Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Robot. AI, 13 October 2021
Sec. Humanoid Robotics
Volume 8 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2021.755150

Implementation and Evaluation of a Grip Behavior Model to Express Emotions for an Android Robot

  • 1Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International, Kyoto, Japan
  • 2Graduate School of Engineering Science, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan
  • 3Guardian Robot Project, RIKEN, Kyoto, Japan

In this study, we implemented a model with which a robot expressed such complex emotions as heartwarming (e.g., happy and sad) or horror (fear and surprise) by its touches and experimentally investigated the effectiveness of the modeled touch behaviors. Robots that can express emotions through touching behaviors increase their interaction capabilities with humans. Although past studies achieved ways to express emotions through a robot’s touch, such studies focused on expressing such basic emotions as happiness and sadness and downplayed these complex emotions. Such studies only proposed a model that expresses these emotions by touch behaviors without evaluations. Therefore, we conducted the experiment to evaluate the model with participants. In the experiment, they evaluated the perceived emotions and empathies from a robot’s touch while they watched a video stimulus with the robot. Our results showed that the touch timing before the climax received higher evaluations than touch timing after for both the scary and heartwarming videos.

1 Introduction

For social robots that interact with people, emotional expression is becoming necessary to be accepted by people. For this purpose, robotics researchers developed various kinds of robots that can express emotions using facial expressions (Hashimoto et al., 2006; Glas et al., 2016; Cameron et al., 2018; Ghazali et al., 2018), full-body gestures (Venture and Kulić, 2019; Yagi et al., 2020), and voice (Lim et al., 2012; Crumpton and Bethel, 2016). Affective touch is also an essential part of expressing emotions for human beings (Lee and Guerrero, 2001; Hertenstein et al., 2009; Field, 2010). Due to the advancement of touch interaction-related research works, robots have also acquired the ability to express emotions through touch interactions. For example, a past study investigated how participants touch a robot when they express emotions to elucidate the relationships between the touched parts of the robot and the emotions expressed by people (Alenljung et al., 2018). Other past studies investigated the relationships between facial expressions and touch characteristics to express a robot’s emotions and intimacy with their interacting partners (Zheng et al., 2019a; Zheng et al., 2019b). Another study investigated the effects of the warmness of a robot’s touch toward creating social warmth (Willemse et al., 2018), and the effects of robot-initiated touch behaviors were also investigated in the context of affective and behavioral responses (Willemse et al., 2017). These studies showed the importance and usefulness of affective touch interactions for social robots to convey their emotions.

However, the above studies mainly focused on expressing relatively simple emotions, such as happiness and sadness, which are defined as basic emotions (Ekman and Friesen, 1971). Touch characteristics, such as type and place, are essential to express such basic emotions (Zheng et al., 2019a; Zheng et al., 2019b). Although for expressing complex emotions (i.e., heartwarming and horror feelings that combine multiple simple emotions), we need to consider such time-dependent features as touch timing and longer durations. For example, past studies reported that a heartwarming emotion lasts relatively longer than a negative emotion after the former is evoked (Tokaji, 2003; Takada and Yuwaka, 2020). Another past study focused on modeling appropriate touch timing and duration to express such complex emotions, but they did not evaluate our models (Zheng et al., 2020). In other words, there is room to investigate the effectiveness of the affective touches of social robots with complex emotions.

Based on our previous study (Zheng et al., 2020), this new study implements and evaluates a touch behavior model that expresses heartwarming and horror emotions with a robot, i.e., this paper is a follow-up study of Zheng et al., 2020. We used an android named ERICA (Glas et al., 2016) to implement our developed model. We also conducted an experiment with human participants to evaluate its effectiveness in the context of expressing both heartwarming and horror emotions by the robot.

2 Materials and Methods

This study used similar materials and methods from our past study (Zheng et al., 2020) that modeled touch behaviors to express heartwarming and horror emotions. The participants identified appropriate touch (grip) timing and durations using a robot in the data collection of our previous work. Thus, the participants adjusted the timing and duration of the robot’s touch behavior by themselves to reproduce a natural feeling while watching heartwarming and horror video stimuli together (Figure 1). We note that such video-induction settings are effective in arousing specific emotions (Lithari et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2014). In other words, the participants designed touch behaviors that provide the contextually relevant emotional feeling as a natural touch behavior. We used a fitting approach for probabilistic distribution based on the gathered data and modeled the touch behaviors to express heartwarming and horror emotions.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Participant and ERICA watch a video.

2.1 Target Emotions and Video Stimulus

We focused on heartwarming and horror emotions as target emotions that our past study also targeted (see II.A of Zheng et al., 2020). We focused on positive emotion because past related studies also focused on positive emotion expressions to build a positive relationship with interacting people (; Ekman, 1993; Fong et al., 2003; Kanda et al., 2007; Weining and Qianhua, 2008; Bickmore et al., 2010; Kanda et al., 2010; Leite et al., 2013a; Leite et al., 2013b; Tielman et al., 2014; Wang and Ji, 2015; Rossi et al., 2017; Cameron et al., 2018) . To investigate whether our implementation approach is useful to reproduce a different kind of emotion, we focused on a negative emotion as a counterpart to the target (positive) emotion. These emotions consist of multiple basic emotions; past studies with Japanese participants reported that Japanese people feel happy and sad under deeply heartwarming situations (Tokaji, 2003). Horror is an intense feeling that combines fear and surprise or disgust. Note that our study experimented with Japanese participants and evaluated these two emotions as target feelings with them.

We also used video stimuli to provoke these emotions based on our past study (Zheng et al., 2020), which used six video clips (three each for heartwarming and horror) from YouTube1 to gather data about touch characteristics. Although the settings of our experiment are different from Zheng et al. (2020), we believe that using all identical video stimuli would be unsatisfactory for investigating the generality of our implemented models. Therefore, we mixed a part of the original videos with new video stimuli for our evaluation in this study and used eight commercially available videos from YouTube. Four videos (two heartwarming/horror), which are identical materials from the original study, and four new videos (two heartwarming and two horror videos) for this study.2 Thus, we used eight videos in total.

2.2 Robot Hardware

We used an android with a feminine appearance, ERICA (Glas et al., 2016), which was also used in our past study. All of her hardware configurations are identical as in the past study (Section II.B of Zheng et al., 2020). The frequency of her motor control system for all the actuators was 50 ms. ERICA wore gloves to avoid mismatched feelings between appearance and touch. Her skin is a silicone-based design even though its appearance is human-like, whose touch feeling is quite different from human’s hand. The difference between the actual feeling and the feeling evoked by the appearance may cause strong discomfort. In order to reduce this effect, we put on gloves to avoid any discomfort.

2.3 Implementation of Touch Behavior

Our experiment follows the same setting to design the touch behaviors as in the past study (II.C of Zheng et al., 2020). The robot grips the participant’s right hand with its left hand by closing her five fingers while they are watching video stimuli together. We re-implemented our proposed model because the original paper simply implemented it in the robot and only checked its timings and durations.

The implementation process requires four kinds of parameters: 1) the most appropriate climax timing of that video, tclimax; 2) the timing at which the robot should start its grip as a reaction (or anticipation) to the climax, ttouch; 3) the grip’s duration ∆t; and 4) ∆tstart (i.e., tclimax—ttouch), which is the difference between the touch and climax to extract the timing features (Figure 2). The reason for using grip behaviors is that a past study succeeded in conveying emotions via a grip behavior (Cabibihan and Chauhan, 2017).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Illustration of tclimax, ttouch, ∆t, and ∆tstart.

Our past study concluded that using a normal-inversed Gaussian (NIG) function is a better approach to model tstart and ∆t compared to other functions (e.g., beta, normal, triangle, log-normal, and exponential). Therefore, we used the NIG function in this study, and it showed the best fitting results for implementation. Figure 3 shows the histograms and the fitting results with the NIG functions, which used the defined parameters by Zheng et al. (2020) [the detailed information about the parameters are written in Zheng et al. (2020)].

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Histograms of grip timing and duration and fitting results with functions. (A) Heartwarming videos (timing). (B) Horror videos (timing). (C) Heartwarming videos (duration). (D) Horror videos (duration).

One concern from the implementation perspective is the relatively large standard deviations of the models, which we did not previously discuss (Zheng et al., 2020). In extreme cases, 24.53–32.99 and 55.37–37.85 are the possible ∆tstart ranges of the heartwarming and horror NIG models. Directly sampling from these ranges might fail to reproduce typical touch timings based on gathered data, i.e., reacting before the horror climax or after the heartwarming climax. We mitigated this problem by limiting the sampling range within one standard deviation (dotted lines in Figure 3). If a sampled value falls outside the range, we sample it again until the value is within one standard deviation from the mean. Although this approach is rather ad-hoc, it effectively reproduced typical touch parameters based on gathered data. Based on this implementation policy, the possible ∆tstart ranges of the heartwarming and horror NIG models are −4.37–17.42 and −11.55–4.13 s. We note that tclimax is pre-defined for each video based on the data collection results; it is used for calculating the ttouch .= tclimax + ∆tstart.

For selecting touch duration ∆t, we re-analyzed the touch data within one standard deviation sampling range due to the above implication policies. First, 11 of 11 touches in the heartwarming videos and 49 of 59 touches in the horror videos started before the climax timing and ended after it, i.e., a similar touch characteristic from the original data. We again conducted a binominal test and found significant differences between them (heartwarming: p < 0.001, horror: p < 0.001). However, using a sampling method might fail to reproduce typical touch parameters. The majority of touches started before the climax timing and ended after it. But due to the likelihood of exceedingly small touch durations, for example, a sampled minus −3 s ∆tstart and a sampled 1-s ∆t mean the touch will end 2 s before the climax. In fact, even though we limited the sampling ranges, ∆t’s sampling ranges are still relatively wide to reliably reproduce the typical touch parameters. To reproduce them, we simply used the mean values instead of the sampling approach, i.e., ∆t is 8.525 s for the heartwarming videos and 12.857 s for the horror videos. Based on this calculation, the system calculates the end timing of robot’s grip (ttouch + ∆t).

3 Experiment

3.1 Hypotheses and Predictions

Past studies investigated what kinds of touch behaviors effectively conveyed emotions in human-robot touch interaction and provided rich knowledge that contributed to the design of robot behaviors (Lee and Guerrero, 2001; Hertenstein et al., 2009; Field, 2010; Zheng et al., 2019a; Zheng et al., 2019b). Unfortunately, appropriate touch timings and their durations to convey emotions have received insufficient focus.

To identify appropriate touch timings and durations in the context of conveying emotions, we previously implemented touch timing and duration models based on the data collection of human-robot touch-interaction settings (Zheng et al., 2020). According to our proposed model, touching before the climax with a relatively long duration is suitable for expressing horror, and touching after a climax with a relatively short duration is suitable for heartwarming emotion. If the modeling is appropriate, a robot’s touch that follows the model will be perceived as more natural than disregarding the model.

Moreover, at the data collection in the past study (Zheng et al., 2020), the robot’s grip behaviors are designed by considering not only conveying emotions but also showing empathy following the instructions. Another past study reported that touch parameters such as length and frequency have influenced of effects of empathic touch between humans and robots. Therefore, if the modeling is appropriate, the participants will feel that they and the robot empathize with each other (Bickmore et al., 2010). Based on these hypotheses, we made the following three predictions:

Prediction 1 If the robot touches the participant using the heartwarming NIG model when it is watching heartwarming videos with the participants, its touch will be perceived as more natural than a robot that uses the horror NIG model.

Prediction 2 If the robot touches using the horror NIG model when it is watching horror videos with the participants, its touch will be perceived as more natural than a robot that uses the heartwarming NIG model.

Prediction 3 If the robot touches with a NIG model for videos in the same category, the participants will feel that they and the robot empathized with each other.

3.2 Participants

We recruited 16 people (eight females and eight males) whose ages ranged from 21 to 48 and averaged 34. They had diverse backgrounds, and none joined our previous data collection (Zheng et al., 2020).

3.3 Conditions

Our experiment had a within-participant design. Each participant experienced the four conditions under two factors described below (category factor: heartwarming video and horror video and model factor: heartwarming NIG and horror NIG). The category factor is related to video stimuli, and the model factor is related to the robot’s behaviors. For example, in the combination of heartwarming video (the category factor) and heartwarming NIG (the model factor), the participant will watch heartwarming videos, and the robot will grip the participant based on the heartwarming NIG function.

3.3.1 Category Factor

This factor has two video conditions: heartwarming and horror. In the heartwarming video condition, the participants and the robot watched heartwarming videos together. In the horror video condition, they watched horror videos together. As described in Section 2.1, we downloaded eight videos from YouTube. Four videos (two heartwarming/horror) are identical materials from our previous data collection. We selected four new videos (two heartwarming/horror videos) for this experiment.

3.3.2 Model Factor

This factor also has two NIG conditions: heartwarming and horror. In the heartwarming NIG condition, the robot samples from one standard deviation range of the heartwarming NIG, and in the horror NIG condition, it samples from the horror NIG, described in Section 2, to determine the touch-timing characteristics.

Since the robot needs to know the tclimax for each video, we conducted a preliminary survey for these eight videos to control the touch timing in both conditions. Fifteen participants from our institutions (without any knowledge of our study), whose ages ranged from 24 to 35 and averaged 26, provided their perceived climax timing of each video. We used the average of the largest clusters of the histograms of the climax timing as tclimax for each video. We edited the videos to only have one typical climax timing (i.e., extracted tclimax for the robot) and at least another 30 s after the climax to the end of the video to leave enough time to finish the robot’s touch.

3.4 Measurements

To compare and investigate the perceived naturalness of ERICA’s touch behaviors, the participants compared two aspects in the first questionnaire: Q1) naturalness of touch (degree of naturalness of touch behavior to express emotion) and Q2) naturalness of touch timing (degree of naturalness of touch timing). Participants answered this questionnaire for each video.

We also asked the participants about their perceived empathy with ERICA from two aspects in the second questionnaire: Q3) perceived empathy to ERICA (degree of perceived empathy to ERICA) and Q4) perceived empathy from ERICA (degree of perceived empathy of ERICA to you). Participants answered this questionnaire after each condition (i.e., once for each condition). We used a response format on a seven-point scale for these questionnaires, i.e., describing the options ranging from most negative to most positive.

In the second questionnaire as a manipulation check, we asked the participants about their perceived emotions from the robot’s touch. The only emotional signal from the robot is her grip behavior; she said nothing throughout the entire experiment and maintained a neutral facial expression. Although we expected the perceived emotions to reflect the category factor, confirming them is important. We asked the participants to select the top two perceived emotions (Q5/Q6) from Ekman’s basic six emotions (Ekman, 1993). The participants selected one emotion from six candidates by using radio buttons.

3.5 Procedure

Before the experiment, the participants were given a brief description of its purpose and procedure. Our institution’s ethics committee approved this research for studies involving human participants (20-501-4). Written, informed consent was obtained from them.

First, we explained that they would be watching a series of heartwarming/horror videos with ERICA, who would continue to touch their hand during the process. Sometimes she would grip it to convey emotion. The participants sat on ERICA’s left. To reproduce identical touch behaviors for all the participants, they put their right hands on specific table markers to guarantee that they all experienced identical interactions during the identical touch behaviors, and we asked the participants to use the mouse with their left hand to start the video stimuli. To avoid discomfort feeling due to constraining their hand position, we adjusted the height of the chair.

After the experiment started, the participants used a user interface to play the videos. Each video played independently. Before starting each video, the robot calculates ttouch and ∆t based on the tclimax of each video by using the NIG function. The used parameters are different due to the condition; the robot uses the parameters for the heartwarming model under the heartwarming NIG condition or the horror model under the horror NIG condition. Because we used a probabilistic distribution approach, the grip timing and duration are different between the participants even though they watched the same video. When a video started, ERICA put her hand on a participant’s hand and gripped at a selected moment that lasted a certain duration using the mechanism described in Section 2. After each video, ERICA resumed her default pose (i.e., ERICA’s hand would leave the participant’s hands), and the UI showed Q1 (“Do you feel ERICA’s touch naturally conveys her emotions?”) and Q2 (“Do you feel ERICA’s touch timing is natural?”) and radio buttons with texts (1: most negative, 7: most positive) to evaluate them. When the video was the final stimulus (i.e., the fourth video) for each condition, the UI showed the second questionnaire from Q3(“Did you feel sympathy to ERICA?”), Q4 (“Did you feel whether ERICA feels sympathy to you?”) and the same radio buttons with the texts to evaluate them. In addition, the UI showed Q5 (“Which emotion is the top perceived emotion from ERICA?”) and Q6 (“Which emotion is the second top perceived emotion from ERICA?”) and radio buttons with texts about the name of basic emotions.

We adopted a counterbalanced design for each factor. The order of the category factor is randomized, and all the horror or heartwarming videos were played randomly, during which ERICA drew samples from either the horror or the heartwarming NIG. Then the videos were played again as ERICA drew samples from the second NIG. These steps were repeated for the remaining videos.

4 Results

4.1 Manipulation Check

Table 1 shows the integrated number of perceived emotions from Q5/Q6. The total number for each NIG is 32 because we asked about two perceived emotions, which depended (by touching) on the category factor. The majority of the perceived emotions for the heartwarming and horror categories are happy/sad and fear/surprise, regardless of the NIG functions. For the former category, similar to a past study that investigated expressions of deeply heartwarming emotion in Japan (Tokaji, 2003), the participants selected happy and sad emotions as perceived emotions. For the latter category, participants reported typical emotions, i.e., surprise and fear about horror videos. The results showed that most participants felt happy/sad or fear/surprise toward heartwarming and horror videos as we expected.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Perceived emotion from robot’s touch.

4.2 Verification of Predictions 1 and 2

Figure 4A shows the questionnaire results of the naturalness of touch. We conducted a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for each factor on category and model. The sphericity of the analysis was not violated in this setting. We identified a significant main effect in the model factor [F(1,15) = 16.736, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.527]. We did not identify a significant main effect in the category factor [F(1,15) = 1.306, p = 0.271, partial η2 = 0.080] or in the interaction effect [F(1,15) = 1.823, p = 0.197, partial η2 = 0.108].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Questionnaire results with all videos. (A) Naturalness of touch. (B) Naturalness of touch timing.

Figure 4B shows the results of the naturalness of the touch timing. We conducted a two-way ANOVA for each factor on category and model. The sphericity of the analysis was not violated in this setting. We identified a significant main effect in the model factor (F(1,15) = 47.481, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.760). We did not identify a significant main effect in the category factor [F(1,15) = 0.148, p = 0.706, partial η2 = 0.010] or in the interaction effect [F(1,15) = 575, p = 0.021, partial η2 = 0.328].

These results show that the participants evaluated the touches higher with the horror NIG regardless of the video categories. Thus, Prediction 2 was supported, but not Prediction 1.

4.3 Verification of Prediction 3

Figure 5A shows the results of the perceived empathy to ERICA. We conducted a two-way ANOVA for each factor on category and model. The sphericity of the analysis was not violated in this setting. We identified a significant main effect in the category factor [F(1,15) = 9.765, p = 0.007, partial η2 = 0.394]. We did not identify a significant main effect in the model factor [F(1,15) = 0.256, p = 0.620, partial η2 = 0.017] or in the interaction effect [F(1,15) = 0.016, p = 0.900, partial η2 = 0.001].

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Questionnaire results about perceived empathy. (A) Perceived empathy to ERICA. (B) Perceived empathy from ERICA.

Figure 5B shows the results of the perceived empathy from ERICA. We conducted a two-way ANOVA for each factor on category and model. The sphericity of the analysis was not violated in this setting. We identified a significant main effect in the category factor [F(1,15) = 21.626, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.590[. We did not identify a significant main effect in the model factor [F(1,15) = 0.852, p = 0.371, partial η2 = 0.054] or in the interaction effect [F(1,15) = 0.028, p = 0.868, partial η2 = 0.002].

These results show that participants felt empathy with the robot when they watched horror videos, regardless of the NIG models. Thus, Prediction 3 was not supported.

5 Discussion

5.1 Additional Analysis of Model’s Validity

We applied two video stimuli in each video category and used them in the data collection experiment in our verification experiment. We did not think combining the old and new video stimuli were problematic because the participants who trained the models in the first experiment and evaluated them in the second experiment differed. Evaluating the models with only new video stimuli would provide additional evidence of effectiveness.

Figure 6A shows the questionnaire results of the naturalness of touch with only new videos. We conducted a two-way ANOVA for each factor on category and model. The sphericity of the analysis was not violated in this setting. We identified significant main effects in the model factor [F(1,15) = 13.720, p = 0.002, partial η2 = 0.478] and in the category factor [F(1,15) = 6.505, p = 0.022, partial η2 = 0.303]. We did not identify a significant main effect in the interaction effect [F(1,15) = 2.517, p = 0.133, partial η2 = 0.144].

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Questionnaire results with only new videos. (A) Naturalness of touch. (B) Naturalness of touch timing.

Figure 6B shows the questionnaire results of the naturalness of the touch timing for only new videos. We conducted a two-way ANOVA for each factor on category and model. The sphericity of the analysis was not violated in this setting. We identified a significant main effect in the model factor [F(1,15) = 30.612, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.671]. We did not identify a significant main effect in the category factor [F(1,15) = 1.086, p = 0.314, partial η2 = 0.068] or in the interaction effect [F(1,15) = 0.789, p = 0.388, partial η2 = 0.050]. These results show the models are effective for the video stimuli that were not used in the data collection. We note that the statistical analysis for only the videos in the data collection showed similar trends.

5.2 Design Implications

Unlike our hypotheses, the experiment results showed a better impression invoked by the horror NIG model where the robot and the participants watched both heartwarming and horror videos together. This result provides design implications for a robot’s touch behavior.

First, as a touch behavior implementation for a social robot, touch timing before a climax provides better impressions than touch timing after it, at least in a touch-interaction scenario where videos were the only external emotional stimuli that people intended to watch with the robot. One technical consideration is how to estimate the climax timing. Several past studies proposed methods to identify highlighted movie scenes by information processing (Weining and Qianhua, 2008; Wang and Ji, 2015). Such an approach might be useful to define climax timing for videos.

Second, our result suggests that directly using the parameters observed from human behaviors might overlook better parameters for the behavior designs of robots in emotional interaction contexts. From our study, even with an abundant number of participants for data collection, the observed touch models, i.e., heartwarming NIG, failed to reflect the people’s actual expectations in the evaluations.

Why did the observed heartwarming NIG model show disadvantages? Perhaps the setting was different between the data collection and the experiment. During the data collection, our participants watched the videos repeatedly to identify the robot’s touch-timing characteristics. They already knew the climax timing of the video. On the other hand, they had no prior knowledge about the video stimuli or their climax timing in the experiment. In such situations, perhaps the touch timing before the possible climax was interpreted more favorably because such touch timing might demonstrate a sense of empathy to people who were touched in this way. Another possibility is that the climax timing was different between the system and participants because the variance of heartwarming videos is relatively larger than the horror videos. For example, if the climax timing of the participants were earlier than the system’s timing, perceived naturalness becomes lower.

In addition, even though the estimated emotions by participants matched the video categories, we identified no significant effects for perceived empathy. The expressions of emotions only by touching might be implicit. To perceive empathy, feeling, and sharing another’s emotions are important. Therefore, such implicit expressions might be insufficient to increase the perceived empathy. As we previously discussed (Zheng et al., 2020), using different modalities to express emotions might effectively increase the perceived empathy explicitly.

Another point of view is the category of the contents for the shared experiences. Our results did not show any significant effect of our touch behavior design toward perceived empathy, although the participants perceived higher empathy when they watched the horror video stimuli than the heartwarming video stimuli. Moreover, participants felt happy/sad and fear/surprise for the heartwarming and horror video stimuli, regardless of the NIG functions, which might indicate that the types of visual stimuli have relatively stronger effects than the touch stimuli toward perceived empathy and emotions. Whether this phenomenon is common regardless of the co-viewer types (e.g., different appearances or beings) remains unknown. Investigating the relationships among perceived empathy, co-viewer’s characteristics, and the categories of co-viewing content are interesting future work.

5.3 Limitations

Since we only used a specific android robot with a female appearance, we must test different types of robots before generalizing our experimental results. In addition, our android’s hands resemble human hands and can perform gripping behaviors. For robots without such hand structure, other touch characteristics must be considered. The robot’s appearance and the participant’s ages influence the perceived emotions (Zheng et al., 2020). Moreover, the experiences of the participants, whether they are used to be touched, and the degree of their perceived emotions via visual stimulus would have influences.

We only used heartwarming and horror videos as emotional stimuli because they are typically used in human-robot interaction studies and human science literature. Investigating appropriate touch timing for different emotions is needed to convey such emotions by touching.

6 Conclusion

Affective touch is an essential part of expressing emotions for social robots. However, past studies investigated the effectiveness of using touch behaviors to express robot emotions and generally focused on expressing relatively simple emotions. Although our past study (Zheng et al., 2020) focused on modeling appropriate touch timing and duration to express such complex emotions as heartwarming (mixing happiness and sadness) and horror emotions (mixing fear, surprise, and disgust), we did not evaluate our models. Therefore, in this study, we implemented and evaluated our previous touch behavior model in experiments with human participants, which expresses heartwarming and horror emotions with a robot. We modified the normal-inversed Gaussian (NIG) distribution functions proposed in our past study and used an android with a feminine appearance for implementation.

To evaluate the developed models, we experimented with 16 participants to investigate the effectiveness of both models using heartwarming/horror videos as emotional stimuli. The horror NIG model has advantages compared to the heartwarming NIG model, regardless of the video types, i.e., people preferred a touch timing before climax for both videos. This knowledge will contribute to the emotional touch-interaction design of social robots.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the ATR Ethics Committee (20-501-4). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All the authors listed made substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved its publication.

Funding

This research work was supported in part by JST CREST Grant Number JPMJCR18A1, Japan, and JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP20K11915.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Sayuri Yamauchi for her help during the execution of our experiments.

Footnotes

1https://youtu.be/PFhQhpR5Z8M, https://youtu.be/r1gz-m5Ai_E, https://youtu.be/b2MH-yxIR4Y, https://youtu.be/4LYK0rTjlM8, https://youtu.be/dCPiAOiKSyo, https://youtu.be/gXfLl3qYy0k

2https://youtu.be/ftaXJlvn5f4, https://youtu.be/cPHLllSvKr8, https://youtu.be/hJxvt5LNnKg, https://youtu.be/7sVl_Mi9d0Q, https://youtu.be/b2MH-yxIR4Y, https://youtu.be/4LYK0rTjlM8, https://youtu.be/dCPiAOiKSyo, https://youtu.be/gXfLl3qYy0k

References

Alenljung, B., Andreasson, R., Lowe, R., Billing, E., and Lindblom, J. (2018). Conveying Emotions by Touch to the Nao Robot: A User Experience Perspective. Mti 2 (4), 82. doi:10.3390/mti2040082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bickmore, T. W., Fernando, R., Ring, L., and Schulman, D. (2010). Empathic Touch by Relational Agents. IEEE Trans. Affective Comput. 1 (1), 60–71. doi:10.1109/t-affc.2010.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabibihan, J.-J., and Chauhan, S. S. (2017). Physiological Responses to Affective Tele-Touch during Induced Emotional Stimuli. IEEE Trans. Affective Comput. 8 (1), 108–118. doi:10.1109/taffc.2015.2509985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, D., Millings, A., Fernando, S., Collins, E. C., Moore, R., Sharkey, A., et al. (2018). The Effects of Robot Facial Emotional Expressions and Gender on Child-Robot Interaction in a Field Study. Connect. Sci. 30 (4), 343–361. doi:10.1080/09540091.2018.1454889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crumpton, J., and Bethel, C. L. (2016). A Survey of Using Vocal Prosody to Convey Emotion in Robot Speech. Int. J. Soc. Robotics 8 (2), 271–285. doi:10.1007/s12369-015-0329-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (1993). Facial Expression and Emotion. Am. Psychol. 48 (4), 384–392. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.48.4.384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across Cultures in the Face and Emotion. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 17 (2), 124–129. doi:10.1037/h0030377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (2010). Touch for Socioemotional and Physical Well-Being: A Review. Develop. Rev. 30 (4), 367–383. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2011.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fong, T., Nourbakhsh, I., and Dautenhahn, D. (2003). A Survey of Socially Interactive Robots. In Rob. Auton. Syst. 42 (3-4), 143–166.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghazali, A. S., Ham, J., Barakova, E. I., and Markopoulos, P. (2018). Effects of Robot Facial Characteristics and Gender in Persuasive Human-Robot Interaction. Front. Robotics AI 5–73. doi:10.3389/frobt.2018.00073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glas, D. F., Minato, T., Ishi, C. T., Kawahara, T., and Ishiguro, H. (2016). “Erica: The Erato Intelligent Conversational Android,” in Proceeding of the 2016 25th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN), New York, NY, USA, Aug. 2016 (IEEE), 22–29.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hashimoto, T., Hiramatsu, S., Tsuji, T., and Kobayashi, H. (2006). “Development of the Face Robot SAYA for Rich Facial Expressions,” in Proceeding of the 2006 SICE-ICASE International Joint Conference, Busan, Korea (South), Oct. 2006 (IEEE), 5423–5428.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertenstein, M. J., Holmes, R., McCullough, M., and Keltner, D. (2009). The Communication of Emotion via Touch. Emotion 9 (4), 566–573. doi:10.1037/a0016108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanda, T., Sato, R., Saiwaki, N., and Ishiguro, H. (2007). A Two-Month Field trial in an Elementary School for Long-Term Human–Robot Interaction. In IEEE Trans. Robot. 23 (5), 962–971.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanda, T., Shiomi, M., Miyashita, Z., Ishiguro, H., and Hagita, A. (2010). A Communication Robot in a Shopping Mall. In IEEE Trans. Robot. 26 (5), 897–913.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. W., and Guerrero, L. K. (2001). Types of Touch in Cross-Sex Relationships between Coworkers: Perceptions of Relational and Emotional Messages, Inappropriateness, and Sexual Harassment. J. Appl. Commun. Res. 29 (3), 197–220. doi:10.1080/00909880128110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leite, I., Martinho, C., and Paiva, A. (2013a). Social Robots for Long–Term Interaction: A Survey. In Int. J. Soc. Robot. 5 (2), 308–12.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leite, I., Pereira, A., Mascarenhas, S., Martinho, C., Prada, R., and Paiva, A. (2013b). The Influence of Empathy in Human–Robot Relations. Int. J. Hum. Comput. 7 (3), 250–260.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, A., Ogata, T., and Okuno, H. G. (2012). Towards Expressive Musical Robots: a Cross-Modal Framework for Emotional Gesture, Voice and Music. J. Audio Speech Music Proc. 2012 (2012), 3. doi:10.1186/1687-4722-2012-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lithari, C., Frantzidis, C. A., Papadelis, C., Vivas, A. B., Klados, M. A., Kourtidou-Papadeli, C., et al. (2010). Are Females More Responsive to Emotional Stimuli? A Neurophysiological Study across Arousal and Valence Dimensions. Brain Topogr 23 (1), 27–40. doi:10.1007/s10548-009-0130-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, S., Ferland, F., and Tapus, A. (2017). User Profiling and Behavioral Adaptation for HRI: A Survey. In Pattern Recognition Letters 99), 3–12.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takada, T., and Yuwaka, S. (2020). Persistence of Emotions Experimentally Elicited by Watching Films. Bull. Tokai Gakuen Univ. 25, 31–41.

Google Scholar

Tielman, M., Neerincx, M., Meyer, J. -J., and Looije, R. (2014). Adaptive Emotional Expression in Robot-Child Interaction. In Proceedings of the 2014 ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, 407–414.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tokaji, A. (2003). Research for D Eterminant Factors and Features of Emotional Responses of "kandoh" (The State of Being Emotionally Moved). Jpn. Psychol. Res. 45 (4), 235–249. doi:10.1111/1468-5884.00226

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venture, G., and Kulić, D. (2019). Robot Expressive Motions. J. Hum.-Robot Interact. 8 (4), 1–17. doi:10.1145/3344286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, S., and Ji, Q. (2015). Video Affective Content Analysis: A Survey of State-Of-The-Art Methods. IEEE Trans. Affective Comput. 6 (4), 410–430. doi:10.1109/taffc.2015.2432791

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X.-W., Nie, D., and Lu, B.-L. (2014). Emotional State Classification from EEG Data Using Machine Learning Approach. Neurocomputing 129, 94–106. doi:10.1016/j.neucom.2013.06.046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weining, W., and Qianhua, H. (2008). “A Survey on Emotional Semantic Image Retrieval,” in Proceeding of the 2008 15th IEEE International Conference on Image Processing, San Diego, CA, USA, Oct. 2008 (IEEE), 117–120.

Google Scholar

Willemse, C. J. A. M., Heylen, D. K. J., and van Erp, J. B. F. (2018). Communication via Warm Haptic Interfaces Does Not Increase Social Warmth. J. Multimodal User Inter. 12 (4), 329–344. doi:10.1007/s12193-018-0276-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willemse, C. J. A. M., Toet, A., and van Erp, J. B. F. (2017). Affective and Behavioral Responses to Robot-Initiated Social Touch: Toward Understanding the Opportunities and Limitations of Physical Contact in Human–Robot Interaction. Front. ICT 4–12. doi:10.3389/fict.2017.00012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yagi, S., Ise, N., Yu, S., Nakata, Y., Nakamura, Y., and Ishiguro, H. (2020). “Perception of Emotional Gait-like Motion of Mobile Humanoid Robot Using Vertical Oscillation,” in Proceeding of the Companion of the 2020 ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Cambridge, United Kingdom, 23 March 2020 (IEEE), 529–531. doi:10.1145/3371382.3378319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, X., Shiomi, M., Minato, T., and Ishiguro, H. (2019). How Can Robot Make People Feel Intimacy through Touch. J. Robotics Mechatronics 32 (1), 51–58. (to appeear), doi:10.20965/jrm.2020.p0051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, X., Shiomi, M., Minato, T., and Ishiguro, H. (2020). Modeling the Timing and Duration of Grip Behavior to Express Emotions for a Social Robot. IEEE Robotics Automation Lett. 6 (1), 159–166. doi:10.1109/lra.2020.3036372

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, X., Shiomi, M., Minato, T., and Ishiguro, H. (2019). What Kinds of Robot's Touch Will Match Expressed Emotions. IEEE Robotics Automation Lett. 5 (1), 127–134. doi:10.1109/lra.2019.2947010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social touch, human-robot interaction, emotional touch, affective touch, haptics

Citation: Shiomi M, Zheng X, Minato T and Ishiguro H (2021) Implementation and Evaluation of a Grip Behavior Model to Express Emotions for an Android Robot. Front. Robot. AI 8:755150. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2021.755150

Received: 08 August 2021; Accepted: 29 September 2021;
Published: 13 October 2021.

Edited by:

Kenji Hashimoto, Meiji University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Yue Hu, University of Waterloo, Canada
Tatsuhiro Kishi, Panasonic, Japan

Copyright © 2021 Shiomi, Zheng, Minato and Ishiguro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Masahiro Shiomi, m-shiomi@atr.jp

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Download