Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 November 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology

Teachers’ emotions in the time of COVID: Thematic analysis of interview data reveals drivers of professional agency

\r\nKaren Porter
Karen Porter*Paula Jean MilesPaula Jean MilesDavid Ian Donaldson David Ian Donaldson
  • School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, United Kingdom

Purpose: We explored two complex phenomena associated with effective education. First, teachers’ professional agency, the volitional actions they take in response to perceived opportunities, was examined to consider individual differences in its enactment. Second, “strong” emotions have been proposed as important in teaching and learning, and we wished to clarify which basic emotions might be involved, besides curiosity, which is a known emotional factor in engagement in teaching. We also explored how agency and basic emotions might be related.

Approach: Thirteen teachers working in Scottish secondary schools were interviewed at the start of the covid pandemic in 2020 to discuss relevant feelings, thoughts and actions arising from unprecedented changes in their lives and professional practices. Thematic analysis was used to identify aspects of agentic behavior and basic emotions expressed.

Findings: Teacher agency was expressed through adaptability, collective agency, constrained agency, and non-action. Four basic emotion percepts were identified, which we label as “CARE”, “CURIOSITY”, “COOPERATION”, and “CHALLENGE”.

Originality: We extend the definition of agency to include volitional non-action as a response to opportunity. In contrast to prior research emphasizing emotions as an outcome of volitional behavior, we explore emotions preceding agency. We develop four theoretical propositions related to teacher emotions. (1) Four emotion percepts substantially influence teachers’ voluntary motivated behavior. (2) The amount and proportion of emotions experienced varies between individual teachers. (3) The four percepts are experienced concurrently or in rapid succession in engaged teaching contexts. (4) Professional experience and specific situational factors also influence teachers’ behavioral choices. For future consideration, we suggest that awareness of emotion percepts may encourage both teachers’ engagement and their professional agency for the benefit of their pedagogical practice and outcomes for their students.

Introduction

The commitment that humans make to teaching is unique. Despite the prevalence of social learning behavior, few other species have been found to demonstrate teaching, changing their behavior at a cost to themselves to support a learner in acquiring new skills or knowledge (Caro and Hauser, 1992; Kline, 2015). Many of our own species, on the other hand, intentionally devote significant amounts of their limited resources to transmit cumulative cultural capital to others. Research investigating teaching has largely focused on cognitive phenomena and behavioral outcomes, notably neglecting close examination of the role of emotions in teachers’ volitional engagement in their chosen profession (Carson and Chase, 2009; Han and Yin, 2016), despite growing recognition of the role played by emotions in teaching and learning (Pekrun et al., 2007; Keller and Becker, 2021). The research reported in the current paper therefore seeks to expand on the question “what motivates us to teach?” with an assumption that there is a significant role for basic emotions in addition to cognitive and behavioral factors, recognizing that relationships between factors involved in teaching constitute complex dynamic systems. Before detailing the research methods and findings, we offer context by expanding on theories about teacher agency, and on current accounts of the role emotion plays in teaching.

Existing theories of teacher agency

Teacher agency remains poorly conceptualized and under-theorized, despite growing recognition of its value in education settings (Emirbayer and Mische, 1998; Priestley et al., 2015; Aspbury-Miyanishi, 2022). The lack of consensus is partly due to recognition that teacher agency is a complex multilevel construct, often considered in conjunction with other constructs such as identity and power (Impedovo, 2021; Sherman and Teemant, 2021). Nevertheless, we take as a starting position Sherman and Teemant’s summary of agency as “action taken not accidentally, unknowingly, unwittingly or unwillingly” (2021, p. 5). A recurring consideration of teacher’s agency has been the influence of the social context in which it is experienced, including factors such as available resources, relations with others (Impedovo, 2021), policies, norms, language, and support structures (Lasky, 2005), as well as relationships between teachers, their colleagues, pupils, and parents (Pyhältö et al., 2015).

Reasons proposed for the recent growing interest in teachers’ agency may appear contradictory (Erss, 2018). On the one hand is the perception that recent education initiatives necessitate teachers’ active engagement in collective issues requiring change, such as shaping pedagogical practices, curriculum reform, and social progress (Pyhältö et al., 2014; Biesta et al., 2015; Priestley et al., 2015). Individually, teachers are also required to be “horizon workers” (Tateo, 2021, p. 93), responding dynamically to their students’ ongoing progression and their own professional development. Indeed, teachers subject to fewer constraints imposed by rigidly defined curricula (Sang, 2020) are assumed to be acting in response to their own values and goals rather than simply delivering centrally mandated curriculum content (Scottish Executive, 2006b). On the other hand, education reforms have also made teachers accountable for delivery of practices, such as standard tests, to which they may have had no input, resulting in perceptions of being deprived of professional standing, collective power, and individual agency (Sahlberg, 2010; Tao and Gao, 2017; Benesch, 2018). Combining these two viewpoints, personal history, school contexts and the prevailing political milieu unite in complex ways to affect teachers’ experience of agency, both from a bottom-up perspective driven by the individual’s values, and from a top-down socio-cultural perspective involving a web of intricate interactions.

Compared to the sociocultural and temporal focus on teacher agency, relatively little effort has been focused on individual differences in teachers’ experience of agency. Although MacLean (2016) concurs with the notion of teacher agency as an emergent phenomenon, she proposes that it develops through a process of reflective, individualized practice. By contrast, Aspbury-Miyanishi (2022) argues that, although teachers may retrospectively ascribe actions to be the result of conscious choices, in fact experienced teachers recognize themselves as making few conscious choices. Instead, they act in the only way they consider appropriate to the circumstances in which they find themselves. From a theoretical perspective therefore, a clear gap exists in our understanding of how teachers differ in their enactment of agency.

The role of emotion in education

Emotions are increasingly recognized as integral to engagement in teaching and learning, and to teachers’ experience of agency when contentious situations arise (Benesch, 2018). Research into the role of emotions in workplaces has been limited compared to more general contexts (Hökkä et al., 2019), with education settings forming a subset of such research, and a still smaller body of work specifically focused on teachers’ rather than students’ emotions (e.g., Hannula, 2012; Frenzel, 2014; Xu, 2018; Henritius et al., 2019; Sun and Yang, 2021). Relations between teachers’ and students’ emotions have been investigated in both directions. Effects of teachers’ emotions on students include the impact on Students’ emotions and perceptions, competence, motivation, use of regulatory strategies, academic performance, discipline, and social behavior (Fried, 2010; Rodrigo-Ruiz, 2016). Conversely, teacher emotions are impacted by students’ academic performance, behavior, student-teacher relationships, students’ reported enjoyment and the perceived effectiveness of support they receive (Frenzel et al., 2007; Scott and Sutton, 2009; Beilock et al., 2010; Klassen et al., 2012; Chang, 2013). Students believe that good teachers nurture their wellbeing, despite researchers’ ongoing issues with how to measure “goodness” (Usher, 2021).

In practice, teachers’ emotions are proposed as resulting from appraisal and evaluation of multi-component relationships (Reschly and Christenson, 2012; Frenzel, 2014; Reeve and Su, 2014). Although there is recognition in existing literature of reciprocity between cognitive constructs (such as goals, perceptions, and appraisals), and teachers’ emotions (Baird et al., 1990; Frenzel, 2014), examination of relationships between these constructs has typically considered the impact of cognitions on emotion (Chang, 2013). Specifically, Control-Value Theory proposes that emotions result from appraisal of the perception of control one has, and the value assigned to specific goals and situations (Pekrun and Perry, 2014). In the specific case of teachers, Frenzel (2014) proposes a model that links emotions with antecedents such as perception of student behaviors, goals for student behaviors, and appraisals of student and teacher performance and progress toward meeting goals. The resulting achievement emotions include enjoyment, pride, enthusiasm, anger, anxiety and shame, depending on appraisal of outcomes (Frenzel, 2014; Owens and Hudson, 2021). Similarly, beliefs about self-efficacy have been related to resulting emotions and the degree of effort teachers subsequently expend (Jarrell et al., 2017; Harley et al., 2020). The focus to date on teachers’ emotional outcomes contrasts with the dearth of research on routes by which teachers’ emotions impact their beliefs and actions.

Distinguishing basic and secondary emotions

In broad terms, emotions are complex constructs incorporating neural, physiological and behavioral responses to stimuli, together with subjective responses to the experience (Celeghin et al., 2017). A contentious debate continues about whether humans experience basic emotions that are distinguishable from secondary emotions (Ortony, 2021; compare Fossati, 2012; Feldman-Barrett, 2017). Secondary emotions are distinguished by their abundance and by presumed complex processes involving cognitive appraisal of situations that gives rise to their experience (Braniecka et al., 2014; Linnenbrink-Garcia and Barger, 2014). One set of criteria for basic emotions includes the presence of distinct biological mechanisms closely associated with specific behavior, thought and memory responses, characterized by distinct physiology and physiognomy, early ontogeny and phylogeny, and rapid arousal and short duration (Ekman, 1992). Theoretical accounts disagree, among other things, on the number of basic emotions proposed, which some argue do not correlate directly with what they term “survival circuits” (Panksepp et al., 2017, p. 192). In addition to biological definitions of what constitutes a basic emotion, Celeghin et al. (2017) also offer categorical and psychological definitions of basicness. Categorical definitions incorporate sufficient criteria to allow specific examples to be confidently included in, or excluded from that category, for example “trees” incorporates oak and ash, while excluding daffodil. For present purposes, therefore, basic emotions are defined as referring to only a single emotion, as distinct from combinations of single emotions or combinations of emotion and cognition (Celeghin et al., 2017).

In education settings, teachers’ curiosity has been shown to impact relationships with students to a small but significant extent (Amorim Neto et al., 2020). For example, Izard (2007) identified curiosity as a ubiquitous quality that sustains active engagement in exploration, improved learning, information retrieval and behavior choices (see also Grossnickle, 2016; Oudeyer et al., 2016). Experiencing curiosity also allows humans to benefit from their own and others’ experiences, passed on through cultural knowledge (Silvia, 2017). Curiosity is also considered to be a fundamental emotion driving and maintaining cognitive system development (Buck, 2014). Consistent with the comments about emotion above, there is also no universally agreed definition of curiosity (Pekrun, 2019), nor is it always clear when potential synonyms such as openness, interest and seeking are being used to describe the same phenomenon (Grossnickle, 2016). Nonetheless, curiosity is considered as an adaptive emotion that motivates learning (Alcaro and Panksepp, 2011), distinct from cognitive or behavioral phenomena (Pekrun and Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2014; Ryan and Deci, 2017). Defining curiosity as a basic emotion therefore acknowledges biological processes distinct from cognitively derived states that prime autonomous seeking behavior (Ainley and Hidi, 2014). For example, a study of the association between curiosity and agency found energetic engagement with novel or ambiguous situations was associated with a willingness to wait and, rather than being given the right answer, a preference for hints that allow a correct solution to be found through one’s own effort (Metcalfe et al., 2021). Findings of a direct association between curiosity and volitional effort highlights the importance of considering how the experience of curiosity and other basic emotions is associated with teachers’ agency, complementary to research focused on cognitive appraisals giving rise to complex emotions that in turn influence autonomous behaviors (Schutz et al., 2007; Braniecka et al., 2014; Frenzel, 2014; Pekrun and Perry, 2014; Feldman-Barrett, 2017).

Motivation for the current study

As the preceding review makes clear, we recognize reciprocal causality between various constructs that interact in complex dynamic systems to give rise to teachers’ agency. Crucially, the current study diverges from the existing literature in one key way, namely in how the complex processes that we examine are related. As illustrated in Figures 1A,B, existing theories typically begin with cognitive constructs when describing routes to the experience and enactment of agency, viewing emotion as either an experience that results from behavior (Figure 1A) or as a way of influencing behavioral outcomes (Figure 1B). Whilst we would not disagree that both viewpoints provide a useful way of understanding emotion, here we offer a distinctly different perspective. Specifically, the research reported in the current paper aims to examine the role that emotions play in the generation and enactment of agentic behavior (Figure 1C), viewing emotion as a driver of cognition and behavior, rather than simply an outcome (Damasio, 2006).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Existing theories of learning recognize reciprocal interactions between multiple factors, typically emphasizing routes to action that begin with cognitive constructs (A,B). Here we propose an alternative framework emphasizing routes to action that begin with emotion (C).

In addition to exploring how curiosity might relate to the initiation of agency in teachers, we considered that other emotions may also influence teachers’ expression of agency, given earlier reports of a role for unspecified “strong” emotions in education (e.g., Danvers, 2015; Tillmanns, 2019). Indeed, we consider the lack of both empirical research and theory development about the role of emotions in teaching to be surprising given the prevalence of emotions in the classroom (Pekrun and Linnenbrink-Garcia, 2014). Consequently, we address the question of how teachers report the experience of emotions in the classroom by exploring the following hypotheses:

• Teachers report experiencing agency in the course of their professional lives.

• Teachers experience a range of emotions that influence their expression of agency during teaching, incorporating a small number of distinct emotions including, but not limited to, curiosity.

• Individual differences in the profile of teachers’ emotional experiences are reflected in the number and proportion of emotions observed.

Materials and methods

Research approach

We employed a broadly constructivist research paradigm using grounded theory. Grounded theory was selected in view of its suitability for exploring hypotheses and constructing theories, in contrast with postpositivist and positivist paradigms (Oktay, 2012). Our starting position was the hypothesis that a teacher’s curiosity relates to their experience of agency (Metcalfe et al., 2021), and our theoretical aim was to explore whether additional basic emotions or biological drives would also be involved in developing and experiencing agency.

Interviews were conducted by the lead author, a Ph.D student whose interest in psychological factors associated with teachers’ experience of, and expression of, professional agency arose from previous roles managing change projects for commercial and educational organizations, during which she had experienced the significance of emotions in participants’ responses to disruption. The lead author’s aim was to address both idiographic and nomothetic perspectives on data collected to explicate individuals’ experiences and to establish what range of emotions might operate within a population of teachers. Although experienced in a variety of data analysis techniques, the lead author was unacquainted with Nvivo software (V12, QSR International, 2020), and therefore required a period of training prior to analyzing the transcribed data.

Context

Education reform implemented in Scotland from 2010 onward has sought to achieve improvements in educational attainment through teachers’ reflective practices. The Curriculum for Excellence (CfE) aims to empower teachers to think and share ideas about their goals and values, and about their professional practice (Scottish Executive, 2006a), although movement from an “intended to an implemented curriculum” (Sinnema et al., 2020, p. 182) has been criticized as being slow. Scotland aligns with education reform internationally in shifting from the “top-down” control of curricula to prioritizing teachers’ agency, particularly their capability to design and deliver flexible curricula aligned with the needs of their school and classroom. The Curriculum for Excellence prioritizes outcomes in the form of children and young people becoming successful learners, confident individuals, responsible citizens and effective contributors rather than focusing on specific curriculum content (Hedge and MacKenzie, 2016). Critics have, however, identified incongruity between declarations of support for teacher agency, and subsequent narrow definitions of expected experiences and outcomes that promote compliance at odds with teachers’ autonomy (Priestley et al., 2015).

Following a decade of ongoing curriculum change then, participants in the current study faced specific challenges as schools abruptly closed, with teaching and learning redirected to online channels with immediate effect (Swinney, 2020). Sudden fundamental changes in teaching practices, and concern over their own and others’ wellbeing, meant unparalleled challenges to teachers’ roles as educators. Increased opportunities to exercise agency therefore arose from the need to develop innovative practices to augment or replace previously dominant approaches (Tsui et al., 2020), while the resultant upheaval was assumed to elicit a range of emotions and beliefs in relation to their own and their students’ unprecedented experiences (Narayanan and Ordynans, 2022). Teachers were uniquely placed to articulate their experiences of agency, motivation and internal emotional landscape since they are assumed to be reflective practitioners in their professional role as change agents for learners (GTCS, 2020).

Thirteen teachers were recruited using opportunistic sampling through an advertisement on professional support groups on Facebook whose members were primarily teachers in Scottish secondary schools (serving 12–18-year-olds). Secondary teachers were targeted since, as well as sharing challenges with teachers of other stages, they faced additional issues in respect of students scheduled to take crucial external exams in the near future. Potential participants contacting the lead author were provided with a participant information sheet giving details of what was required, inclusion criteria, and a consent form. All respondents subsequently agreed to participate in an interview, and none were excluded through failing to match inclusion criteria. Consent forms were either returned prior to the interview, or acknowledged and verbally agreed to at the start of the interview. All interviews were conducted between April and June 2020. The authors analyzed developing themes as each interview concluded and determined that data saturation was reached when analyses no longer introduced new themes, although rich data was still being found (Guest et al., 2016). Ethical approval for the study was granted by the School of Psychology and Neuroscience Ethics Committee, University of St. Andrews (approval code PS14861).

Data collection methods

Having confirmed consent and collected brief biographical details, the interviewer explained that the scope of the discussion included participants’ emotions, thoughts and behaviors in response to recent events, and invited the participant to begin with whichever of the three they were most comfortable. Where necessary the interviewer referenced the topic guide (Supplementary Appendix 1) to ensure all three were discussed, following a semi-structured hierarchical focused format (Bernard, 2013). An example of a prompt related to actions is “In what ways have your actions changed recently, and how have they remained the same or similar to previously?” Related to thoughts, we asked “What consideration have you given to what is possible and what is no longer possible in the changing circumstances?” A prompt related to feelings is “What is your recent experience of levels of energy and ability to focus?” All interviews were conducted using Microsoft Teams (Version 1.5.00.21668) and were recorded for analysis with participants’ consent. Interviews lasted between 35 min and 1 h 55 min, with a median length of 1 h. In all cases the interviewer and interviewees were alone in a room in their homes since they were subject to lockdown restrictions. On a few occasions there were brief interruptions from family members, specifically children of interviewees asking questions about domestic situations. Textual analysis was facilitated by the use of Nvivo software (V12, QSR International, 2020).

Units of study

Participants included a teacher of students with special education needs, as well as teachers of English, mathematics and science, history and art. Teaching experience ranged from 4 years to over 30 years, with four participants having entered teaching following careers in unrelated fields. Eight participants were female and five were male, consistent with the typical gender ratio in Scottish secondary schools (Scottish Government, 2019).

Data processing and analysis

Each interview recording was transcribed into a separate file and all identifying information was anonymized/pseudonymized as appropriate. Recordings and all subsequent files used for analysis were stored securely in line with data security and retention guidance issued by the University of St. Andrews.

Since the scope of the current study extended to at least two complex phenomena, we adopted a transdisciplinary integrative approach to defining aims, selecting the unit of analysis, and selecting research methods (Hiver et al., 2022). Our aims were twofold: to investigate whether agency and curiosity were reported by our participants, as has been previously reported, and to explore whether other emotions were reported as relevant to participants’ professional lives.

The primary unit of analysis was individual interviews, with additional analysis conducted to confirm thematic findings by comparing individuals’ responses. Individual transcription files were analyzed using six phase thematic analysis (Braun and Clarke, 2006, 2013) to identify common and repeated emergent themes. Initial codes and themes were generated using a combined deductive/inductive approach aligned with phases 1–3 of Braun and Clarke’s approach. The deductive component arose from a priori assumptions that (a) teachers would report feelings of curiosity related to opportunities arising from recent novel circumstances, and (b) teachers would report behaviors that demonstrate their experience of professional agency. Inductive analysis was driven by interest in what other psychological phenomena, particularly emotions, would emerge from the data (Fereday and Muir-Cochrane, 2006). For phase 4, the first author and two independent coders met to discuss initial findings from the first six interviews, and reached agreement on theme descriptions, numbers and codes. The first author and one coder each then separately coded a transcript. During analysis, coders recognized that the same utterance could refer to more than one emotion and agreed that multiple codes should be assigned where relevant. Assigned codes matched in 87% of instances. A similar exercise with a second transcript and the second coder resulted in 85% agreement on codes assigned, indicating a high level of consistency in interpreting participants’ meaning. The first author completed coding on the remainder of the transcripts. Subsequent meetings between coders, and between all authors further clarified emergent themes, definitions and names of themes (phase 5). The final phase is addressed in the findings reported below (Braun and Clarke, 2013). In addition to textual analysis, we reference examples where gestures added to the communication of emotion states (Hostetter, 2011). Although we recognize the limitations of a tool initially aimed at biomedicine and health services (Buus and Perron, 2020), and the risk of formulaic interpretation of qualitative data (LaMarre and Chamberlain, 2022), nevertheless the Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ) served as a valuable guideline for including relevant methodological detail in reporting findings (Tong et al., 2007, Supplementary Appendix 2).

Results

Thematic analysis resulted in the emergence of key themes related to both agency and emotions. We identified four sub-themes related to the experience of agency and four themes related to distinct emotions. An a priori assumption that teachers would report behaving with agency was supported. Examples of participants’ expressions of agency are shown in Table 1, highlighting the dominance of adaptability as a sub-theme. The first three examples of agency in Table 1 exemplify adaptability at strategic, tactical and operational levels, respectively. Heather reported previously making a strategic decision when she seized an opportunity to leave a career in financial services and retrain as a teacher; Amanda made tactical efforts to change her behavior so that she continued to perform as an effective teacher; and Sheila described a specific circumstance in which she chose an unprecedented course of action (Greer, 1991; Goi, 2018).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Examples illustrating the expression of agency for each teacher.

Besides adaptability, themes of collective agency, constrained agency, and non-action are also illustrated in Table 1. Collective agency was illustrated by participants’ choices to engage in collaborative efforts such as subdividing large tasks among colleagues and then sharing results for the greater good of their students, colleagues, and themselves (Bandura, 2016). The agency shown by Emily was distinct from other participants in that, having perceived that there were no “good” choices open to her, she expressed a desire to have had other options available (Damman and Henkens, 2017). The label of “constrained agency” in these circumstances recognizes that whereas Sherman and Teemant (2021) include willingness to act in their definition of agency, Emily explicitly described reluctance to act or unenthusiastic inaction. In contrast, George’s decision not to take a job offer was a deliberate response after considering the opportunity offered, his future goals, potential alternatives, and his emotional “gut” response. His decision informs our proposed extension to Sherman and Teemant’s definition of agency in that his inaction was not accidental, unknowing, unwitting, nor unwilling (2021). We therefore propose an addendum to include intentional inaction as a possible expression of agency.

Thematic analysis also confirmed the a priori assumption that curiosity would be identified as a motivating emotion that underpinned agentic behavior. Crucially, a posteriori themes of care, cooperation and challenge also emerged. For the purposes of disambiguation, subsequent references will be to CURIOSITY, CARE, COOPERATION, and CHALLENGE, following the convention of using block capitals to distinguish themes related to emotions as descriptions of perceptual experience, and not simply lexical entities (Montag and Panksepp, 2017). We prefer the term percept over concept since percepts incorporate multiple modalities including thinking about and experiencing bodily feelings, and exhibiting actions associated with or exemplifying emotions.

For each participant, the number of utterances related to emotion was tallied, with subtotals calculated for each percept (Table 2), demonstrating considerable variation in the number of utterances. Nine participants made most utterances related to CARE, one made most utterances about CURIOSITY, and two made the same number of utterances related to CURIOSITY and CARE. The highest proportions of utterances were Gregory’s 55% related to CARE and Emily’s 55% related to CHALLENGE. Profiles of emotions expressed by each teacher are illustrated graphically in Figure 2, highlighting differences between individuals. Notably, for all participants other than Emily, utterances related to CURIOSITY and CARE constituted over half of the total. Table 3 gives a selection of examples of utterances for each participant.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The total number of utterances made about emotion, and proportion of utterances related to each percept.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Percentages of utterances related to each of the four emotion percepts. CURIOSITY and CARE constituted between 55 and 88% of utterances for all participants except Emily.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Examples of basic emotions for each teacher.

Our identification of CURIOSITY was expected given previous claims within the wider literature, reinforcing the view that CURIOSITY plays an important role in teaching (Grossnickle, 2016; Ceha et al., 2019). For example, participants reported that changing circumstances had provided opportunities to pursue longstanding interests in practices, tools and technologies that they had previously never acted on, suggesting a direct link between CURIOSITY and acting with agency. Perhaps surprisingly given our a priori hypothesis about the important role of emotion, CURIOSITY was the dominant percept for only one participant, with considerable variability in its reported frequency (Table 2).

The most prevalent emotion reported was CARE, which was the most frequently noted percept for nine of the thirteen participants, suggesting a key role for CARE in motivating teachers’ agency. Indeed, Heather suggested this was the primary reason most teachers enter the profession. A variety of behavior was enacted in response to feelings of CARE, including presenting students with small physical tokens, some of which were practical (such as delivering jotters and pens to students’ homes), while others were symbolic (such as crocheted hearts). Other demonstrations of CARE included shifting the workday to support students working in the evenings, reassuring students of teachers’ high levels of availability, offering words of encouragement, and expressing recognition of students’ individuality and uniqueness. In addition, both Heather and Amanda made expansive embracing gestures, hugging themselves and “virtually” hugging the interviewer via their cameras, in indication of how much they missed and valued physical contact with learners.

With one exception, participants reported experiencing COOPERATION as warm feelings toward colleagues. Participants valued companionship, camaraderie and distraction provided by belonging to communities experiencing similar circumstances and with comparable goals. As well as explicit emotional support, participants recognized that sharing materials and practices also contributed to their emotional wellbeing, whether as net contributors or net beneficiaries. Another association between emotion and the experience of agency can be seen in Heather’s reporting of valuing her school’s focus on “relationships and building those relationships and giving teachers agency to do things” since “if we get the relationships right then other things will follow from there.” Here Heather directly links COOPERATION, with “doing things,” distinguishing between the emotional feeling and collective agency in action. Uniquely, the participant who reported having no professional community, either within her school or beyond, demonstrated poor emotional wellbeing. We will shortly consider the distinct circumstances reported by Emily.

All participants reported having to cope with CHALLENGE arising from their prevailing circumstances. Being unable to be physically present with students and colleagues was often experienced as distressing but was also clearly associated with agentic behavior (Sepulveda-Escobar and Morrison, 2020). Participants acknowledged emotional discomfort arising from their circumstances, then offered examples of innovative ways in which they had attempted to mitigate their difficulties through, for example, exploiting previously underutilized communication channels to make connections and share experiences with empathetic colleagues. Whilst most of our participants reported relatively few experiences of CHALLENGE despite the sudden disruption to their professional and personal lives, in one case CHALLENGE was dominant. Uniquely, 55% of Emily’s speech was focused on CHALLENGE, particularly emphasizing that she felt she was not coping, compared with others’ range of between 4 and 35%. Emily’s self-reported mental wellbeing was also notably poor, possibly as a result of the difficulties she was experiencing. She criticized herself (using “stupid” and “silly”), her students who she felt were not engaging appropriately, unsupportive colleagues, and senior management who she felt imposed unrealistic demands.

Having noted earlier how COOPERATION and agency align, here we identify another example of a link between emotion and agency. Emily reported both high levels of CHALLENGE and low levels of COOPERATION and CURIOSITY, indicating potential combinations of specific emotions, as well as links between emotions and her constrained agency. Conversely, more adaptability in agency may result from experiencing permutations of emotions that create recognizable opportunities to act in novel ways. For example, Gregory exemplified the experience of combinations of emotions when he reported CURIOSITY-driven development of expertise in creating videos that he subsequently shared online. He expressed “a lot of satisfaction that something I spent a lot of time doing for my students (CARE) is actually having a wider benefit for the teaching community (COOPERATION).”

Although presented above as distinct themes, the combination of multiple emotions and rapid switching between them was also notable, suggesting that the different percepts are typically experienced in combination or at least in rapid succession, and that emotional complexity and variation is the norm. We found support for the hypothesis that a combination of emotions influences teachers’ expression of agency and that there are individual differences in the profile of emotions teachers experience. Evidence of the fundamental nature of these differences was identifiable particularly in relation to CURIOSITY and CARE focused individuals. Quotations from two participants exemplify the distinction between the two:

“I’ve always had an enquiring mind and a desire to, to improve what I’m doing but also just enquiring about the subjects that I’m teaching. I’m curious about mathematics, and I’m curious about how to then compare the two, mathematics and peopleI want (students) to have those moments of discovery and to facilitate thatI always say to my pupils, you may never be passionate about maths but by the time you leave school I want you to be able to say, I know at least one person who is.” (Gregory).

and,

“When I started my teacher training (the lecturer) said ‘If you’re here because youcan’t wait to impart your love of Shakespeare to the young peopleyou’re here for the wrong reason. If you’re here because you like young people, because you like making connections with young people, you’re in the right place.’ And I remember thinking, thank goodness for that, I might even be able to do this after all. I think most teachers are the same. Some of them it’s very much about the love of their subject but most of us it’s just about people.” (Heather).

Each quotation indicates an important role for emotions, respectively CURIOSITY and CARE, in teachers’ professional motivation. Whilst all participants demonstrated each percept (Table 2), indicating that all four are relevant for each of our teachers, they were reported in highly variable proportions (illustrated in Figure 2), suggesting distinct individual differences in the ways that participants experience and act on emotions associated with their professional life (see Table 3 for more exemplars).

Discussion

Shortly after the covid pandemic was declared, we interviewed thirteen teachers working in Scottish secondary schools. Thematic analysis of interview transcriptions confirmed assumptions related to professional agency that teachers would take volitional action in response to prevailing exigencies. We identified four sub-themes of agency: adaptability, collective agency, constrained agency, and inaction. We found evidence in support of the assumption that teachers would report feelings of CURIOSITY in respect of their professional lives, and we identified three other emotion percepts participants reported as important to their commitment as teachers, these were CARE, COOPERATION and CHALLENGE. Further, we found that the four emotions were experienced in different proportions between individuals, and that they were frequently reported together.

Contemporary approaches to teaching reform such as Scotland’s Curriculum for Excellence emphasize the role of teachers in developing and delivering content in the context of their unique classroom situations in a climate of reflexivity and cooperation between classroom practitioners exercising their professional agency (cf. Scottish Executive, 2006a). In emphasizing flexibility and adaptability, contemporary teaching approaches aim to exploit the complexity arising from individual differences in teachers’ attributes, knowledge and experience, rather than imposing uniformity on teaching practice. To date, however, exploration of individual differences in teachers’ basic emotions and their experience and expression of agency has been limited. The current study demonstrates teachers acting with agency in response to the unprecedented circumstances unfolding at the time the interviews were conducted, and highlights the relevance of basic emotions, albeit in differing proportions, in achieving that agency.

The project to explore emotions relevant to teachers’ experience of agency was conceived before the start of the pandemic, and it is evident that the challenges arising from the unprecedented circumstances impacted the results we found. Practically, teachers had to deal with pupils who lacked digital connections required for remote learning. Socially, they were supporting those living in poverty in inadequate housing. Emotionally, they were addressing mental health difficulties in themselves and their learners (Moss et al., 2021). The prevalence of CARE toward learners in the circumstances prevailing at the time of data collection is perhaps unsurprising given teachers’ pastoral role in supporting student wellbeing and the considerable threat to mental health resulting from the experience of a pandemic. Although data about teachers’ responses to the pandemic are still scarce, a recent survey found an association between teachers’ wellbeing and the support they felt from friends, colleagues and senior management (Connor et al., 2022). This association aligns with our findings of the importance of teachers feeling CARED for and recognizing that they have opportunities for COOPERATION with others. It also aligns with the experience of collective agency, in which colleagues provide practical support in tackling CHALLENGING situations.

Implications for theory and practice

Distinct from existing research that offers cognitive rationalizations for teacher agency such as serving society, on the basis of the current findings we make a novel proposal for four emotion percepts that influence the experience and expression of agency. Our proposal is also distinct from research exploring secondary emotions arising from appraisals of teachers own performance or that of their students by its focus on basic emotions preceding such appraisals.

We have reported connections between emotions and teacher agency, both of which are recognized as complex constructs. Complex systems operate on multiple levels, across different temporal scales, and are liable to be impacted in non-linear ways by myriad factors (Overton, 2014; Wilson, 2016). A corollary of recognizing an area of study as complex is acknowledging that investigating that domain solely by identifying and measuring variables will generate little insight (Lykken, 1991; Agar, 2004; Gigerenzer, 2010). Our methodological choices recognize that phenomenological and methodological issues constrain how research is conducted within and between complex and dynamic systems, and further, that there is no single effective approach to studying complex systems. Instead, research methods that recognize subtle and complex interactions that influence teachers’ agency are preferable to attempting to establish categorical distinctions such as assuming that teachers are either CURIOUS or CARING or COOPERATIVE or CHALLENGED. Our open process of analysis proved particularly appropriate for identifying complex patterns of interaction between emotions and feelings of agency.

Paradoxically, complex systems can be described by a few simple rules governing how component entities interact to generate a wide range of behaviors at higher levels of the system (Wilson, 2016). The challenge is to accurately identify and describe elemental components, together with relevant rules under which they operate. In response to calls for psychology to mature as a discipline through greater theory development (Borghi and Fini, 2019), we therefore propose four theoretical rules that aim to articulate teachers’ experience of agency, in particular extending existing theories that omit or downplay references to emotions (Emirbayer and Mische, 1998; Priestley et al., 2015; Aspbury-Miyanishi, 2022). The first three encapsulate results and discussions already reported above: Proposition (1) four emotion percepts of CARE, CURIOSITY, COOPERATION, and CHALLENGE substantially influence teachers’ voluntary motivated behavior; Proposition (2) individual differences between teachers are reflected in the amount and proportion of emotions reported; Proposition (3) the four emotion percepts are experienced by engaged teachers concurrently or in rapid succession.

In recognition of the unique circumstances of heightened concern about students’ wellbeing, we offer Proposition (4) prior experiences and the exigencies of the current situation influence which category of action has priority in the moment. For example, although in the current situation he expressed a greater emphasis on CARE, Gregory reported an earlier episode when he was driven by CURIOSITY to gain skills in digital learning. While proposition 3 highlights concurrency of emotions, we also recognize that proportions of each percept may change over time for an individual. For instance, a similar data-gathering exercise at another time may have seen the same participants reporting more CURIOSITY and COOPERATION, and less emphasis on CARE.

As well as novel theoretical propositions, we highlight three distinct features of our approach. The first is the contrast with existing theories of teacher emotions. We examined the role that emotions play in producing behaviors, in contrast with the prevailing focus on emotions that result from appraisal of actions (Pekrun, 2006; Pekrun et al., 2007; Frenzel, 2014; Pekrun and Perry, 2014). Other research has identified “strong” emotions as relevant to educational practices (Danvers, 2015; Tillmanns, 2019), and CURIOSITY specifically has been shown to relate to outcomes (Silvia, 2008; Grossnickle, 2016). Here we have offered greater specificity about “strong” emotions, presenting evidence that teacher agency can be understood as a behavioral outcome motivated by CURIOSITY, as well as three additional emotion percepts of CARE, COOPERATION, and CHALLENGE.

The second distinct feature of our approach is the contrast with the existing body of research focused on teachers’ negative emotions, such as UK teachers reporting high levels of stress, with symptoms of poor wellbeing attributed to high workload and lack of support (Education Support, 2020). Stress leads to decreased motivation, absenteeism, poor student behavior, and lower academic achievement (Ravalier and Walsh, 2017). Particularly in Emily’s case we found evidence supportive of an association between negative emotions, wellbeing, and the experience of agency. However, an inference from the focus on negative emotions (Frenzel, 2014; Pekrun and Perry, 2014) might be that these exert more influence on teachers’ motivation than positive emotions. In contrast with Emily though, other participants did not report feeling overwhelmed by the CHALLENGE they were experiencing, but rather appeared able to balance the influence of positive and negatively valenced emotions in their experience of agency. Further effort to gain understanding of interactions between emotions of different valence is needed, particularly given findings that positive engagement is more conducive to favorable outcomes than punishment (Skinner, 1953; Prescott and Buchanan-Smith, 2003).

Limitations and opportunities

As we highlighted in the introduction, there is ongoing ideological debate about the nature of emotions, specifically whether there are “basic” emotions, their nature and number, and how they might relate to secondary emotions (Barrett et al., 2007; Chen, 2016; Panksepp et al., 2017). Researchers distinguish between innate, phylogenetically developed basic emotions (Damasio, 2006) and secondary emotions characterized by the degree of cognitive processing associated with their experience (Leyens et al., 2001; Becker-Asano and Wachsmuth, 2008). For instance, the experience of enjoyment is reported as “resulting from” historical actions, current engagement or future anticipation (Frenzel, 2014, p. 495, italics added). Given that a consensus is unlikely to emerge anytime soon between opposing viewpoints, we limit our current position to a pragmatic proposal that the four percepts at least satisfy the categorical and psychological definitions outlined by Celeghin et al. (2017), and therefore offer potentially useful distinctions (Box, 1979). Future interdisciplinary efforts to clarify definitions, distinctions, phylogenesis and necessary and sufficient components of emotions would prove valuable. Similarly, the current paper proposes no physiological mechanisms or emotional/cognitive processes linking the four percepts with agency. A starting point could be work characterizing CURIOSITY as the inherent reward experienced when acquiring knowledge, leading to a cycle of information-seeking behavior and increasing knowledge (Murayama, 2022). We are unaware of similar work linking multiple basic emotions. In order to record relevant behavioral and physiological measures of emotion in the classroom, recent developments in cognitive neuroscience (cf. the mobile cognition approach: Ladouce et al., 2017) indicate how future studies may augment understanding by include measures such as heart rate variability or mobile EEG recordings.

We present a qualitative study, which remains relatively unusual in a discipline that favors self-report measures (Dinsmore et al., 2008; Dinsmore, 2017). Of course, the nature of complex systems necessarily requires dynamism and flexibility throughout the analysis process, as well as recognition that a variety of means will be needed to guard against spurious associations, lack of reproducibility and future replication failures (Wasserstein and Lazar, 2016). As psychology transitions from assumptions that behavior is purely rational, to acknowledging that motivated behavior incorporates emotion (Hall and Knapp, 2015) opportunities arise to incorporate multiple disciplinary and conceptual foci in future efforts to understand complex psychological processes. We advocate proceeding cautiously using appropriate methods to extend understanding of the impact of emotion percepts in catalyzing effective teaching. In particular, relationships between researchers and the objects of investigation need to be explicit and subject to scrutiny, with a range of methods used to improve validity and triangulation of findings.

Although we have responded to ongoing encouragement to vary methodological approaches (e.g., Lykken, 1991; Borsboom et al., 2009; Gigerenzer, 2010), we recognize that single engagements with participants do not present the same opportunities to record change over time that longitudinal and ethnographic methods would have afforded (Agar, 2004; Dinsmore et al., 2008). While participants in the present study reported major recent changes, conducting subsequent studies over extended timeframes might enhance the validity of reports by allowing intraindividual comparisons of multiple observations.

Also related to methodological concerns, we identified instances of gesture conveying supplementary information about participants’ emotional states (Hostetter, 2011). To our surprise, reporting of such additional data is often missing from relevant literature. For example, Oplatka (2012) reports only the words of teacher interviews. Similarly, although Eidsvåg and Rosell (2021) report actions, their analysis to “make sense of what we perceived was happening” (p. 88) results in summary explanations such as reporting that a participant’s “body language is vigorous and confident” (p. 91), rather than describing specific gestures and movements. Aligned with concerns about the universality of facial expressions conveying specific emotions (Feldman-Barrett, 2017), there are concerns about the ubiquity of specific gestures communicating additional information (Hostetter, 2011) and a pressing need to develop effective coding strategies to facilitate robust interpretation of gestures. Nonetheless, we encourage the reporting of non-verbal communication and physiological measures alongside verbal responses as a way of generating richer data leading to more complete understanding.

It is also important to acknowledge that the current study was unable to determine the extent to which lack of CURIOSITY, CARE, COOPERATION, and excessive CHALLENGE each influenced constrained agency. Indeed, further studies are needed to establish whether the four percepts provide a complete account of the emotional experience associated with agency. For example, it may be that teacher agency is enhanced or compromised by the experience of specific profiles of emotional percepts. We also note that although there is widespread adoption of the idea that agency is an important feature of effective teaching practice, to date there is relatively limited empirical evidence regarding how agency operates in the classroom, whether it can be developed, and how it changes with experience. Further enquiry is clearly necessary and we particularly advocate proceeding cautiously before encouraging widespread adoption of pedagogical practices that lack strong evidential support (Newton and Salvi, 2020).

The current study involved only teachers working in the secondary stage, giving rise to obvious opportunities to examine further questions. Do teachers in the primary stage show similar sub-themes of agency? Do tertiary-stage teachers have different experiences of basic emotions? Do teachers of different subjects have distinct differences in their basic emotions? Are there different basic emotions at work for some teachers compared to those already identified in the current paper? We also recognize the need to investigate the roles of students and the wider social context in teachers’ experience of agency and the efficacy of their practices (Priestley et al., 2015). Moreover, participants in the current study volunteered their efforts at a time when teachers were experiencing unprecedented disruption. They may therefore have been unrepresentative of the profession in judging that they had capacity for additional effort, whilst others might have felt that their ongoing volume of work and personal circumstances precluded their participation. Future research should also examine effects of students’ and teachers’ emotions on student satisfaction, wellbeing and academic achievement (Collie et al., 2017). More broadly, work in classrooms could inform understanding about whether each of the four percepts is always necessary for teachers to experience agency, why some teachers feel overwhelmed by professional and personal circumstances affecting their work, and whether students and teachers necessarily experience interactions in the same way (for example, do teachers’ expressions of CARE sometimes translate into experiences of CHALLENGE for students?) Likewise, since organizational and environmental factors affect individuals’ development and enactment of agency (Hökkä et al., 2019), pressure to achieve centrally imposed targets and increase efficiency might have significant impact on teachers’ capacity to experiment with innovative practices and pedagogical resources. Since context-dependent curricula recognize teachers’ individual differences and focus on their critical thinking skills and reflectivity to promote increased self-awareness (Education Scotland, 2021), increased awareness of the role of emotions in teaching may support relevant institutions in developing appropriate curricula and monitoring learning outcomes.

Coaching focused on emotional awareness has been shown to impact the sense of personal agency experienced by managerial staff (Hökkä et al., 2019). Adopting a similar approach using longitudinal and comparative studies would provide invaluable insights into the impact of teachers’ awareness of emotion percepts on motivation, self-efficacy, engagement, behavior, and teaching outcomes (Reeve and Shin, 2020; Usher, 2021). We are optimistic about opportunities and outcomes arising from greater focus on emotions, particularly in teachers’ effective modeling of emotion percepts and associated behaviors for students in the same way that they model meta-cognitive abilities (cf. Branigan and Donaldson, 2019, 2020). Partnerships between psychology and education practitioner-researchers should also encourage more nuanced development and adoption of research findings in education settings (Immordino-Yang, 2016), particularly exploring the impact and limitations of awareness of emotion percepts on pedagogical practices through collection and analysis of rich, robust data (Newton and Salvi, 2020).

Conclusion

We examined teachers’ professional agency through enthusiastic and effortful commitment to adapting their behaviors as circumstances demanded, to support themselves, their colleagues and their students. We clarified existing conceptualizations of agency by identifying that teachers act adaptably at multiple levels, making strategic, tactical and operational choices, including choices not to act. We also identified constrained agency when choices felt limited, leading to action without wholehearted engagement, as well as agency operating within communities of teachers acting collectively. In questioning “what motivates us to teach,” we identified factors linked to basic emotions and proposed a novel theory that teachers’ agency is substantially influenced by feelings of CARE, CURIOSITY, COOPERATION, and CHALLENGE. We also proposed that the degree to which these emotions are experienced differs between individuals, that the percepts are experienced concurrently or in rapid succession, and that circumstances influence which emotions are most influential in any situation. We distinguish basic emotions that motivate engagement in teaching from achievement emotions that result from such commitment. For example, we suggest that feelings of CARE that motivate a teacher to spending additional time with students is more basic than the pride that results from students’ success at reaching goals, although the potential link between the two is evident.

At a time of major challenges, participants in the current study showed how dedication to their craft, students, and colleagues led them to volitional actions aimed at minimizing losses and exploiting opportunities for the perceived benefit of each of those populations. Those concerned with whether students reliably experience supportive teaching may be reassured by our findings of highly motivated, agentic teachers. Further careful and critical consideration of the role of emotion percepts may also help educators to fine-tune responses to their own and their students’ individual needs. Indeed, our view is that gaining a clearer understanding of the role that emotions play in the classroom will encourage learner-shaped education that promotes the development of agency, instead of attempting to mold compliant school-shaped learners. By listening to and observing teachers we can see that implicit awareness of emotion percepts already supports agency and good pedagogy. Our hope is that future studies will show that enhancing teachers’ explicit understanding of the roles of CARE, CURIOSITY, COOPERATION, and CHALLENGE in their experience of agency will result in more great practice.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the School of Psychology and Neuroscience Ethics Committee, University of St. Andrews. The participants either provided written informed consent to participate in this study or gave verbal informed consent at the start of the interview.

Author contributions

KP: conceptualization, methodology, project administration, formal analysis, investigation, resources, data curation, writing—original draft, visualization, writing—review and editing, and funding acquisition. PM: conceptualization, methodology, writing—review and editing, and supervision. DD: conceptualization, methodology, project administration, visualization, writing—review and editing, supervision, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by a studentship at the Scottish Graduate School of Social Science (award number ES/P000681/1), which was funded by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) and the Scottish Funding Council (SFC).

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Judith Lowes and Jay Burgess for their coding of interview transcripts and discussion of emerging themes. We thank the Profs. Mark Priestley and Roger Watt, and Dr. Ken Mavor for valuable discussions that informed our practical investigation and theory development.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.987690/full#supplementary-material

References

Agar, M. (2004). We have met the other and we’re all nonlinear: Ethnography as a nonlinear dynamic system: Ethnography as a nonlinear dynamic system. Complexity 10, 16–24. doi: 10.1002/cplx.20054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ainley, M., and Hidi, S. (2014). “Interest and enjoyment,” in International handbook of emotions in education, eds L. Linnenbrink-Garcia and R. Pekrun (London: Routledge), 205–227.

Google Scholar

Alcaro, A., and Panksepp, J. (2011). The SEEKING mind: Primal neuro-affective substrates for appetitive incentive states and their pathological dynamics in addictions and depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1805–1820. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amorim Neto, R., do, C., Golz, N., Polega, M., and Stewart, D. (2020). The impact of curiosity on teacher–student relationships. J. Educ. 202, 15–25. doi: 10.1177/0022057420943184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aspbury-Miyanishi, E. (2022). The affordances beyond what one does: Reconceptualizing teacher agency with Heidegger and Ecological Psychology. Teach. Teach. Educ. 113:103662. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2022.103662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baird, J. R., Gunstone, R. F., Penna, C., Fensham, P. J., and White, R. T. (1990). Researching balance between cognition and affect in science teaching and learning. Res. Sci. Educ. 1, 11–20. doi: 10.1007/BF02620475

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2016). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 9, 75–78. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. A., Bliss-Moreau, E., Duncan, S., Gendron, M., Mize, J., et al. (2007). Of mice and men: Natural kinds of emotions in the mammalian brain? A response to panksepp and Izard. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 297–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00046.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker-Asano, C., and Wachsmuth, I. (2008). “Affect simulation with primary and secondary emotions,” in International workshop on intelligent virtual agents, (Berlin: Springer), 15–28. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-85483-8_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beilock, S. L., Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., and Levine, S. C. (2010). Female teachers’ math anxiety affects girls’ math achievement. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 1860–1863. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910967107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benesch, S. (2018). Emotions as agency: Feeling rules, emotion labor, and English language teachers’ decision-making. System 79, 60–69. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2018.03.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, H. R. (2013). Social research methods: Qualitative and quantitative approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Biesta, G., Priestley, M., and Robinson, S. (2015). The role of beliefs in teacher agency. Teach. Teach. Theory Pract. 21, 624–640. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2015.1044325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., and Fini, C. (2019). Theories and explanations in psychology. Front. Psychol. 10:958. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borsboom, D., Kievit, R. A., Cervone, D., Brian, S., and Borsboom, H. D. (2009). “The two disciplines of scientific psychology, or: The disunity of psychology as a working hypothesis,” in Dynamic process methodology in the social and developmental sciences, eds J. Valsiner, P. C. M. Molenaar, M. C. D. P. Lyra, and N. Chaudhary (Berlin: Springer Science), 67–97. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-95922-1_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Box, G. E. P. (1979). Some problems of statistics and everyday life. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 74:1. doi: 10.2307/2286713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braniecka, A., Trzebińska, E., Dowgiert, A., and Wytykowska, A. (2014). Mixed emotions and coping: The benefits of secondary emotions. PLoS One 9:e103940. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Branigan, H. E., and Donaldson, D. I. (2019). Learning from learning logs: A case study of metacognition in the primary school classroom. Br. Educ. Res. J. 45, 791–820. doi: 10.1002/berj.3526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Branigan, H. E., and Donaldson, D. I. (2020). Teachers matter for metacognition: Facilitating metacognition in the primary school through teacher-pupil interactions. Think Skills Creat. 38:100718. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2020.100718

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2013). Successful qualitative research: A practical guide for beginners. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Buck, R. (2014). Emotion: A biosocial synthesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139049825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buus, N., and Perron, A. (2020). The quality of quality criteria: Replicating the development of the Consolidated Criteria for Reporting Qualitative Research (COREQ). Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 102:103452. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2019.103452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caro, T. M., and Hauser, M. D. (1992). Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Q. Rev. Biol. 67, 151–174. doi: 10.1086/417553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carson, R. L., and Chase, M. A. (2009). An examination of physical education teacher motivation from a self-determination theoretical framework. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagogy. 14, 335–353. doi: 10.1080/17408980802301866

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceha, J., Chhibber, N., Goh, J., McDonald, C., Oudeyer, P. Y., Kulić, D., et al. (2019). “Expression of curiosity in social robots design, perception, and effects on behaviour,” in Conference on human factors in computing systems - proceedings (Association for Computing Machinery), (Glasgow). doi: 10.1145/3290605.3300636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Celeghin, A., Diano, M., Bagnis, A., Viola, M., and Tamietto, M. (2017). Basic emotions in human neuroscience: Neuroimaging and beyond. Front. Psychol. 8:1432. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, M. L. (2013). Toward a theoretical model to understand teacher emotions and teacher burnout in the context of student misbehavior: Appraisal, regulation and coping. Motiv. Emotion 37, 799–817. doi: 10.1007/s11031-012-9335-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J. (2016). Understanding teacher emotions: The development of a teacher emotion inventory. Teach. Teach. Educ. 55, 68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2016.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collie, R. J., Martin, A. J., and Frydenberg, E. (2017). “Social and emotional learning: A brief overview and issues relevant to Australia and the Asia-Pacific,” in Social and emotional learning in Australia and the Asia-Pacific: Perspectives, programs and approaches, eds E. Frydenberg, A. J. Martin, and R. J. Collie (Berlin: Springer Science), 1–13. doi: 10.1007/978-981-10-3394-0_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connor, C., de Valliere, N., Warwick, J., Stewart-Brown, S., and Thompson, A. (2022). The COV-ED Survey: Exploring the impact of learning and teaching from home on parent/carers’ and teachers’ mental health and wellbeing during COVID-19 lockdown. BMC Public Health 22:889. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-13305-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. (2006). Descartes’ Error. New York, NY: Vintage.

Google Scholar

Damman, M., and Henkens, K. (2017). Constrained agency in later working lives: Introduction to the special Issue. Work Aging Retire 3, 225–230. doi: 10.1093/workar/wax015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danvers, E. C. (2015). Criticality’s affective entanglements: Rethinking emotion and critical thinking in higher education. Gend. Educ. 28, 282–297. doi: 10.1080/09540253.2015.1115469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinsmore, D. L. (2017). Examining the ontological and epistemic assumptions of research on metacognition, self-regulation and self-regulated learning. Educ. Psychol. (Lond) 37, 1125–1153. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2017.1333575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinsmore, D. L., Alexander, P. A., and Loughlin, S. M. (2008). Focusing the conceptual lens on metacognition, self-regulation, and self-regulated learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 20, 391–409. doi: 10.1007/s10648-008-9083-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Education Support (2020). Teacher Wellbeing Index. Available online at: https://www.educationsupport.org.uk/media/yfrhfjca/teacher_wellbeing_index_2020.pdf (accessed on October 2, 2021).

Google Scholar

Eidsvåg, G. M., and Rosell, Y. (2021). The power of belonging: Interactions and values in children’s group play in early childhood programs. Int. J. Early Childh. 53, 83–99. doi: 10.1007/s13158-021-00284-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cogn. Emot. 6, 169–200. doi: 10.1080/02699939208411068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emirbayer, M., and Mische, A. (1998). What is agency? Am. J. Soc. 103, 962–1023. doi: 10.1086/231294

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erss, M. (2018). ‘Complete freedom to choose within limits’–teachers’ views of curricular autonomy, agency and control in Estonia, Finland and Germany. Curr. J. 29, 238–256. doi: 10.1080/09585176.2018.1445514

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman-Barrett, L. (2017). How emotions are made. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

Google Scholar

Fereday, J., and Muir-Cochrane, E. (2006). Demonstrating rigor using thematic analysis: A hybrid approach of inductive and deductive coding and theme development. Int. J. Qual. Methods 5, 80–92. doi: 10.1177/160940690600500107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fossati, P. (2012). Neural correlates of emotion processing: From emotional to social brain. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 22, S487–S491. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C. (2014). “Teacher emotions,” in International handbook of emotions in education, (London: Routledge), 504–529.

Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., and Goetz, T. (2007). Girls and mathematics A “hopeless” issue? A control-value approach to gender differences in emotions towards mathematics. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 22, 497–514.

Google Scholar

Fried, L. (2010). Understanding and enhancing emotion and motivation regulation strategy use in the classroom. Int. J. Learn. 17, 115–130. doi: 10.18848/1447-9494/CGP/v17i06/47118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gigerenzer, G. (2010). Personal reflections on theory and psychology. Theory Psychol. 20, 733–743. doi: 10.1177/0959354310378184

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goi, C. L. (2018). The use of business simulation games in teaching and learning. J. Educ. Bus. 94, 342–349. doi: 10.1080/08832323.2018.1536028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greer, R. D. (1991). The teacher as strategic scientist: A solution to our educational crisis? Behav. Soc. Issues 2, 25–41. doi: 10.5210/bsi.v1i2.165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossnickle, E. M. (2016). Disentangling Curiosity: Dimensionality. Definitions, and Distinctions from Interest in Educational Contexts. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 28, 23–60. doi: 10.1007/s10648-014-9294-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

GTCS. (2020). Scotland professional review and development. Available online at: https://www.gtcs.org.uk/professional-update/professional-review-and-development/ (accessed on September 8, 2022).

Google Scholar

Guest, G., Bunce, A., and Johnson, L. (2016). How many interviews are enough?: An experiment with data saturation and variability. Field Methods 18, 59–82. doi: 10.1177/1525822X05279903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, J. A., and Knapp, M. (2015). APA handbook of nonverbal communication. Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/14669-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, J., and Yin, H. (2016). Teacher motivation: Definition, research development and implications for teachers. Cogent Educ. 3, 1217819. doi: 10.1080/2331186X.2016.1217819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannula, M. S. (2012). Exploring new dimensions of mathematics-related affect: Embodied and social theories. Res. Math. Educ. 14, 137–161. doi: 10.1080/14794802.2012.694281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harley, J. M., Lajoie, S. P., Tressel, T., and Jarrell, A. (2020). Fostering positive emotions and history knowledge with location-based augmented reality and tour-guide prompts. Learn. Instruct. 70:101163. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedge, N., and MacKenzie, A. (2016). Scotland’s Curriculum for Excellence: A defence of autonomy and personhood. Oxford Rev. Educ. 42, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/03054985.2015.1128890

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henritius, E., Löfström, E., and Hannula, M. S. (2019). University students’ emotions in virtual learning: A review of empirical research in the 21st century. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 50, 80–100. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12699

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiver, P., Al-Hoorie, A. H., and Larsen-Freeman, D. (2022). Toward a transdisciplinary integration of research purposes and methods for complex dynamic systems theory: Beyond the quantitative-qualitative divide. Int. Rev. Appl. Lingu. Langu. Teach. 60, 7–22. doi: 10.1515/iral-2021-0022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hökkä, P., Vähäsantanen, K., Paloniemi, S., Herranen, S., and Eteläpelto, A. (2019). Emotions in leaders’ enactment of professional agency. J. Workplace Learn. 31, 143–165. doi: 10.1108/JWL-07-2018-0086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 137:297. doi: 10.1037/a0022128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Immordino-Yang, M. H. (2016). Emotion, Sociality, and the Brain’s default mode network: Insights for educational practice and policy. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 3, 211–219. doi: 10.1177/2372732216656869

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Impedovo, M. A. (2021). “Personal resources,” in Identity and teacher professional development, (Cham: Springer), 31–45. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-71367-6_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new Paradigm. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 260–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarrell, A., Harley, J. M., Lajoie, S., and Naismith, L. (2017). Success, failure and emotions: Examining the relationship between performance feedback and emotions in diagnostic reasoning. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 65, 1263–1284. doi: 10.1007/s11423-017-9521-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keller, M. M., and Becker, E. S. (2021). Teachers’ emotions and emotional authenticity: Do they matter to students’ emotional responses in the classroom? Teach. Teach. Theory Pract. 27, 404–422. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2020.1834380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klassen, R. M., Perry, N. E., and Frenzel, A. C. (2012). Teachers’ relatedness with students: An underemphasized component of teachers’ basic psychological needs. J. Educ. Psychol. 104:150. doi: 10.1037/a0026253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behav. Brain Sci. 38:e31. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14000090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ladouce, S., Donaldson, D. I., Dudchenko, P. A., and Ietswaart, M. (2017). Understanding minds in real-world environments: Toward a mobile cognition approach. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:694. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaMarre, A., and Chamberlain, K. (2022). Innovating qualitative research methods: Proposals and possibilities. Methods Psychol. 6:100083. doi: 10.1016/j.metip.2021.100083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasky, S. (2005). A sociocultural approach to understanding teacher identity, agency and professional vulnerability in a context of secondary school reform. Teach. Teach. Educ. 21, 899–916. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2005.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leyens, J. P., Rodriguez-Perez, A., Rodriguez-Torres, R., Gaunt, R., Paladino, M. P., Vaes, J., et al. (2001). Psychological essentialism and the differential attribution of uniquely human emotions to ingroups and outgroups. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 31, 395–411. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.50

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., and Barger, M. M. (2014). “Achievement goals and emotions,” in International handbook of emotions in education, (London: Routledge), 142–161.

Google Scholar

Lykken, D. T. (1991). “What’s wrong with Psychology, anyway?,” in Thinking clearly about psychology, eds D. Cicchetti and W. M. Grove (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press), 1–39.

Google Scholar

MacLean, J. (2016). Teachers as agents of change in curricular reform: The position of dance revisited. Sport Educ. Soc. 23, 563–577. doi: 10.1080/13573322.2016.1249464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metcalfe, J., Kennedy-Pyers, T., and Vuorre, M. (2021). Curiosity and the desire for agency: Wait, wait don’t tell me! Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 6, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00330-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montag, C., and Panksepp, J. (2017). Primary emotional systems and personality: An evolutionary perspective. Front. Psychol. 8:464. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss, G., Bradbury, A., Braun, A., Duncan, S., and Levy, R. (2021). Learning through Disruption: Using schools’ experiences of Covid to build a more resilient education system. Available online at: https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/10136102/1/Learning%20Through%20Disruption%20Main%20Report.pdf (accessed on September 9, 2022).

Google Scholar

Murayama, K. (2022). A reward-learning framework of knowledge acquisition: An integrated account of curiosity, interest, and intrinsic–extrinsic rewards. Psychol. Rev. 129, 175–198. doi: 10.1037/rev0000349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narayanan, M., and Ordynans, J. (2022). Self-efficacy is a story: Teachers’ possible selves in a pandemic world. J. Multicult. Educ. 16, 64–76. doi: 10.1108/JME-08-2021-0157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newton, P. M., and Salvi, A. (2020). How common is belief in the learning styles neuromyth, and does it matter? A pragmatic systematic review. Front. Educ. (Lausanne) 5:270. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2020.602451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oktay, J. S. (2012). Grounded theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199753697.001.0001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oplatka, I. (2012). Emotional aspects of extra-role behaviours in prevention education: Insights from interviews with exceptional teachers and school principals. Teach. Teach. 18, 717–732. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2012.746506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortony, A. (2021). Are All “Basic Emotions” Emotions? A Problem for the (Basic) emotions construct. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17, 41–61. doi: 10.1177/1745691620985415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oudeyer, P.-Y., Gottlieb, J., and Lopes, M. (2016). Intrinsic motivation, curiosity and learning: Theory and applications in educational technologies. Prog. Brain Res. 229, 257–284. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2016.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overton, W. (2014). “Relational developmental systems and developmental science: A focus on methodology,” in Handbook of developmental systems theory and methodology, (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 19–65.

Google Scholar

Owens, J. K., and Hudson, A. K. (2021). Prioritizing teacher emotions: Shifting teacher training to a digital environment. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 69, 59–62. doi: 10.1007/s11423-020-09921-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Lane, R. D., Solms, M., and Smith, R. (2017). Reconciling cognitive and affective neuroscience perspectives on the brain basis of emotional experience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 76, 187–215. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 18, 315–341. doi: 10.1007/s10648-006-9029-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R. (2019). The murky distinction between curiosity and interest: State of the art and future prospects. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 4, 905–914. doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09512-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., and Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2014). International handbook of emotions in education. London: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203148211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., and Perry, R. P. (2014). “Control-value theory of achievement emotions,” in International handbook of emotions in education, eds R. Pekrun and L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (New York: Routledge):120.

Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., and Perry, R. P. (2007). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: An integrative approach to emotions in education. Available online at: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-99861 (accessed June 16, 2021).

Google Scholar

Prescott, M. J., and Buchanan-Smith, H. M. (2003). Training nonhuman primates using positive reinforcement techniques. J. Appl. Animal Welfare Sci. 6, 157–161. doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0603_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Priestley, M., Biesta, G., and Robinson, S. (2015). Teacher agency: An ecological Approach. London: Bloomsbury. doi: 10.4324/9781315678573-15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyhältö, K., Pietarinen, J., and Soini, T. (2014). Comprehensive school teachers’ professional agency in large-scale educational change. J. Educ. Change 15, 303–325. doi: 10.1007/s10833-013-9215-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyhältö, K., Pietarinen, J., and Soini, T. (2015). Teachers professional agency and learning-from adaption to active modification in the teacher community. Teach. Teach. Theory Pract. 21, 811–830. doi: 10.1080/13540602.2014.995483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

QSR International (2020). Nvivo. Doncaster: QSR International.

Google Scholar

Ravalier, J., and Walsh, J. (2017). Scotland’s Teachers: Working conditions and wellbeing. Available online at: https://www.eis.org.uk/Content/images/Employ%20Relations/WorkingConditionsReport.pdf (accessed on February 10, 2021).

Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Shin, S. H. (2020). How teachers can support students’. Agentic Engagement. Theory Pract. 59, 150–161. doi: 10.1080/00405841.2019.1702451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J., and Su, Y.-L. (2014). “Teacher motivation,” in The oxford handbook of work engagement, motivation, and self-determination theory, ed. M. Gagné (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Reschly, A. L., and Christenson, S. L. (2012). Moving From “Context Matters” to engaged partnerships with families. J. Educ. Psychol. Consult. 22, 62–78. doi: 10.1080/10474412.2011.649650

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigo-Ruiz. (2016). Effect of Teachers’. Emotions on their students: Some evidence. J. Educ. Soc. Policy 3, 73–79.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development and wellness. New York: Guilford Press. doi: 10.1521/978.14625/28806

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahlberg, P. (2010). Rethinking accountability in a knowledge society. J. Educ. Change 11, 45–61. doi: 10.1007/s10833-008-9098-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sang, G. (2020). “Teacher agency,” in Encyclopedia of teacher education, (Singapore: Springer), 1–5. doi: 10.1007/978-981-13-1179-6_271-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutz, P. A., Pekrun, R., and Phye, G. (2007). Emotion in education. Amsterdam: Elsevier Inc. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-372545-5.X5000-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, C., and Sutton, R. E. (2009). Emotions and change during professional development for teachers: A mixed methods study. J. Mixed Methods Res. 3, 151–171. doi: 10.1177/1558689808325770

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scottish Executive (2006a). A curriculum for excellence: Progress and proposals. Edinburgh.

Google Scholar

Scottish Executive (2006b). The curriculum for excellence: Building the curriculum 1. Available online at: https://education.gov.scot/Documents/btc1.pdf (accessed on June 24, 2021).

Google Scholar

Scottish Government (2019). Summary statistics for schools in Scotland no. 10, 2019 Edn. Available online at: https://www.gov.scot/publications/summary-statistics-schools-scotland-no-10-2019-edition/pages/4/ (accessed on June 16, 2021).

Google Scholar

Sepulveda-Escobar, P., and Morrison, A. (2020). Online teaching placement during the COVID-19 pandemic in Chile: Challenges and opportunities. Eur. J. Teach. Educ. 43, 587–607. doi: 10.1080/02619768.2020.1820981

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, B., and Teemant, A. (2021). Agency, identity, power: An agentive triad model for teacher action. Educ. Philos. Theory 54, 1464–1475. doi: 10.1080/00131857.2021.1929174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest - The curious emotion. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 57–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00548.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvia, P. J. (2017). “Curiosity,” in The science of interest, eds P. O’Keefe and J. Harackiewicz (Berlin: Springer), 97–107. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-55509-6_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinnema, C., Nieveen, N., and Priestley, M. (2020). Successful futures, successful curriculum: What can Wales learn from international curriculum reforms? Curric. J. 31, 181–201. doi: 10.1002/curj.17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, B. F. (1953). Some contributions of an experimental analysis of behavior to psychology as a whole. Am. Psychol. 8, 69–78. doi: 10.1037/h0054118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, X., and Yang, L. (2021). A narrative case study of chinese senior high school english teachers’ emotions: An ecological perspective. Front. Psychol. 12:792400. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.792400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swinney, J. (2020). Coronavirus (COVID19) – impact on education: Deputy First Minister speech, 19 March 2020. Available online at: https://www.gov.scot/publications/statement-covid19-managing-impacts-scottish-education/ (accessed on June 16, 2021).

Google Scholar

Tao, J., and Gao, X. (2017). Teacher agency and identity commitment in curricular reform. Teach. Teach. Educ. 63, 346–355. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2017.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tateo, L. (2021). “The horizon worker,” in Identity and teacher professional development, ed. M. A. Impedovo (Cham: SpringerBriefs), 93–96.

Google Scholar

Tillmanns, T. (2019). Learning sustainability as an effect of disruption. Environ. Educ. Res. 26, 14–26. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2019.1682125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, A., Sainsbury, P., and Craig, J. (2007). Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ): A 32-item checklist for interviews and focus groups. Int. J. Qual. Health Care 19, 349–357. doi: 10.1093/intqhc/mzm042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsui, A. B. M., Chan, C. K. K., Harfitt, G., and Leung, P. (2020). Crisis and opportunity in teacher preparation in the pandemic: Exploring the “adjacent possible.”. J. Profess. Capital Commun. 5, 237–245. doi: 10.1108/JPCC-07-2020-0061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Usher, E. L. (2021). Contextualizing teacher motivation research. Learn. Instr. 76:101544. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2021.101544

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wasserstein, R. L., and Lazar, N. A. (2016). The ASA’s Statement on p-Values: Context. process, and purpose. Am. Stat. 70, 129–133. doi: 10.1080/00031305.2016.1154108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, D. S. (2016). “Two meanings of complex adaptive systems,” in Complexity and evolution: Toward a new synthesis for economics, (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press):19. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9780262035385.003.0003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y. (2018). “A Methodological Review of L2 teacher emotion research: Advances, challenges and future directions. emotions,” in Second language teaching: Theory, research and teacher education, (New York, NY: Springer International Publishing), 35–49. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-75438-3_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: teaching, individual differences, education, agency, emotion, emotion percept, complex dynamic systems, thematic analysis

Citation: Porter K, Miles PJ and Donaldson DI (2022) Teachers’ emotions in the time of COVID: Thematic analysis of interview data reveals drivers of professional agency. Front. Psychol. 13:987690. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.987690

Received: 06 July 2022; Accepted: 18 October 2022;
Published: 03 November 2022.

Edited by:

Nahia Idoiaga, University of the Basque Country, Spain

Reviewed by:

Marija Marković, University of Niš, Serbia
Sanjeev Kumar, All India Institute of Medical Sciences Bhopal, India
Parmeshwar Satpathy, Indian Institute of Technology Kharagpur, India

Copyright © 2022 Porter, Miles and Donaldson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Karen Porter, kp81@st-andrews.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.