and Network Analyses to Describe Multiple Signal Use in Captive Mangabeys. Anim. Behav. 2021, 182, 203–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Altmann, J. Observational Study of Behavior: Sampling Methods. Behaviour 1974, 49, 227–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Friard, O.; Gamba, M. BORIS: A Free, Versatile Open-Source Event-Logging Software for Video/Audio Coding and Live Observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2016, 7, 1325–1330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Pollick, A.S.; Jeneson, A.; de Waal, F.B.M. Gestures and Multimodal Signaling in Bonobos. In The Bonobos: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation; Furuichi, T., Thompson, J., Eds.; Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 75–94. ISBN 978-0-387-74787-3. [Google Scholar]
  74. Gupta, S.; Sinha, A. Gestural Communication of Wild Bonnet Macaques in the Bandipur National Park, Southern India. Behav. Processes 2019, 168, 103956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Call, J.; Tomasello, M. (Eds.) The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007; ISBN 978-0-8058-6278-2. [Google Scholar]
  76. Liebal, K.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Use of Gesture Sequences in Chimpanzees. Am. J. Primatol. 2004, 64, 377–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Pika, S.; Liebal, K.; Tomasello, M. Gestural Communication in Young Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Gestural Repertoire, Learning, and Use. Am. J. Primatol. 2003, 60, 95–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Genty, E.; Breuer, T.; Hobaiter, C.; Byrne, R.W. Gestural Communication of the Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): Repertoire, Intentionality and Possible Origins. Anim. Cogn. 2009, 12, 527–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Liebal, K.; Pika, S.; Tomasello, M. Gestural Communication of Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Gesture 2006, 6, 1–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Roberts, A.I.; Roberts, S.G.B.; Vick, S.-J. The Repertoire and Intentionality of Gestural Communication in Wild Chimpanzees. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 317–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Aychet, J.; Blois-Heulin, C.; Palagi, E.; Lemasson, A. Facial Displays in Red-Capped Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus): Repertoire, Social Context, and Potential Intentionality. J. Comp. Psychol. 2021, 135, 98–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Prieur, J. Chimpanzee’s and Gorillas’ Intraspecific Gestural Laterality: A Multifactioral Investigation. Ph.D. Dissertation, Université de Rennes 1, Rennes, France, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  83. Baraud, I.; Buytet, B.; Bec, P.; Blois-Heulin, C. Social Laterality and ‘Transversality’ in Two Species of Mangabeys: Influence of Rank and Implication for Hemispheric Specialization. Behav. Brain Res. 2009, 198, 449–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Fröhlich, M.; Wittig, R.M.; Pika, S. Play-Solicitation Gestures in Chimpanzees in the Wild: Flexible Adjustment to Social Circumstances and Individual Matrices. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2016, 3, 160278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Liebal, K.; Pika, S.; Tomasello, M. Social Communication in Siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus): Use of Gestures and Facial Expressions. Primates 2004, 45, 41–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Easley, S.P.; Coelho, A.M. Is Lipsmacking an Indicator of Social Status in Baboons ? Folia Primatol. 1991, 56, 190–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Baraud, I. Interactions Sociales et Modalités Sensorielles (Vocale et Visuelle) Impliquées, Chez Deux Espèces de Mangabés (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus et Lophocebus albigena albigena). Ph.D. Dissertation, Université de Rennes 1, Rennes, France, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  88. Zumpe, D.; Michael, R.P. Dominance Index: A Simple Measure of Relative Dominance Status in Primates. Am. J. Primatol. 1986, 10, 291–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  90. Hamilton, C.R.; Vermeire, B.A. Cognition, Not Handedness, Is Lateralized in Monkeys. Behav. Brain Sci. 1988, 11, 723–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Borod, J.C.; Koff, E.; Caron, H.S. Right Hemispheric Specialization for the Expression and Appreciation of Emotion: A Focus on the Face Cognitive Processing in the Right Hemisphere.–ScienceOpen. In Cognitive Processing in the Right Hemisphere; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1983; pp. 83–110. [Google Scholar]
  92. Lindell, A. Chapter 9—Lateralization of the Expression of Facial Emotion in Humans. In Progress in Brain Research; Forrester, G.S., Hopkins, W.D., Hudry, K., Lindell, A., Eds.; Cerebral Lateralization and Cognition: Evolutionary and Developmental Investigations of Behavioral Biases; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2018; Volume 238, pp. 249–270. [Google Scholar]
  93. Gainotti, G. Emotions and the Right Hemisphere: Can New Data Clarify Old Models? Neuroscientist 2019, 25, 258–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Silberman, E.K.; Weingartner, H. Hemispheric Lateralization of Functions Related to Emotion. Brain Cogn. 1986, 5, 322–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Ehrlichman, H. Hemispheric Asymmetry and Positive-Negative Affect. In Duality and Unity of the Brain: Unified Functioning and Specialisation of the Hemispheres; Wenner-Gren Center International Symposium Series; Ottoson, D., Ed.; Palgrave Macmilla: London, UK, 1987; pp. 194–206. ISBN 978-1-349-08940-6. [Google Scholar]
  96. Schwartz, G.E.; Ahern, G.L.; Brown, S.-L. Lateralized Facial Muscle Response to Positive and Negative Emotional Stimuli. Psychophysiology 1979, 16, 561–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Rohlfs, P.; Ramírez, J.M. Aggression and Brain Asymmetries: A Theoretical Review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2006, 11, 283–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  98. Harmon-Jones, E.; Harmon-Jones, C.; Price, T.F. What Is Approach Motivation? Emot. Rev. 2013, 5, 291–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Harmon-Jones, E.; Gable, P.A. On the Role of Asymmetric Frontal Cortical Activity in Approach and Withdrawal Motivation: An Updated Review of the Evidence. Psychophysiology 2018, 55, e12879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Fagot, J.; Vauclair, J. Manual Laterality in Nonhuman Primates: A Distinction between Handedness and Manual Specialization. Psychol. Bull. 1991, 109, 76–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Faurie, C.; Bonenfant, S.; Goldberg, M.; Hercberg, S.; Zins, M.; Raymond, M. Socio-Economic Status and Handedness in Two Large Cohorts of French Adults. Br. J. Psychol. 2010, 99, 533–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Ruebeck, C.S.; Harrington, J.E.; Moffitt, R. Handedness and Earnings. Laterality 2007, 12, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Sion, G. Foot-Preference Underlies Bite-Scar Asymmetry in the Gecko Ptyodactylus Guttatus. Laterality 2018, 23, 129–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Chapelain, A. Hand Preferences in Bonobos (Pan paniscus) for a Variety of Actions: Spontaneous Daily Actions (Non-Social and Social), Bimanual Coordination (Tube Task), Tool-Use (Termite Fishing) and Induced Gestures (Begging). Ph.D. Thesis, Loughborough University, Loughborough, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  105. Hopkins, W.; Cantalupo, C. Individual and Setting Differences in the Hand Preferences of Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): A Critical Analysis and Some Alternative Explanations. Laterality 2005, 10, 65–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  106. Hopkins, W.D. Comparative and Familial Analysis of Handedness in Great Apes. Psychol. Bull. 2006, 132, 538–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Percentage of gestures produced with the left or right hand, depending on the emotional valence of context. “Negative” valence corresponds to aggression and submission contexts, and “positive/neutral” corresponds to affiliation, grooming, play, and sexual contexts. GLMM Binomial: *: p < 0.05.
Figure 1. Percentage of gestures produced with the left or right hand, depending on the emotional valence of context. “Negative” valence corresponds to aggression and submission contexts, and “positive/neutral” corresponds to affiliation, grooming, play, and sexual contexts. GLMM Binomial: *: p < 0.05.
Animals 12 00186 g001
Figure 2. (a) Percentage of gestures produced with the left or right hand, depending on the receiver position in the signaler’s visual field; (b) Percentage of gestures produced with the left or right hand, depending on the signaler position in the receiver’s visual field. GLMM Binomial: ns: non-significant difference; #: 0.05 < p < 0.1; **: p < 0.01; ***: p < 0.001.
Figure 2. (a) Percentage of gestures produced with the left or right hand, depending on the receiver position in the signaler’s visual field; (b) Percentage of gestures produced with the left or right hand, depending on the signaler position in the receiver’s visual field. GLMM Binomial: ns: non-significant difference; #: 0.05 < p < 0.1; **: p < 0.01; ***: p < 0.001.
Animals 12 00186 g002
Table 1. Characteristics of the red-capped mangabeys.
Table 1. Characteristics of the red-capped mangabeys.
Social GroupIndividual’s NameSexDate of BirthAge Category
I.BellFemale31 March 2002Old adult
ChipieFemale28 June 1992Old adult
GofretteFemale8 November 1996Old adult
JolyFemale22 October 2000Old adult
ZunieFemale3 July 1987Old adult
ChipseFemale3 January 2006Middle-aged
JulieFemale8 May 2004Middle-aged
MailletteFemale29 December 2009Middle-aged
ManyFemale14 August 2008Middle-aged
TriskelleFemale21 April 2015Juvenile
KargiMale19 May 2005Middle-aged
LitchiMale20 April 2015Juvenile
PouëtMale14 March 2015Juvenile
II.KamelMale7 September 2010Middle-aged
RobyMale18 November 2010Middle-aged
III.BanditMale15 June 1991Old adult
CoëtMale31 August 2011Middle-aged
TipsMale10 July 2011Middle-aged
IV.PirateMale17 October 1992Old adult
CarillonMale2 April 2007Middle-aged
ElkyMale6 November 2009Middle-aged
GeorgeMale5 June 2006Middle-aged
V.MartiMale16 October 1998Old adult
IsbaMale20 April 2004Middle-aged
LenniMale7 October 2006Middle-aged
Table 2. Social contexts of gestures (adapted from Reference [70]).
Table 2. Social contexts of gestures (adapted from Reference [70]).
ValenceContextAssociated Behaviors
Positive or neutralAffiliationPhysical proximity between subjects (at less than one arm length), calm approach of recipient or gentle physical contact.
GroomingGrooming, tactile examination
Social playPlay-fight, rough or gentle (involving manual fighting, biting, gentle or rough touching and grabbing), or locomotor-rotational play (play with few physical contacts, but involving pursuits, jumps, somersaults)
Socio-sexualTouching, smelling, licking genital part, or mounting
NegativeAggressionPhysical aggression of the recipient by the signaler (biting, beating, rough manipulation), flight or avoidance of the signaler by the recipient, or intergroup conflict in which the signaler and recipient were in different social groups
SubmissionFlight or avoidance of recipient by signaler
Table 3. Manual gestures observed in captive red-capped mangabeys (adapted from Reference [70]). Nb: number of instances; N: number of different signalers; *: previously described as intentional gestures [69].
Table 3. Manual gestures observed in captive red-capped mangabeys (adapted from Reference [70]). Nb: number of instances; N: number of different signalers; *: previously described as intentional gestures [69].
GestureDescriptionSensory ModalityNbN
Embrace *Signaler puts one arm around the receiver’s body.Tactile137
Grab body part *Signaler closes one hand on a receiver’s body part.Tactile14417
Grabbing movement *Signaler throws one arm in the receiver’s direction, with hand closing at the end of the movement.Visual94
Pull body partSignaler holds and pulls a receiver’s body part.Tactile63
PushSignaler pushes the receiver away.Tactile11
Slap object *Signaler slaps cage element or ground with an open hand.Audible166
Slap receiver *Signaler hits a receiver’s body part with an open hand.Tactile146
Slap selfSignaler hits itself with an open hand, in a unique or repeated movement.Visual21
Touch *Signaler gently puts one open hand on receiver’s body.Tactile11
Throw arm *Signaler throws one arm in the receiver’s direction.Visual6918
Table 4. Fixed effects of the GLMM Binomial. The first factor cited for each comparison corresponds to the reference factor. Positive values of the estimate indicate that the reference factor drives higher proportion of right-hand gestures, and conversely. *: p < 0.05; **: p < 0.01; ***: p < 0.001.
Table 4. Fixed effects of the GLMM Binomial. The first factor cited for each comparison corresponds to the reference factor. Positive values of the estimate indicate that the reference factor drives higher proportion of right-hand gestures, and conversely. *: p < 0.05; **: p < 0.01; ***: p < 0.001.
Fixed EffectsComparisonEstimateStandard Errorzp
Context valenceNegative vs. Positive/neutral1.1110.4402.5260.012 *
Receiver positionSignaler’s right vs. left2.9450.5495.363<0.001 ***
Signaler’s right vs. front1.5760.4353.626<0.001 ***
Signaler’s left vs. front−1.3690.467−2.9310.003 **
Signaler positionReceiver’s right vs. left−0.8460.472−1.7920.073
Receiver’s right vs. front−1.1170.423−2.6430.008 **
Receiver’s left vs. front−0.2720.407−0.6670.505
Sensory modalityAudible vs. Tactile0.1500.6900.2170.828
Audible vs. Visual−0.5230.693−0.7540.451
Tactile vs. Visual−0.6730.383−1.7540.079
Posture“Engaged” hand vs. “free”−0.3320.440−1.1720.241
Dominance of signalerDominant vs. subordinate0.1120.3280.3420.732
Dominant vs. unclear −0.3030.582−0.5210.603
Subordinate vs. unclear−0.4150.596−0.6960.486
KinshipNon-kin vs. kin0.0290.3950.0730.941
Age differenceDifferent vs. same−0.6810.527−1.2920.196
Sex differenceDifferent vs. same0.1110.4340.2550.798
Signaler age categoryJuvenile vs. middle-aged−0.1530.366−0.4180.676
Juvenile vs. old adult−0.5800.685−0.8460.397
Middle-aged vs. old adult−0.4270.613−0.6960.486
Signaler sexFemale vs. male0.2360.4760.4960.620
Table 5. Nb: number of unimodal gestures; Prop.R: proportion of gestures produced with the right hand; HI: handedness index; p: binomial test p-values.
Table 5. Nb: number of unimodal gestures; Prop.R: proportion of gestures produced with the right hand; HI: handedness index; p: binomial test p-values.
SignallerSexAge CategoryNbProp.R.HIp
BellFemaleOld adult70.710.430.453
ChipieFemaleOld adult2---
GofretteFemaleOld adult70.710.430.453
JolyFemaleOld adult51.00--
ZunieFemaleOld adult130.690.380.267
ChipseFemaleMiddle-aged40.25--
JulieFemaleMiddle-aged110.45−0.091
MailletteFemaleMiddle-aged1---
ManyFemaleMiddle-aged120.33−0.330.388
TriskelleFemaleJuvenile80.630.250.727
PirateMaleOld adult40.50--
CoëtMaleMiddle-aged170.590.180.629
IsbaMaleMiddle-aged50.60--
KamelMaleMiddle-aged2---
KargiMaleMiddle-aged210.570.140.664
LenniMaleMiddle-aged41.00--
RobyMaleMiddle-aged190.630.260.359
TipsMaleMiddle-aged2---
LitchiMaleJuvenile610.44−0.110.443
PouetMaleJuvenile700.47−0.060.720
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Aychet, J.; Monchy, N.; Blois-Heulin, C.; Lemasson, A. Context-Dependent Gestural Laterality: A Multifactorial Analysis in Captive Red-Capped Mangabeys. Animals 2022, 12, 186. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12020186

AMA Style

Aychet J, Monchy N, Blois-Heulin C, Lemasson A. Context-Dependent Gestural Laterality: A Multifactorial Analysis in Captive Red-Capped Mangabeys. Animals. 2022; 12(2):186. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12020186

Chicago/Turabian Style

Aychet, Juliette, Noémie Monchy, Catherine Blois-Heulin, and Alban Lemasson. 2022. "Context-Dependent Gestural Laterality: A Multifactorial Analysis in Captive Red-Capped Mangabeys" Animals 12, no. 2: 186. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12020186

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop