Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 08 September 2022
Sec. Organizational Psychology

Burned-out with burnout? Insights from historical analysis

  • 1Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway
  • 2University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 3Flinders University, Adelaide, SA, Australia
  • 4City College of New York (CUNY), New York City, NY, United States

Fierce debates surround the conceptualization and measurement of job-related distress in occupational health science. The use of burnout as an index of job-related distress, though commonplace, has increasingly been called into question. In this paper, we first highlight foundational problems that undermine the burnout construct and its legacy measure, the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI). Next, we report on advances in research on job-related distress that depart from the use of the burnout construct. Tracing the genesis of the burnout construct, we observe that (a) burnout’s definition was preestablished rather than derived from a rigorous research process and (b) the MBI has little in the way of a theoretical or empirical foundation. Historical analysis suggests that the burnout construct was cobbled together from unchallenged personal impressions and anecdotal evidence before getting reified by the MBI. This state of affairs may account for many of the disconcerting problems encountered in burnout research. We close our paper by presenting the Occupational Depression Inventory (ODI), a recently developed instrument reflective of a renewed approach to job-related distress. The ODI has demonstrated robust psychometric and structural properties across countries, sexes, age groups, occupations, and languages. The instrument addresses job-related distress both dimensionally and categorically. A dimensional approach can be useful, for instance, in examining the dynamics of etiological processes and symptom development. A categorical approach can serve screening and diagnostic purposes and help clinicians and public health professionals in their decision-making. It is concluded that the ODI offers occupational health specialists a promising way forward.

Introduction

Job-related distress constitutes a major public health concern (Hassard et al., 2018; Howard et al., 2021). The phenomenon harms individuals, organizations, and society as a whole, with yearly costs in dozens of billions of US$/€ in Western countries (European Agency for Safety and Health at Work et al., 2015; Hassard et al., 2018). Over the last decades, burnout has become a commonly employed index of job-related distress (Schaufeli, 2017). Burnout has been defined as a stress-induced syndrome reflecting symptoms of exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy (Maslach et al., 2001). The Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI) embodies this three-component definition (Maslach and Jackson, 1981; Maslach et al., 1996, 2016). The MBI has been the most widely used measure of burnout and has played a decisive role in shaping burnout research (Maslach et al., 2001; Schaufeli, 2017; Schonfeld et al., 2019b).

While burnout has gained considerable popularity since the introduction of the MBI in the early 1980s (Maslach and Jackson, 1981), occupational health specialists have identified worrying shortcomings in the construct (Rotenstein et al., 2018; Schwenk and Gold, 2018; Bianchi et al., 2021a; Meier, 2022; Saul and Nikolitch, 2022). Despite their gravity, these shortcomings have largely been overlooked, and few efforts have been devoted to investigating their root causes. In this paper, we highlight foundational problems that undermine the burnout construct and report on recent advances in research on job-related distress. These recent advances may help the researcher, practitioner, and policymaker communities address job-related distress more effectively.

Foundational problems affecting the burnout construct

The importance of the MBI for the definition and legitimization of the burnout construct has often been underlined (Maslach et al., 2001; Friberg, 2009; Schaufeli et al., 2009). For instance, Schaufeli (2017) noted: “Initially, the scientific community deemed burnout a ‘pseudoscientific’ or ‘fad’ concept and denounced it as ‘pop psychology’, but this soon changed after the introduction of the MBI” (p. 108). The MBI has been used in a vast majority of the studies involving burnout (Schaufeli and Enzmann, 1998; Schaufeli et al., 2009; Schonfeld et al., 2019b). The instrument has been so influential as to inspire the ICD-11’s description of burnout among the “factors influencing health status or contact with health services” (World Health Organization, 2022).1 However, historical analysis reveals that the foundations of the MBI are not nearly as solid as suggested by the instrument’s hagiographers. The studies that led to the development of the MBI were rudimentary in their designs and analyses (e.g., uncontrolled observations having indeterminable reliability and validity), lacked methodological safeguards (e.g., to reduce the impact of investigators’ expectations and preconceived beliefs), and showed little anchorage in the literature on stress-related conditions available at the time (Friberg, 2009; Bianchi and Schonfeld, 2018; Bianchi and Sowden, 2022). The MBI was neither firmly grounded in clinical research nor based on sound theorizing (Schaufeli, 2003). As put by Schaufeli and Enzmann (1998):

“…burnout is what the MBI measures. This tautology is a serious problem since…the MBI has been developed inductively by factor-analy[z]ing a rather arbitrary set of items. What would have happened if other items had been included? Most likely, other dimensions would have appeared!” (p. 188)

Observations of Schaufeli and Enzmann (1998) have broad ramifications. They suggest that the three-component definition of burnout may be an artifact of the MBI’s problematic development process. Because the definition of burnout attached to the MBI has been the point of reference for the entire domain of burnout research, including the alternative conceptualizations and operationalizations of the entity, this state of affairs is of concern. Schaufeli and Enzmann’s (1998) observations resonate with the difficult question, raised by Friberg (2009), of whether the burnout syndrome was “invented” rather than “discovered” (p. 553).

Interestingly, the developers of the MBI themselves cast doubt on burnout’s definition when recommending that exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy be analyzed and interpreted separately due to “limited knowledge about the[ir] relationships” (Maslach et al., 1996, p. 5). Such a recommendation undercuts the idea that burnout is a syndrome—i.e., a combination of co-occurring symptoms forming a unified entity (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Crucially, the recommendation formulated by the MBI developers creates a contradiction between burnout’s conceptualization and operationalization and leaves MBI users with a double-bind dilemma. To respect burnout’s syndromal definition, investigators have to contravene the operational prescription of examining exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy separately; to respect the operational prescription of examining exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy separately, investigators have to contravene burnout’s syndromal definition. The recommendation made by the MBI developers has serious implications. If exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy are three separate entities to be treated individually, then the burnout construct loses its raison d’être (Bianchi and Schonfeld, 2021b; Bianchi and Sowden, 2022). Burnout is supposed to emerge from the combination of exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy symptoms. If the operational prescription is adhered to, then burnout is nowhere to be found. As a corollary, the MBI ceases to be a measure of burnout.

The conditions under which the MBI was developed may account for the surprising symptom scope of the measure (Schaufeli and Enzmann, 1998; Hallsten, 2005). On the one hand, the MBI disregards crucial symptoms of job-related distress, such as cognitive impairment and suicidal ideation; on the other hand, the instrument emphasizes symptoms such as “cynical attitudes,” which are less relevant to stress-induced health alterations and have ambiguous implications for job performance (Taris, 2006; Orton et al., 2012; Bianchi et al., 2019). The content of the MBI has been called into question by many investigators, including Schaufeli and De Witte (2017), who concluded that the MBI lacks “internal coherence” (p. 59). Why exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy were considered the most relevant symptoms to characterize stressed-out workers has never been clarified. It should be underlined that Maslach’s three-component definition of burnout elicited dissent early on. As an illustration, Pines, who was a collaborator of Maslach during the pre-MBI phase of burnout research (e.g., Maslach and Pines, 1977; Pines and Maslach, 1980), distanced herself from the characterization of burnout that the MBI crystallized (Pines et al., 1981; Pines and Aronson, 1988).

The longstanding inability of burnout researchers to establish (differential) diagnostic criteria for their entity of interest can be viewed as another sign that the burnout construct fails to capture a coherent and distinct phenomenon (Brisson and Bianchi, 2017; Heinemann and Heinemann, 2017; Oquendo et al., 2019; Bianchi et al., 2021a). The impossibility of diagnosing burnout has been a hindrance to case identification, prevalence estimation, treatment development, workers’ access to compensation (e.g., sick pay), and public health decision-making (Rotenstein et al., 2018; Schwenk and Gold, 2018; Bianchi et al., 2021a).2 Fascinatingly, burnout is commonly presented as dramatically prevalent despite the absence of a diagnosis. Without a diagnosis, cases of burnout cannot be identified. Because they cannot be identified, they cannot be counted. Because they cannot be counted, no prevalence estimate can be produced.

Burnout or depression

Although the nature of burnout has been debated over the years, by today, a large body of evidence has accumulated to indicate that what the pioneers of burnout research regarded as a new and unique condition is best understood as a depressive condition (Ahola et al., 2014; Wurm et al., 2016; Schonfeld et al., 2019a,b; Bianchi et al., 2021b; Rotenstein et al., 2021; Verkuilen et al., 2021). This observation is consistent with the well-established finding that depression constitutes a basic response to intractable stress—either work-related or not—in individuals with no history of depression and no noticeable genetic susceptibility to depression (Dohrenwend, 2000; Willner et al., 2013). From a neural standpoint, the stress system is critically involved in the regulation of mood, motivation, cognition, and action (Thase, 2009; Sapolsky, 2021). When adversity is chronically experienced as out of the individual’s control and no rewarding, stressor-neutralizing action is available to the individual, the sustained activation of the stress response plays a crucial role in the emergence of depressed mood, apathy, depressive cognition, and behavioral inhibition (Pryce et al., 2011; McEwen, 2012; Willner et al., 2013; Kunz, 2014; Grahek et al., 2019). In keeping with these findings, occupational health practitioners (including psychiatrists) have long argued that the burnout-depression distinction is nosologically superfluous and therapeutically unworkable (Durand-Moreau and Dewitte, 2015). The longstanding difficulties in characterizing burnout have often been attributed to the presumed “complexity” of the phenomenon. Research on the overlap of burnout with depression suggests that the burnout phenomenon may not be so much complex as ill-conceived.

The burnout-depression distinction has often been promoted based on a faulty conception of depression. The conception in question reduces depression to its clinical stage (at which a depressive disorder can be diagnosed), thereby failing to consider that research at the forefront of the field of psychopathology regards depression dimensionally, that is, as a continuum (Pickles and Angold, 2003; Haslam et al., 2012; Bianchi et al., 2021b). The view that burnout may constitute a pre-depressive condition, for instance, overlooks the dimensional aspect of depression and the fact that depressive symptoms vary in degree, from virtually absent symptoms to extremely severe symptoms (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Dimensional view of depression. The clinical stage of depression, at which a depressive disorder can potentially be diagnosed, represents only a fraction of the continuum of depression—its high end.

Attempts at distinguishing burnout from depression have also relied on the view that burnout is a social phenomenon whereas depression is an individual one (e.g., Pines and Aronson, 1988; Maslach et al., 2001; Epstein and Privitera, 2017). This view has proved to be epistemologically shaky and empirically groundless (Bianchi et al., 2017a; Bianchi and Schonfeld, 2018), in addition to imposing stigma on depressed people. Both burnout and depression can be, and have been, studied from a social and an individual standpoint. Moreover, there is evidence that burnout is, in fact, highly dependent on individual dispositions (Swider and Zimmerman, 2010; Bianchi et al., 2018; Bianchi and Janin, 2019; Rotenstein et al., 2021; Michel et al., 2022). Recent studies relying on relative weight analysis3 even found burnout to be more strongly accounted for by personality trait neuroticism than by occupational factors (e.g., Bianchi, 2018; Bianchi et al., 2021b). These findings do not support Maslach and Leiter’s (2016) narrative that “job variables and the organizational context are the prime predictors of burnout” (p. 355).

The overlap of burnout with depression has been further obscured by a misunderstanding of the nature of exhaustion, “the central quality of burnout” (Maslach et al., 2001, p. 402), as an outcome of unresolvable stress. In the context of stress research, exhaustion does not reflect a healthy state of fatigue that would merely result from a temporarily-unrecovered expense of energy. Feeling momentarily exhausted after a lot of effort (e.g., an intense workday) is a normal response. The form of exhaustion that results from unresolvable stress is of a different kind. It emerges from the experience of helplessness and entrapment that unresolvable stress involves (Seligman, 1975; Selye, 1976; Laborit, 1977; Pryce et al., 2011; Kunz, 2014). Individuals get exhausted by prolonged confrontation with stress factors that cannot be controlled and coped with. It is the experience of helplessness and entrapment in the face of adverse conditions that leads to exhaustion. Unsurprisingly, exhaustion is a diagnostic criterion for depression and a common presenting complaint among depressed patients (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

More archaeology

An examination of the very first papers of Maslach—the leading developer of the MBI—and her colleagues sheds light on what might be a root cause of the imbroglio surrounding burnout for nearly five decades. Such an examination indeed suggests that burnout’s definition was largely preestablished.

In what is known as her first paper on the topic, Maslach (1976) already offered a detailed description and turnkey explanation of burnout although no systematic research had been conducted on the entity at the time. No information was provided on the validity and reliability of the modus operandi that was followed to produce the description and explanation in question. In the same article, the author discussed variations in “burnout rates” despite the absence of established criteria for identifying “burned-out” individuals. One year later, exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy symptoms were presented as nodal characteristics of burnout without any clear evidence in support of this particular characterization (Maslach and Pines, 1977).

Importantly, no comparative investigations contrasting burnout with already-described stress-related conditions (such as depressive syndromes) can be found in these early publications (Maslach, 1976; Maslach and Pines, 1977) or in the other articles that Maslach and her colleagues published before the release of the MBI (e.g., Maslach, 1978; Pines and Maslach, 1980). It thus appears that the originality of burnout was postulated rather than demonstrated. Moreover, there is no sign that burnout was characterized with the help of stress researchers specializing in behavioral psychology, medicine, or neurobiology. A transdisciplinary dialogue may have put the so-called “discovery of burnout” into perspective and averted the emergence of illusions of novelty regarding the observed phenomenon. The link between (psychosocial) stress and depression, for instance, was already documented in the 1970s (Kollar, 1961; Lundquist, 1961; Forrest et al., 1965; Seligman, 1975; Laborit, 1977; Brown and Harris, 1978; Dohrenwend, 1979).

Historical analysis indicates that burnout’s definition did not proceed from a rigorous research process. In the light of burnout’s genesis, the publication of the MBI in 1981 can be seen as the culmination of an exercise of self-confirmation. As a standardized quantitative measure, the MBI gave burnout the patina of scientificity, allowing the construct to establish itself in the academic arena. The low quality of the research on which the MBI rested, as well as the unclear operations and arbitrary decisions that accompanied the instrument’s creation (see Schaufeli and Enzmann, 1998), went largely unnoticed. All in all, the burnout construct and the MBI may owe much of their success to collective amnesia regarding the conditions of their creation.

Addressing job-related distress differently

In response to burnout’s incapacity to serve as a dependable indicator of job-related distress, new ways to approach job-related distress have begun to emerge. The Occupational Depression Inventory (ODI) was recently developed as part of this renewal (Bianchi and Schonfeld, 2020; Schonfeld and Bianchi, 2021; Bianchi et al., 2022b).4

The ODI is designed to assess work-attributed depressive symptoms. Unlike the MBI, the instrument exhibits robust psychometric and structural properties (e.g., high factorial validity, strong reliability, measurement invariance across countries, sexes, age groups, occupations, and languages), allows for both continuum-based and diagnostic approaches to job-related distress, and is available free of charge (Bianchi and Schonfeld, 2020; Hill et al., 2021; Bianchi et al., 2022a,b).5 The ODI assesses each of the nine core symptoms of major depression (including cognitive impairment and suicidal ideation) in connection to work (e.g., “My experience at work made me feel like a failure”). Symptoms are assessed within a two-week time window. The measure can be employed based on its total score, reflecting the severity of work-attributed depressive symptoms, or with the help of an algorithm providing provisional diagnoses of occupational depression. The algorithm references diagnostic criteria for major depression found in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The ODI includes a supplementary turnover-intention item to help evaluate the implications of the reported symptoms (Bianchi and Schonfeld, 2020).

The ODI has demonstrated criterion validity in relation to a variety of variables, including work-life characteristics (e.g., interpersonal conflict at work, job incivility, unreasonable work tasks, unnecessary work tasks, work overload, social support at work, job autonomy, skill development, job recognition, job meaningfulness), general health status, and objective cognitive performance (Bianchi and Schonfeld, 2021a, 2022; Hill et al., 2021; Bianchi et al., 2022a; Schonfeld and Bianchi, 2022). An ODI-based study involving a deep-learning framework recently found occupational depression to be (a) negatively linked to companies’ stock growth and (b) positively linked to states’ economic deprivation (Sen et al., 2022). By assessing symptoms such as depressed mood, fatigue/loss of energy, and feelings of worthlessness, the ODI captures the substance of what the burnout experience purportedly entails. As previously mentioned, the ODI assesses many additional symptoms (e.g., cognitive impairment and suicidal ideation).

Researchers, practitioners, and policymakers need robust indicators to address job-related distress effectively. Robust indicators are central to both knowledge-building and action-taking. They are also important to avoid wasting resources in research settings increasingly marked by sophisticated logistics and reliance on advanced technologies, notably in neurobiological and computational sciences (Wac, 2018). Anchored in the well-established area of stress and depression research, the ODI offers a way to surmount the myriad problems affecting the burnout construct and its measures—most emblematically, the MBI (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Advantages of the Occupational Depression Inventory (ODI) over the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI).

Conclusion

Seldom examined, the genesis of burnout calls into question the very foundation of the construct. Historical analysis suggests that the burnout construct was cobbled together from unchallenged personal impressions and anecdotal evidence before getting reified by the MBI. Burnout epitomizes the problem of construct proliferation in psychological science (Le et al., 2010; Bianchi et al., 2017b; Hodson, 2021). The tendency to imprudently add constructs to the scientific marketplace requires more attention given its detrimental consequences for research and practice.

Job-related distress can dramatically affect people’s health and, in the most severe cases, result in suicide (Hassard et al., 2018; Howard et al., 2021). The approach to job-related distress reflected in the ODI promises to help occupational health specialists support personnel more effectively.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material; further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en#/http://id.who.int/icd/entity/129180281

2. ^It might be assumed that the exhaustion disorder diagnostic category recognized in Sweden constitutes a proxy for "clinical burnout." An exhaustion disorder diagnosis, however, does not require any etiological connection to work.

3. ^Relative weight analysis allows investigators to rank a set of predictors according to their importance.

4. ^https://doi.org/10.1037/t84940-000

5. ^The MBI has consistently exhibited problematic psychometric and structural properties (see Bianchi et al., 2022b).

References

Ahola, K., Hakanen, J., Perhoniemi, R., and Mutanen, P. (2014). Relationship between burnout and depressive symptoms: a study using the person-centred approach. Burn. Res. 1, 29–37. doi: 10.1016/j.burn.2014.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Bianchi, R. (2018). Burnout is more strongly linked to neuroticism than to work-contextualized factors. Psychiatry Res. 270, 901–905. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., and Janin, L. (2019). Burnout, depression and paranoid ideation: a cluster-analytic study. Occup. Med. 69, 35–38. doi: 10.1093/occmed/kqy150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Manzano-García, G., Montañés-Muro, P., Schonfeld, E. A., and Schonfeld, I. S. (2022a). Occupational depression in a Spanish-speaking sample: associations with cognitive performance and work-life characteristics. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 38, 59–74. doi: 10.5093/jwop2022a5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Manzano-García, G., and Rolland, J.-P. (2021a). Is burnout primarily linked to work-situated factors? A relative weight analytic study. Front. Psychol. 11:623912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.623912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Rolland, J.-P., and Salgado, J. F. (2018). Burnout, depression, and borderline personality: a 1,163-participant study. Front. Psychol. 8:2336. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., and Schonfeld, I. S. (2018). Burnout-depression overlap: Nomological network examination and factor-analytic approach. Scand. J. Psychol. 59, 532–539. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., and Schonfeld, I. S. (2020). The Occupational Depression Inventory: A new tool for clinicians and epidemiologists. J. Psychosom. Res. 138:110249. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2020.110249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., and Schonfeld, I. S. (2021a). Occupational depression, cognitive performance, and task appreciation: a study based on Raven’s advanced progressive matrices. Front. Psychol. 12:695539. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.695539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., and Schonfeld, I. S. (2021b). Who needs to be “burned-out”? Time for a new approach to job-related distress. Eur. Psychiatry 64:e65. doi: 10.1192/j.eurpsy.2021.2240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., and Schonfeld, I. S. (2022). Is the occupational depression inventory predictive of cognitive performance? A focus on inhibitory control and effortful reasoning. Personal. Individ. Differ. 184:111213. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Schonfeld, I. S., and Laurent, E. (2017a). Burnout or depression: both individual and social issue. Lancet 390:230. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31606-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Schonfeld, I. S., and Laurent, E. (2017b). Vital exhaustion, burnout, and other avatars of depression. Psychosom. Med. 79, 835–836. doi: 10.1097/psy.0000000000000500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Schonfeld, I. S., and Laurent, E. (2019). Burnout: moving beyond the status quo. Int. J. Stress. Manag. 26, 36–45. doi: 10.1037/str0000088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., and Sowden, J. F. (2022). A paradigm shift from burnout to occupational depression. J. Affect. Disord. 303, 230–232. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.02.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Verkuilen, J., Schonfeld, I. S., Hakanen, J. J., Jansson-Fröjmark, M., Manzano-García, G., et al. (2021b). Is burnout a depressive condition? A 14-sample meta-analytic and bifactor analytic study. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 9, 579–597. doi: 10.1177/2167702620979597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bianchi, R., Verkuilen, J., Sowden, J. F., and Schonfeld, I. S. (2022b). Toward a new approach to job-related distress: a three-sample study of the occupational depression inventory. Stress. Health doi: 10.1002/smi.3177 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brisson, R., and Bianchi, R. (2017). Stranger things: on the upside-down world of burnout research. Acad. Psychiatry 41, 200–201. doi: 10.1007/s40596-016-0619-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, G. W., and Harris, T. (1978). Social Origins of Depression. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Dohrenwend, B. P. (1979). “Stressful life events and psychopathology: some issues of theory and method,” in Stress and Mental Disorder. eds. J. E. Barrett, E. M. Rose, and G. L. Klerman (New York, NY: Raven Press), 1–15.

Google Scholar

Dohrenwend, B. P. (2000). The role of adversity and stress in psychopathology: some evidence and its implications for theory and research. J. Health Soc. Behav. 41, 1–19. doi: 10.2307/2676357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durand-Moreau, Q., and Dewitte, J.-D. (2015). La création d’un tableau de maladie professionnelle pour le syndrome d’épuisement professionnel (burnout) n’est pas une bonne réponse pour sa prévention. Presse Med. 44, 1215–1218. doi: 10.1016/j.lpm.2015.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, R. M., and Privitera, M. R. (2017). Physician burnout is better conceptualised as depression. Lancet 389:1398. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30898-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

European Agency for Safety and Health at WorkVan den Broek, K., Hassard, J., Flemming, D., Gründler, R., Dewe, P., et al. (2015). Calculating the cost of work-related stress and psychosocial risks. The European Agency for Safety and Health at Work. Belgium: Brussels.

Google Scholar

Forrest, A. D., Fraser, R. H., and Priest, R. G. (1965). Environmental factors in depressive illness. Br. J. Psychiatry 111, 243–253. doi: 10.1192/bjp.111.472.243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friberg, T. (2009). Burnout: from popular culture to psychiatric diagnosis in Sweden. Cult. Med. Psychiatry 33, 538–558. doi: 10.1007/s11013-009-9149-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahek, I., Shenhav, A., Musslick, S., Krebs, R. M., and Koster, E. H. W. (2019). Motivation and cognitive control in depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 102, 371–381. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallsten, L. (2005). “Burnout and wornout: concepts and data from a national survey,” in Research Companion to Organizational Health Psychology. eds. A.-S. G. Antoniou and C. L. Cooper (Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Publishing), 516–536.

Google Scholar

Haslam, N., Holland, E., and Kuppens, P. (2012). Categories versus dimensions in personality and psychopathology: a quantitative review of taxometric research. Psychol. Med. 42, 903–920. doi: 10.1017/s0033291711001966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hassard, J., Teoh, K. R. H., Visockaite, G., Dewe, P., and Cox, T. (2018). The cost of work-related stress to society: a systematic review. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 23, 1–17. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinemann, L. V., and Heinemann, T. (2017). Burnout research: emergence and scientific investigation of a contested diagnosis. SAGE Open 7:215824401769715. doi: 10.1177/2158244017697154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, C., de Beer, L. T., and Bianchi, R. (2021). Validation and measurement invariance of the occupational depression inventory in South Africa. PLoS One 16:e0261271. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0261271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodson, G. (2021). Construct jangle or construct mangle? Thinking straight about (nonredundant) psychological constructs. J. Theor. Soc. Psychol. 5, 576–590. doi: 10.1002/jts5.120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, M. C., Follmer, K. B., Smith, M. B., Tucker, R. P., and Van Zandt, E. C. (2021). Work and suicide: an interdisciplinary systematic literature review. J. Organ. Behav. 43, 260–285. doi: 10.1002/job.2519

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kollar, E. J. (1961). Psychological stress: a re-evaluation. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 132, 382–396. doi: 10.1097/00005053-196105000-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunz, E. (2014). Henri Laborit and the inhibition of action. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 16, 113–117. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2014.16.1/ekunz

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laborit, H. (1977). Decoding the Human Message. London, UK: Allison & Busby.

Google Scholar

Le, H., Schmidt, F. L., Harter, J. K., and Lauver, K. J. (2010). The problem of empirical redundancy of constructs in organizational research: an empirical investigation. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 112, 112–125. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundquist, G. (1961). Somatic and mental stress as causative factors in depression. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 162, 267–271. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1961.tb08759.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslach, C. (1976). Burned-out. Hum. Behav. 5, 16–22.

Google Scholar

Maslach, C. (1978). The client role in staff burn-out. J. Soc. Issues 34, 111–124. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1978.tb00778.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslach, C., and Jackson, S. E. (1981). The measurement of experienced burnout. J. Organ. Behav. 2, 99–113. doi: 10.1002/job.4030020205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslach, C., Jackson, S. E., and Leiter, M. P. (1996). Maslach Burnout Inventory Manual. 3rd Edn. Menlo Park, CA: Mind Garden.

Google Scholar

Maslach, C., Jackson, S. E., and Leiter, M. P. (2016). Maslach Burnout Inventory Manual. 4th Edn. Menlo Park, CA: Mind Garden.

Google Scholar

Maslach, C., and Leiter, M. P. (2016). “Chapter 43—burnout,” in Stress: Concepts, Cognition, Emotion, and Behavior. ed. G. Fink (London, UK: Academic Press), 351–357.

Google Scholar

Maslach, C., and Pines, A. (1977). The burn-out syndrome in the day care setting. Child Care Q. 6, 100–113. doi: 10.1007/BF01554696

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslach, C., Schaufeli, W. B., and Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 397–422. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2012). Brain on stress: how the social environment gets under the skin. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 17180–17185. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121254109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meier, S. T. (2022). The persistent burnout/depression problem. J. Psychosom. Res. 157:110913. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2022.110913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, J. S., Shifrin, N. V., Postier, L. E., Rotch, M. A., and McGoey, K. M. (2022). A meta-analytic validation study of the Shirom–Melamed burnout measure: examining variable relationships from a job demands–resources perspective. J. Occup. Health Psychol. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000334 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oquendo, M. A., Bernstein, C. A., and Mayer, L. E. S. (2019). A key differential diagnosis for physicians-major depression or burnout? JAMA Psychiat. 76, 1111–1112. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.1332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orton, P., Orton, C., and Pereira Gray, D. (2012). Depersonalised doctors: a cross-sectional study of 564 doctors, 760 consultations and 1876 patient reports in UK general practice. BMJ Open 2:e000274. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickles, A., and Angold, A. (2003). Natural categories or fundamental dimensions: on carving nature at the joints and the rearticulation of psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 15, 529–551. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pines, A., and Aronson, E. (1988). Career Burnout: Causes and Cures. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Pines, A., Aronson, E., and Kafry, D. (1981). Burnout: From Tedium to Personal Growth. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Pines, A., and Maslach, C. (1980). Combatting staff burn-out in a day care center: a case study. Child Care Q. 9, 5–16. doi: 10.1007/BF01555032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pryce, C. R., Azzinnari, D., Spinelli, S., Seifritz, E., Tegethoff, M., and Meinlschmidt, G. (2011). Helplessness: a systematic translational review of theory and evidence for its relevance to understanding and treating depression. Pharmacol. Ther. 132, 242–267. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2011.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotenstein, L. S., Torre, M., Ramos, M. A., Rosales, R. C., Guille, C., Sen, S., et al. (2018). Prevalence of burnout among physicians: a systematic review. JAMA 320, 1131–1150. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.12777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotenstein, L. S., Zhao, Z., Mata, D. A., Guille, C., and Sen, S. (2021). Substantial overlap between factors predicting symptoms of depression and burnout among medical interns. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 36, 240–242. doi: 10.1007/s11606-020-05664-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapolsky, R. M. (2021). Glucocorticoids, the evolution of the stress-response, and the primate predicament. Neurobiol. Stress 14:100320. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2021.100320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saul, D., and Nikolitch, K. (2022). The syndrome of burnout: a new spin or reinventing the wheel? J. Psychosom. Res. 157:110914. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2022.110914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B. (2003). Past performance and future perspectives of burnout research. SA J. Ind. Psychol. 29, 1–15. doi: 10.10520/EJC88982

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B. (2017). “Burnout: a short socio-cultural history,” in Burnout, Fatigue, Exhaustion: An Interdisciplinary Perspective on a Modern Affliction. eds. S. Neckel, A. K. Schaffner, and G. Wagner (Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave Macmillan), 105–127.

Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W., and De Witte, H. (2017). Outlook work engagement: real and redundant! [editorial]. Burn. Res. 5, 58–60. doi: 10.1016/j.burn.2017.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., and Enzmann, D. (1998). The Burnout Companion to Study and Practice: A Critical Analysis. London, UK: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., Leiter, M. P., and Maslach, C. (2009). Burnout: 35 years of research and practice. Career Dev. Int. 14, 204–220. doi: 10.1108/13620430910966406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schonfeld, I. S., and Bianchi, R. (2021). From burnout to occupational depression: recent developments in research on job-related distress and occupational health. Front. Public Health 9:796401. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.796401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schonfeld, I. S., and Bianchi, R. (2022). Distress in the workplace: characterizing the relationship of burnout measures to the occupational depression inventory. Int. J. Stress. Manag. doi: 10.1037/str0000261 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schonfeld, I. S., Verkuilen, J., and Bianchi, R. (2019a). An exploratory structural equation modeling bi-factor analytic approach to uncovering what burnout, depression, and anxiety scales measure. Psychol. Assess. 31, 1073–1079. doi: 10.1037/pas0000721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schonfeld, I. S., Verkuilen, J., and Bianchi, R. (2019b). Inquiry into the correlation between burnout and depression. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 24, 603–616. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwenk, T. L., and Gold, K. J. (2018). Physician burnout—a serious symptom, but of what? JAMA 320, 1109–1110. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.11703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On Depression, Development, and Death. W. H. Freeman.

Google Scholar

Selye, H. (1976). Stress in Health and Disease. Oxford, UK: Butterworth-Heinemann.

Google Scholar

Sen, I., Quercia, D., Constantinides, M., Montecchi, M., Capra, L., Scepanovic, S., et al. (2022). Depression at work: exploring depression in major US companies from online reviews. Proc. ACM Hum. Comp. Interact. 6:438. doi: 10.1145/3555539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swider, B. W., and Zimmerman, R. D. (2010). Born to burnout: A meta-analytic path model of personality, job burnout, and work outcomes . J. Vocat. Behav. 76, 487–506. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2010.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taris, T. W. (2006). Is there a relationship between burnout and objective performance? A critical review of 16 studies. Work Stress. 20, 316–334. doi: 10.1080/02678370601065893

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thase, M. E. (2009). “Neurobiological aspects of depression,” in Handbook of Depression. 2nd Edn. eds. I. H. Gotlib and C. L. Hammen (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 187–217.

Google Scholar

Verkuilen, J., Bianchi, R., Schonfeld, I. S., and Laurent, E. (2021). Burnout–depression overlap: exploratory structural equation modeling bifactor analysis and network analysis. Assessment 28, 1583–1600. doi: 10.1177/1073191120911095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wac, K. (2018). “From quantified self to quality of life,” in Digital Health. Health informatics. eds. H. Rivas and K. Wac (Cham: Springer), 83–108.

Google Scholar

Willner, P., Scheel-Krüger, J., and Belzung, C. (2013). The neurobiology of depression and antidepressant action. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2331–2371. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2022). ICD-11 for mortality and morbidity statistics. Author. Available at: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en#/http://id.who.int/icd/entity/129180281

Google Scholar

Wurm, W., Vogel, K., Holl, A., Ebner, C., Bayer, D., Mörkl, S., et al. (2016). Depression-burnout overlap in physicians. PLoS One 11:e0149913. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: burnout, occupational depression, methodology, diagnosis, construct proliferation, historical analysis

Citation: Bianchi R, Wac K, Sowden JF and Schonfeld IS (2022) Burned-out with burnout? Insights from historical analysis. Front. Psychol. 13:993208. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.993208

Received: 13 July 2022; Accepted: 22 August 2022;
Published: 08 September 2022.

Edited by:

Erich Christian Fein, University of Southern Queensland, Australia

Reviewed by:

Sasho Stoleski, Saints Cyril and Methodius University of Skopje, North Macedonia
Veronika Mészáros, Károli Gáspár University of the Reformed Church in Hungary, Hungary

Copyright © 2022 Bianchi, Wac, Sowden and Schonfeld. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Renzo Bianchi, renzo.bianchi@ntnu.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.