Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 26 August 2022
Sec. Developmental Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Exploring Phenotypic Diversity in Ectothermic Reptiles and in Birds: Developmental Physiology Throughout the Lifespan and Across Scales View all 7 articles

Developmental asynchrony might not reduce fitness in early life in painted turtles

P. L. Colbert&#x;P. L. Colbert1R.-J. Spencer&#x;R.-J. Spencer2F. J. Janzen&#x;
F. J. Janzen3*
  • 1SAb Biotherapeutics, Sioux Falls, SD, United States
  • 2School of Science, Western Sydney University, Penrith, NSW, Australia
  • 3Kellogg Biological Station, Departments of Fisheries & Wildlife and Integrative Biology, Michigan State University, Hickory Corners, MI, United States

Synchronous hatching and emergence of turtles from nests may be adaptive in predator avoidance during dispersal. However, little is known about the phenotypic consequences of such synchrony or the generality of predator avoidance in driving the evolution of this trait. Colbert et al. (2010) found that less advanced embryos hatched early in the presence of more advanced sibs, sustaining a persistent reduction in neuromuscular function. In this study, we experimentally assessed the influence of such accelerated embryonic development on hatching success, winter survival, and survival during terrestrial dispersal from the nest. Although we predicted that shortened incubation periods would reduce survival, early-hatching individuals suffered no detectable fitness costs at any stage considered in this study. Incubation temperature did not affect hatching success, and offspring sex did not affect survival across treatment groups. Incubation regime influenced offspring body size and was negatively correlated with dispersal time, however, there was no effect on survival during winter or terrestrial dispersal. Lack of a detectable fitness cost in these key early-life stages associated with hatching synchrony is consistent with a single, predator avoidance origin for this trait and retention in C. picta and other derived turtles via phylogenetic inertia.

Introduction

An understanding of factors that contribute to phenotypic expression during development and early life is central to studies of trait evolution. It is often during these stages that phenotypic variation resulting from genetic, parental, and environmental effects is subject most stringently to selection. In oviparous organisms, particularly those lacking nest attendance, the incubation environment is of singular importance as eggs may be subject to substantial variation in abiotic conditions during development. Thermal variation in particular can influence developmental rate, hatching success, and the sex, morphology, and behavior of offspring (e.g., Ratte, 1985; Deeming and Ferguson, 1991; Andrews, 2004). For turtles, which are characterized by near universal lack of nest attendance, synchronous hatching and its phenotypic and broader biological consequences reside at the interface of the effects of temperature, development, and selection.

Turtle nests are often flask-shaped with eggs deposited in several layers. As a result, thermal gradients can exist such that eggs near the top experience warmer incubation conditions than those near the bottom (Thompson, 1988; Maloney et al., 1990; Telemeco et al., 2016). Because developmental rate generally increases with increasing temperature (Thompson, 1997), asynchronous hatching and emergence of neonates is predicted. Yet synchronous hatching and/or emergence occur (Carr and Hirth, 1961; Balasz and Ross, 1974; Spencer et al., 2001; but see Standing et al., 1999; Houghton and Hays, 2001) and may have evolved in response to predation during dispersal from nests. Hatching synchrony facilitates physical escape from the nest (Carr and Hirth, 1961; Spencer et al., 2001) and may aid predator avoidance via swamping or the per capita dilution of predation risk (Arnold and Wassersug, 1978; Dehn, 1990; Santos et al., 2016; but see Tucker et al., 2008). However, questions remain as to how synchronous hatching occurs, whether the mechanism producing such synchrony generates additional phenotypic variation in offspring, and, if so, how selection acts upon that variation.

Hatching synchrony has been explored in at least five turtle species, occurring in four of them. Riley et al. (2020) detected synchronous hatching in spiny softshell turtles (Apalone spinifera) but not in northern map turtles (Graptemys geographica). Spencer et al. (2001), Colbert et al. (2010), and Field et al. (2021) investigated synchronous hatching experimentally by inducing developmental asynchrony among clutch mates of Murray River turtles (Emydura macquarii), painted turtles (Chrysemys picta), and loggerhead turtles (Caretta caretta), respectively. Synchrony occurred in all cases because less developed embryos hatched earlier than normal while more advanced sibs did not delay hatching. However, early-hatching C. picta exhibited poor righting ability compared to their sibs both at hatching and after overwintering in nests. For E. macquarii, C. caretta, and A. spinifera, the potential benefits of group formation may outweigh any individual developmental costs as neonates generally depart the nest within days after hatching. In contrast, C. picta (like G. geographica; Gibbons and Nelson, 1978) typically remain in the nest up to 9 months after hatching (Weisrock and Janzen, 1999; Murphy et al., 2020). Thus, early hatching with reduced neuromuscular function, even in the absence of metabolic compensation (McGlashan et al., 2018), is puzzling for C. picta. In this study, we asked whether this seemingly detrimental trait in painted turtles was accompanied by reduced fitness, focusing specifically on survival during three key early-life stages in this species: 1) hatching, 2) over-winter in the nest, and 3) terrestrial dispersal from nest to water. In brief, do early-hatching neonates suffer detectable survival-linked costs in early life?

Materials and methods

The painted turtle (C. picta) is a common inhabitant of freshwater environments from coast to coast in the northern U.S. and southern Canada and into parts of the southern U.S. (Ernst and Lovich, 2009). At our study site, typically 10 eggs are deposited in relatively shallow terrestrial nests (ca. 9 cm; Morjan, 2003) within which vertical temperature differentials during embryonic development may span as much as 6°C (FJJ unpubl. data). Following hatching, most neonates remain in the nest throughout the winter and emerge the following spring (Weisrock and Janzen, 1999; Murphy et al., 2020). Cohorts most closely follow a Type III survivorship schedule, with high mortality through the first few years of life followed by low mortality near maturity (Iverson, 1991a; Frazer et al., 1991; Warner et al., 2016). As such, phenotypic traits that offer survival advantages early in life are of great importance (Iverson, 1991a, b; Spencer and Janzen, 2010). Eggs were collected from 24 newly constructed nests (<1 day old) at the Thomson Causeway, Thomson, Illinois, United States from 27 May through 1 June 2004. Upon collection, eggs were labeled according to clutch and egg number using a blunt HB pencil and placed in moist vermiculite for transport to the laboratory. Sixteen of these 24 clutches were part of a previous study of the phenotypic effects of hatching synchrony (Colbert et al., 2010). Evaluation of hatching synchrony involved two experiments; one that tested whether less advanced embryos truncated development time to “catch up” to more advanced sibs while the other assessed whether more advanced embryos could “wait” to hatch in the presence of less advanced sibs.

Incubation regimes to establish developmental asynchrony among clutch-mates followed the general protocol of Spencer et al. (2001). Specifically, half clutches were incubated at either cool (26°C) or warm (30°C) temperatures for the first 11 days of incubation and were then reunited with clutch-mates at a common incubation temperature (Figure 1). Two treatment levels existed within each experiment based on group status (experimental or control [C]) and egg movement (moved [M] or not [NM]), resulting in eight distinct treatments (Table 1). The additional eight clutches were half of a replication of the synchrony experiment, the other half of which was lost due to incubator failure. All 24 clutches were treated in an identical manner and are therefore included, although sample sizes were unequal among treatments (Table 1). Throughout incubation, all egg boxes were maintained at a water potential of −150 kPa in vermiculite.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. General incubation protocol (modified from Figure 2 of Colbert et al., 2010). The steps employed to establish developmental asynchrony and promote shortened incubation periods of less advanced embryos. Temperatures used (26 or 30°C) depended on the treatment and 11 days indicates the period over which asynchrony was established in the study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Experimental groups, treatment designations, and clutch representation.

Beginning at day 40 of incubation, eggs were visually inspected for signs of pipping (the initial breaking of the egg shell by the caruncle) at least three times daily. Neonates that extricated themselves from their eggshell were considered to have hatched successfully. Once fully hatched, individuals were removed from their containers and their righting times measured as a gauge of neuromuscular function. In this procedure, animals are placed on their carapace and the time required to right themselves (i.e., flip over) is recorded. Freedberg et al. (2001, 2004) note that hatchlings are highly motivated to perform this task and righting ability may be important to survival during terrestrial dispersal by reducing the risks of desiccation and predation (Burger, 1976; Steyermark and Spotila, 2001a; Delmas et al., 2007; but see Mitchell et al., 2016). Following righting trials, hatchlings were given a unique combination of notches in their marginal scutes and transferred to clutch-specific plastic cups containing a moist paper towel in preparation for over-winter torpor.

Over the winter months, temperatures in dark environmental chambers were maintained at approximately 5°C and cups were hydrated weekly with distilled water to prevent desiccation of hatchlings. Beginning in the second week of April 2005, chamber temperatures were slowly raised to 18°C (ca. 2°C/d for 7 days). Neonates were removed from the chambers and maintained at 22°C for 48 h before righting trials were again performed and morphological measurements taken (results reported in Colbert et al., 2010). We measured carapace length (CL; measured as the distance from the nuchal scute along the midline to the inter-marginal notch) to the nearest 0.1 mm using a dial caliper and body mass to the nearest 0.01 g using an electronic balance. Neonates that were alive at this point were recorded as having survived through the winter. For about 4 weeks, these turtles were maintained in plastic containers (60 cm × 30 cm x 15 cm) filled with de-chlorinated tap water to a depth of about 8 cm and containing several basking platforms under 12:12 lighting. Turtles were fed a diet of bloodworms and Reptomin® ad libitum, and containers were cleaned twice weekly.

The neonatal dispersal experiment was conducted in the Upper Mississippi River National Fish and Wildlife Refuge in Carroll County, Illinois adjacent to the nesting area. The release site was on an open, west-facing slope of sand prairie where painted turtles are known to nest (Paitz et al., 2007). Vegetation in the immediate area of the experiment included needlegrass (Stipa sp.), Ohio spiderwort (Tradescantia ohiensis), prickly pear cactus (Opuntia humifusa), red cedar (Juniperus virginiana), and skunkbrush (Rhus aromatica). Kolbe and Janzen (2002a) and Warner and Mitchell (2013) provide further description of the study area.

A drift fence was constructed along the base of the slope near the east shore of the Mississippi River. The drift fence was comprised of 30-cm high aluminum flashing entrenched 10 cm in the soil and arranged as a semi-circle with a 13-m radius. Along the fence, seven numbered plastic cylinders (i.e., pits; 10 cm diameter × 25 cm deep) were buried flush with the ground, abutting the fence at 7-m intervals. A semi-circular design was used to reduce the chance of turtles escaping; recording which pits captured turtles allowed us to determine the likelihood that such an event might have occurred (see below). Six naturalistic overwinter nest cavities, or nesticles (Colbert, 2006), were created 10 cm apart at the center of the semi-circle and a brown tarp was pinned down over their back edges to leave a small opening through which neonates could emerge. Although 13 m is below the overall mean nest distance from water at our study site (Harms et al., 2005), it is not atypical (Kolbe and Janzen, 2001). The experiment was initiated at 1025 h on 25 May 2005 when 19-20 randomly chosen turtles were placed into each nesticle. This date falls within the range when neonatal Chrysemys typically disperse from nests in this area (Murphy et al., 2020), and at an hour when emergence is known to occur in related turtle species (Tucker, 1997).

Following release, the fence was checked at 1510 and 1950 h, and on each subsequent day at approximately 0700, 1300, and 1900 h until 0700 h on 7 June. Observers noted the presence of potential predators during each visit and the sand surrounding each pit was checked for tracks and subsequently brushed smooth. Neonates found along the fence or in pits were recovered and their identity and location (i.e., pit number 1-7) recorded. All turtles recovered alive at the fence were considered to have survived terrestrial dispersal. These animals were subsequently given a unique combination of toe clips and released in the water near the nest sites from which the eggs were collected.

Logistic regression was used to determine whether incubation treatment significantly influenced hatching success, over-winter survival, and survival during terrestrial dispersal. For the purposes of this study, the groups of primary interest were those known (26NM) or suspected (26M) to have hatched early (see definitions in Table 1) at a cost to neuromuscular function (Colbert et al., 2010). We expected that premature hatching would result in reduced hatching success relative to sibs and/or controls. In addition, we hypothesized that underdeveloped hatchlings may be more sensitive to temperature extremes and therefore suffer higher winter mortality. During terrestrial dispersal, we predicted that the developmental costs sustained by hatching early would increase the time required for individuals to migrate. Increased exposure to the rigors of the terrestrial environment (e.g., predation and desiccation) would, in turn, cause higher mortality in these treatments. Hence, in regression analyses of all three early-life stages, we set up the following contrasts concerning treatment effects on survival

HA: 26NM < 26CNM, 30M

HA: 26M < 30NM

A general linear model was used to test for treatment effects on the time required for individuals to reach the fence and evaluate the proposed exposure time mechanism for any reduced survival during terrestrial dispersal. Time was measured from the point of release until recapture to the nearest 0.25 days. Individuals recaptured on the first day were scored as reaching the fence in 0.25 days. On subsequent days, recapture times were recorded as composites of the day of capture relative to the release date and the time of day as follows: 0.25 (morning check), 0.5 (afternoon check), or 0.75 (evening check). Individuals with reduced neuromuscular performance were expected to take longer to arrive at the fence than their sibs and/or controls. Therefore, we set up the following contrasts concerning treatment effects on time to recapture:

HA: 26NM > 26CNM, 30M

HA: 26M > 30NM

Chrysemys picta exhibits temperature-dependent sex determination in which males are produced at cooler temperatures (typically <28.5 C) and females at warmer temperatures (Carter et al., 2019). In this study, temperature switches were performed prior to the temperature-sensitive period of sex determination (about the middle-third of incubation; Janzen and Paukstis, 1991), thus no treatment-specific comparisons are made between sexes. We evaluated the influence of sex on hatching success, survival, and time until recapture separately using logistic regression, but without any a priori predictions of its effect.

Many studies have documented that “bigger is better” during terrestrial dispersal from nests in turtles (Janzen, 1993; Janzen et al., 2000a, b, 2007, Tucker, 2000; Myers et al., 2007; Paitz et al., 2007; Tucker et al., 2008; but see Congdon et al., 1999; Kolbe and Janzen, 2001; Filoramo and Janzen, 2002). Such effects could confound inference in this study, particularly if they operate via the exposure time mechanism proposed above. Therefore, we also analyzed the effect of body size (CL and mass) on survival during dispersal using logistic regression. To explicitly test the exposure time link between body size and survival, we performed linear regressions using body size measures to predict dispersal times. Clutch effects were not analyzed statistically in any of our models due to sample size limitations. Instead, the impact of clutch in each analysis was assessed qualitatively by identifying clutches in which mortality was high regardless of treatment or sex. Descriptive statistics were computed using JMP 5.1.2 (SAS Institute Inc, 2004) and regression analyses were performed using SAS 9.1.3 (SAS Institute Inc, 2003).

Results

Of the 144 eggs incubated, 129 successfully hatched (89.6%; Table 2) and success ranged by treatment from 73.3% (30CNM) to 100% (30NM; Table 2). Female-producing temperatures resulted in somewhat lower success than did male-producing temperatures (85.2 vs. 92.2%, respectively; χ21 = 1.7, p = 0.19) but this was heavily influenced by one control clutch (30CM/30CNM) in which five of six eggs failed.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Hatching success and post-hatching survival as a function of treatment and sex.

Treatment-specific contrasts were consistent with our predictions; early-hatching groups suffered reduced hatching success when compared to their controls and/or sibs (Figure 2). Yet, among all groups, early-hatching treatments (26M and 26NM) exhibited intermediate hatching success, and incubation treatment was not a statistically significant source of variation (χ27 = 6.0, p = 0.54). The high mortality in the single control clutch noted above did not account for lack of model fit, as exclusion of this clutch did not alter results (χ27 = 3.4, p = 0.85).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Hatching success and survival of early-hatching embryos versus their corresponding controls and/or sibs (top) early-hatching turtles from the catch-up experiment (26M), their controls (26CM), and sibs (30NM) and (bottom) suspected early-hatching turtles from the wait treatment (26NM) and their sibs (30M). Hatching success and post-hatching survival are reported as the proportion of individuals hatched or alive at the end of a given stage relative to the number in existence at the start of that stage, expressed as a percent. 95% confidence intervals are calculated using the Wilson/Brown method in Prism software (v9.4.0, GraphPad Software). The two treatments expected to suffer a cost are italicized.

Winter survival was high overall; 121 of the 129 turtles that hatched successfully were alive in the spring (93.8%; Table 2). Survival was lowest in the 30CM treatment (75%), and several treatments suffered no losses over winter (Table 2). Sex did not influence winter survival (χ21 = 0.7, p = 0.39), with males and females having similar high survival rates (91.3% female vs. 95.2% male). Clutch was an important factor in winter survival for both sexes; male and female groups each had a single clutch that disproportionately reduced overall survival (50% mortality in both cases).

Our predictions with respect to treatment were not supported. No reduction in winter survival was apparent for the early-hatching treatments (Figure 2), nor did incubation regime significantly affect winter survival (χ27 = 2.2, p = 0.95). The clutch effect noted above likely had little effect on treatment-specific survival, as mortality was distributed near evenly over four treatment groups (30CM, 30CNM, 26NM, and 30M).

Apparent survival during terrestrial dispersal averaged 58.5% over all treatments (Table 2). One individual failed to disperse and was recovered alive from its nesticle at the end of the experiment (excluded from analyses). The distribution of recovery sites (i.e., pit numbers) was approximately normal with a mean value of 3.75, which was near the center of the fence (pit 4). No individuals were recovered near the terminal pits (1 and 7), and the 99% confidence interval about the mean pit value was narrow and nearly centered (3.4–4.1). Thus, it is unlikely that any neonates escaped recapture by missing the fence. The final fence check occurred 5 days after the last individual was recaptured, and no turtles (alive or dead) were found in the release area during a thorough inspection at the end of the experiment. Therefore, it is also unlikely that turtles were migrating at the end of the experiment and recorded as false mortalities. Consequently, 58.5% should be a close approximation to true survival, and failure to find carcasses within the release area points to predation as the primary source of mortality. Potential predators observed in the release site included brown thrasher (Toxostoma rufum), common grackle (Quiscalus quiscula), red-winged blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus), blue racer (Coluber constrictor), and a coyote (Canis latrans) pup.

Treatment-specific survival ranged from 0 (30CM) to 90.9% (26M). Females exhibited reduced survival relative to males, but this difference was not statistically significant (50.0 vs. 64.0%, respectively; χ21 = 1.7, p = 0.20). Although the magnitude of clutch effects during migration and overall was not discerned, only two clutches were not represented among the survivors.

Our hypothesis that reduced incubation period would lead to lower survival during migration was not supported. Rather, early hatching modestly increased survivorship (Figure 2; Table 2), however, such differences were not statistically significant (χ27 = 6.7, p = 0.47).

Individual dispersal times ranged from 0.25–8.75 days, and treatment averages from 1.1–3.5 days. The distribution of recapture times was right-skewed with a mean recapture time of 1.7 days (99% CI = 1.2–2.2 days). Sex effects were not apparent, as males and females required approximately the same amount of time to reach the fence (1.75 and 1.95 days, respectively; F1 = 0.2, p = 0.68). Similarly, clutch did not appear to affect dispersal as, within clutches, times generally ranged the entire spectrum from short (<1.2 days) to long (>2.2 days).

Incubation regime impacted the amount of time required for neonates to reach the fence (F6 = 2.8, p = 0.02). However, contrary to expectation, early-hatching groups were among the fastest relative to both sibs and/or controls (Figure 3) and overall. The average travel time for the 26M group (1.1 d) was below the lower bound of the 99% CI about mean recapture time, and considerably shorter than that of their sibs (30NM, F1 = 8.6, p = 0.005; Figure 3). Consistent with the exposure time hypothesis, the group with the shortest travel time also exhibited the highest survivorship during terrestrial dispersal (26M). However, the two treatments that averaged longer than 2.2 days to reach the fence (26CNM and 30NM) had intermediate survivorship.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Time (d) required to reach the drift fence for early-hatching embryos versus their corresponding controls and/or sibs. The two early-hatching treatments in the two experiments expected to require the most time to disperse are italicized.

Mean measures of CL and body mass of all individuals released were 27.2 mm (SD ± 1.6 mm, range = 24.0–31.3 mm) and 4.55 g (SD ± 0.7 g, range = 2.82–6.01 g) and neither measure differed between sexes (F1 = 0.5, p = 0.47 and F1 = 2.7, p = 0.10, respectively). Results from logistic regressions indicated that neither size measure had a meaningful impact on survival during dispersal (CL: χ21 = 0.4, p = 0.53; mass: χ21 = 0.04, p = 0.85), even though inverse relationships existed between CL and dispersal time (r = -0.27, F1 = 5.3, p = 0.02) and between mass and dispersal time (r = -0.23, F1 = 3.8, p = 0.06).

Discussion

We expected that premature hatching and its associated decline in performance (Colbert et al., 2010) would translate into reduced survival at hatching, during over-wintering, and during terrestrial dispersal, but we detected no such fitness costs in this study. Although not statistically significant, the pattern of hatching success was consistent with our predictions (Figure 2) and Colbert et al. (2010) reported that turtles that hatched prematurely had increased volumes of protruding yolk sacs. Riley et al. (2020) did not find that latter pattern in the two turtle species they investigated, however. Evidence from birds also yields conflicting results (Nilsson and Persson 2004). Early-hatching painted turtles may have reduced capacity to right themselves, but they were among the fastest to complete dispersal in this study. Similarly, Irschick et al. (2005) found a general inverse relationship between maximum sprint speed in the laboratory and the percentage of maximum speed used to escape threats in the field in lizards. The ability for a turtle to right itself quickly does not necessarily reduce predation risk (Mitchell et al., 2016; but see Delmas et al., 2007).

Our overwintering temperatures did not necessarily expose turtles to extreme cold (5°C), but such conditions may represent future overwintering temperature regimes as climates warm. Indeed, winter temperatures in the contiguous U.S. already have risen 3°C in the past 125 years (USGCRP et al., 2017). Minimum temperatures in C. picta nests from our study population in Illinois during the winter of 1995–1996 ranged from approximately −12 to −2°C (Weisrock and Janzen, 1999), yet only fell below −8°C in one nest in the winter of 2016–2017 (Murphy et al., 2020). On the other hand, above-freezing temperatures impose significant metabolic demands on neonatal turtles compared to below-freezing conditions (Willette et al., 2005), where metabolic rates are negligible (Spencer and Janzen, 2011). Hence, although this experiment did not strongly test the cold tolerance of early-hatching turtles, it suggests they can meet increased metabolic burdens.

Although we found no evidence for sex effects on hatching success and post-hatching survival in this study, we see a cumulative reduction in fitness for females across all three stages (Table 2). Sex-specific differences in survival have been documented in other turtle species (e.g., Steyermark and Spotila, 2001b; Freedberg et al., 2001, 2004), as well as a range of other traits in painted turtles (Warner et al., 2020). In species with TSD, sex and temperature effects on life-history traits are often confounded, and the absence of statistically significant sex effects on survival at each stage in this study may be due to the thermal variance incorporated through temperature switches, which could have produced intermediate phenotypes and thus blurred group distinctions. Comparisons of control clutches only (26CM and 26CNM vs. 30CM and 30CNM), which experienced no thermal variance, show that cooler incubation temperatures resulted in higher success and survivorship at every stage considered (Table 2), significantly so over winter (χ23 = 9.46, p = 0.024) and during terrestrial dispersal (χ23 = 15.9, p = 0.001). Because sex-specific differences were only apparent when incubation temperatures were held constant, this finding supports the hypothesis that such differences in early life are attributable primarily to incubation temperatures. Hence, long exposure to constant temperatures may induce sex effects and may not reflect temperatures in nature, where constant temperatures are rare, particularly in shallow nests of freshwater turtles (Thompson, 1988; Weisrock and Janzen, 1999).

In this study, body size did not influence survival during dispersal contra Paitz et al. (2007) for the same population, although we found partial support for reduced exposure times of larger turtles. The interaction of numerous dynamic biotic and abiotic factors likely results in varying selection on offspring traits such that no single factor consistently predicts dispersal success (e.g., Marshall et al., 2006). When predation is high, body size effects may be more pronounced (e.g., Janzen et al., 2000a, b), whereas dry conditions may force young turtles to burrow for extended periods, altering the landscape of selective pressures (e.g., Kolbe and Janzen, 2002b; Filoramo and Janzen, 2002). At other sites or in other years, microhabitat characteristics such as vegetation density and slope may modify the relationship between offspring traits and survival (e.g., Kolbe and Janzen, 2001). Discrepancies in experimental studies of phenotypic selection during early-life dispersal indicate that there may be no straightforward relationships. Although our study reveals no effect of treatment, sex, or body size, the incubation conditions and post-hatching handling of turtles in the laboratory or field may help to explain the lack of consistent patterns and variability between studies. The ontogeny of neonatal turtles in the nest is likely to encompass a complex interaction between genetics, maternal effects, sex, incubation temperature, hatching date, and post-hatching environmental conditions, making general comparisons difficult between species and populations.

This study provides insight into the evolution and maintenance of hatching synchrony in turtles, yet cannot strongly refute either the multiple origins or phylogenetic inertia interpretation of Colbert et al. (2010). Consistent with the inertia hypothesis, our data reject that shortening development time to hatch synchronously reduces hatching success. In addition, we found no evidence that reduced performance in the laboratory translated into reduced fitness in a simulated post-hatching nest environment or during terrestrial dispersal. Finally, although the distribution of hatching synchrony in turtles is poorly understood, the trait has been identified in multiple families and both megaorders of turtles, lending support to a single, basal origin of this phenomenon. Future work should focus on hatching synchrony in other turtle taxa to more firmly resolve these two hypotheses in a formal phylogenetic comparative analysis.

The consequences of environmentally-induced hatching more generally constitute an emerging field of research (Sih and Moore, 1993; Blaustein, 1997; Warkentin, 2005; Doody, 2011; Spencer and Janzen, 2011). Our experiment is novel in linking elements of physiology, ecology, and evolution to elucidate proximate and ultimate functions of synchronous hatching. As further studies are conducted on diverse taxa, we not only will better elucidate evolutionary costs and benefits but also will gain greater insight into physiological mechanisms and ecological dynamics, ultimately leading to a broader understanding of environmentally-induced traits.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by Iowa State University IACUC protocol 6-04-5684-DJ.

Author contributions

The authors confirm contribution to the paper as follows: study conception and design: PC, RS, FJ; data collection: PC, RS, FJ; analysis and interpretation of results: PC, RS, FJ; manuscript preparation: PC, RS, FJ. All authors reviewed the results and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by National Science Foundation grants DEB-0089680 and DEB-0640932 awarded to FJ, including salary for RS and PC, along with data collection, analysis, and initial write-up of the findings. Publication fees were supported by Iowa State University as a Frontiers institutional member.

Acknowledgments

We thank R. Paitz for assistance in nest location and egg collection, Y. Ortiz and K. Birk for help in setting up and maintaining experimental treatments, K. Bowen and S. Titterington for aid in measuring offspring morphology and performance, K. Birk, L. Carroll, E. Myers, Y. Ortiz, and M. Reida for toil on the drift fence, and the rest of the Janzen Lab and Turtle Camp crews for support. As always, we are indebted to the U.S. Army Corps of Engineers and the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service for granting us access to lands under their purview. J. DeVille, M. Haussmann, and J. Houy provided stimulating discussion over the course of this research, and N. Fry proved an invaluable asset for relief from the fieldwork. We are also grateful to A. Bronikowski, J. Chisham, D. Warner, and H. Wickham for their advice and assistance with statistical analyses, and K. Bowen, numerous Janzen Lab members, and reviewers for constructive comments on drafts of this manuscript. Eggs were collected under Illinois DNR permit NH04.0073 and experiments were performed in accordance with IACUC protocol 6-04-5684-DJ from Iowa State University.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Andrews R. M. (2004). “Patterns of embryonic development,” in Reptilian incubation: Environment, evolution and behaviour. Editor D. C. Deeming (Nottingham: Nottingham University Press), 75

Google Scholar

Arnold S. J., Wassersug R. J. (1978). Differential predation on metamorphic anurans by garter snakes (Thamnophis): social behavior as a possible defense. Ecology 59, 1014–1022. doi:10.2307/1938553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balasz G. H., Ross E. (1974). Observations on the pre-emergence behavior of the green turtle. Copeia 1974, 986. doi:10.2307/1442606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blaustein L. (1997). Non-consumptive effects of larval salamandra on crustacean prey: can eggs detect predators? Oecologia 110, 212–217. doi:10.1007/s004420050152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burger J. (1976). Behavior of hatchling diamondback terrapins (Malaclemys terrapin) in the field. Copeia 1976, 742. doi:10.2307/1443457

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr A., Hirth H. (1961). Social facilitation in green turtle siblings. Anim. Behav. 9, 68–70. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(61)90051-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter A. L., Andrews R., Bodensteiner B. L., Iverson J. B., Milne-Zelman C. L., Mitchell T. S., et al. (2019). Breadth of the thermal response captures individual and geographic variation in temperature-dependent sex determination. Funct. Ecol. 33, 1928–1939. doi:10.1111/1365-2435.13410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colbert P. L. (2006). “Hatching synchrony and survival in early life in the painted turtle, Chrysemys picta,”. M.S. Thesis (Ames, Iowa, USA: Iowa State University).

Google Scholar

Colbert P. L., Spencer R.-J., Janzen F. J. (2010). Mechanism and cost of synchronous hatching. Funct. Ecol. 24, 112–121. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2009.01602.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Congdon J. D., Nagle R. D., Dunham A. E., Beck C. W., Kinney O. M., Yeomans S. R. (1999). The relationship of body size to survivorship of hatchling snapping turtles (Chelydra serpentina): an evaluation of the ‘bigger is better’ hypothesis. Oecologia 121, 224–235. doi:10.1007/s004420050924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deeming D. C., Ferguson M. W. J. (1991). “Physiological effects of incubation temperature on embryonic development in reptiles and bird,” in Egg incubation: Its effects on embryonic development in birds and reptiles. Editors D. C. Deeming, and M. W. J. Ferguson (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehn M. M. (1990). Vigilance for predators: detection and dilution effects. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 26, 337

Google Scholar

Delmas V., Baudry E., Girondot M., Prevot-Julliard A.-C. (2007). The righting response as a fitness index in freshwater turtles. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 91, 99–109. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2007.00780.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doody J. S. (2011). Environmentally cued hatching in reptiles. Integr. Comp. Biol. 51, 49–61. doi:10.1093/icb/icr043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernst C. H., Lovich J. E. (2009). Turtles of the United States and Canada. second edition. Baltimore, Maryland: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Google Scholar

Field A., McGlashan J. K., Salmon M. (2021). Evidence for synchronous hatching in marine turtle (Caretta caretta) embryos and its influence on the timing of nest emergence. Chelonian Conserv. Biol. 20, 173–183. doi:10.2744/ccb-1489.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filoramo N. I., Janzen F. J. (2002). An experimental study of the influence of embryonic water availability, body size, and clutch on survivorship of neonatal red-eared sliders, Trachemys scripta elegans. Herpetologica 58, 67–74. doi:10.1655/0018-0831(2002)058[0067:aesoti]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frazer N. B., Gibbons J. W., Greene J. L. (1991). Growth, survivorship and longevity of painted turtles Chrysemys picta in a southwestern Michigan marsh. Am. Midl. Nat. 125, 245. doi:10.2307/2426229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freedberg S., Ewert M. A., Nelson C. E. (2001). Environmental effects on fitness and consequences for sex allocation in a reptile with environmental sex determination. Evol. Ecol. Res. 3, 953

Google Scholar

Freedberg S., Stumpf A. L., Ewert M. A., Nelson C. E. (2004). Developmental environment has long-lasting effects on behavioral performance in two turtles with environmental sex determination. Evol. Ecol. Res. 6, 739

Google Scholar

Gibbons J. W., Nelson D. H. (1978). The evolutionary significance of delayed emergence from the nest by hatchling turtles. Evolution 32, 297–303. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1978.tb00645.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harms H. K., Paitz R. T., Bowden R. M., Janzen F. J. (2005). Age and season impact resource allocation to eggs and nesting behavior in the painted turtle. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 78, 996–1004. doi:10.1086/432920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houghton J. D. R., Hays G. C. (2001). Asynchronous emergence by loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta) hatchlings. Naturwissenschaften 88, 133–136. doi:10.1007/s001140100212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irschick D. J., Herrel A., Vanhooydonck B., Huyghe K., Van Damme R. (2005). Locomotor compensation creates a mismatch between laboratory and field estimates of escape speed in lizards: a cautionary tale for performance-to-fitness studies. Evol. 59, 1579. doi:10.1554/04-733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson J. B. (1991b). Life history and demography of the yellow mud turtle, kinosternon flavescens. Herpetologica 47, 373

Google Scholar

Iverson J. B. (1991a). Patterns of survivorship in turtles (order Testudines). Can. J. Zool. 69, 385–391. doi:10.1139/z91-060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janzen F. J., Tucker J. K., Paukstis G. L. (2007). Experimental analysis of an early life-history stage: direct or indirect selection on body size of hatchling turtles? Funct. Ecol. 21, 162–170. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2006.01220.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janzen F. J. (1993). An experimental analysis of natural selection on body size of hatchling turtles. Ecology 74, 332–341. doi:10.2307/1939296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janzen F. J., Paukstis G. L. (1991). Environmental sex determination in reptiles: ecology, evolution, and experimental design. Q. Rev. Biol. 66, 149–179. doi:10.1086/417143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janzen F. J., Tucker J. K., Paukstis G. L. (2000b). Experimental analysis of an early life-history stage: avian predation selects for larger body size of hatchling turtles. J. Evol. Biol. 13, 947–954. doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.2000.00234.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janzen F. J., Tucker J. K., Paukstis G. L. (2000a). Experimental analysis of an early life-history stage: selection on size of hatchling turtles. Ecology 81, 2290–2304. doi:10.1890/0012-9658(2000)081[2290:eaoael]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolbe J. J., Janzen F. J. (2002b). Experimental analysis of an early life-history stage: water loss and migrating hatchling turtles. Copeia 2002, 220–226. doi:10.1643/0045-8511(2002)002[0220:eaoael]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolbe J. J., Janzen F. J. (2002a). Impact of nest-site selection on nest success and nest temperature in natural and disturbed habitats. Ecology 83, 269–281. doi:10.1890/0012-9658(2002)083[0269:ionsso]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolbe J. J., Janzen F. J. (2001). The influence of propagule size and maternal nest-site selection on survival and behaviour of neonate turtles. Funct. Ecol. 15, 772–781. doi:10.1046/j.0269-8463.2001.00587.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maloney J. E., Darian-Smith C., Takahashi Y., Limpus C. J. (1990). The environment for development of the embryonic loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta) in Queensland (Australia). Copeia 1990, 378. doi:10.2307/1446343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall D. J., Cook C. N., Emlet R. B. (2006). Offspring size effects mediate competitive interactions in a colonial marine invertebrate. Ecology 87, 214–225. doi:10.1890/05-0350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGlashan J. K., Thompson M. B., Janzen F. J., Spencer R.-J. (2018). Environmentally induced phenotypic plasticity explains hatching synchrony in the freshwater turtle Chrysemys picta. J. Exp. Zool. A Ecol. Integr. Physiol. 329, 362–372. doi:10.1002/jez.2217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell T. S., Myers E. M., Tucker J. K., McGaugh S. E. (2016). Righting ability in hatchling turtles does not predict survival during dispersal in the field. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 120, 553–562. doi:10.1111/bij.12896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morjan C. L. (2003). Variation in nesting patterns affecting nest temperatures in two populations of painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) with temperature-dependent sex determination. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 53, 254–261. doi:10.1007/s00265-002-0570-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy K. M., Bodensteiner B. L., Delaney D. M., Strickland J. T., Janzen F. J. (2020). Nest temperatures predict nest emergence of painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) offspring. Chelonian Conserv. Biol. 19, 72. doi:10.2744/ccb-1391.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers E. M., Tucker J. K., Chandler C. H. (2007). Experimental analysis of body size and shape during critical life-history events of hatchling slider turtles, Trachemys scripta elegans. Funct. Ecol. 21, 1106–1114. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2007.01337.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilsson J.-A., Persson I. (2004). Postnatal effects of incubation length in mallard and pheasant chicks. Oikos 105, 588–594. doi:10.1111/j.0030-1299.2004.12594.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paitz R. T., Harms H. K., Bowden R. M., Janzen F. J. (2007). Experience pays: offspring survival increases with female age. Biol. Lett. 3, 44–46. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratte H. T. (1985). “Temperature and insect development,” in Environmental physiology and biochemistry of insects. Editor K. H. Hoffmann (Berlin: Springer-Verlag), 33–66.

Google Scholar

Riley J. L., Hudson S., Frenette-Ling C., Davy C. M. (2020). All together now! hatching synchrony in freshwater turtles. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 74, 58. doi:10.1007/s00265-020-2800-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santos R. G., Pinheiro H. T., Martins A. S., Riul P., Bruno S. C., Janzen F. J., et al. (2016). The anti-predator role of within-nest emergence synchrony in sea turtle hatchlings. Proc. Biol. Sci. 283, 20160697. doi:10.1098/rspb.2016.0697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

SAS Institute Inc (2004). JMP user’s guide 5.1.2. Cary, North Carolina: SAS Institute Inc.

Google Scholar

SAS Institute Inc (2003). SAS OnlineDoc® 9.1. Cary, North Carolina: SAS Institute Inc.

Google Scholar

Sih A., Moore R. D. (1993). Delayed hatching of salamander eggs in response to enhanced larval predation risk. Am. Nat. 142, 947–960. doi:10.1086/285583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer R.-J., Janzen F. J. (2010). Demographic consequences of adaptive growth and the ramifications for conservation of long-lived organisms. Biol. Conserv. 143, 1951–1959. doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2010.04.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer R.-J., Janzen F. J. (2011). Hatching behavior in turtles. Integr. Comp. Biol. 51, 100–110. doi:10.1093/icb/icr045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer R.-J., Thompson M. B., Banks P. B. (2001). Hatch or wait? a dilemma in reptilian incubation. Oikos 93, 401–406. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0706.2001.930305.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Standing K. L., Herman T. B., Morrison I. P. (1999). Nesting ecology of Blanding’s turtle (Emydoidea blandingii) in Nova Scotia, the northeastern limit of the species’ range. Can. J. Zool. 77, 1609–1614. doi:10.1139/z99-122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steyermark A. C., Spotila J. R. (2001a). Body temperature and maternal identity affect snapping turtle (Chelydra serpentina) righting response. Copeia 2001, 1050–1057. doi:10.1643/0045-8511(2001)001[1050:btamia]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steyermark A. C., Spotila J. R. (2001b). Effects of maternal identity and incubation temperature on hatching and hatchling morphology in snapping turtles, Chelydra serpentina. Copeia 2001, 129–135. doi:10.1643/0045-8511(2001)001[0129:eomiai]2.0.co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Telemeco R. S., Gangloff E. J., Cordero G. A., Mitchell T. S., Bodensteiner B. L., Holden K. G., et al. (2016). Reptile embryos lack the opportunity to thermoregulate by moving within the egg. Am. Nat. 188, E13–E27. doi:10.1086/686628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson M. B. (1997). “Egg physiology and biology,” in The biology, husbandry, and healthcare of reptiles. Editors L. Ackerman, 88

Google Scholar

Thompson M. B. (1988). Nest temperatures in the pleurodiran turtle, Emydura macquarii. Copeia 1988, 996. doi:10.2307/1445723

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker J. K. (2000). Body size and migration of hatchling turtles: Inter- and intraspecific comparisons. J. Herpetol. 34, 541. doi:10.2307/1565269

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker J. K. (1997). Natural history notes on nesting, nests, and hatchling emergence in the red-eared slider turtle, Trachemys scripta elegans, in west-central Illinois. Ill. Nat. Hist. Surv. Biol. Notes 140, 1–13.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker J. K., Paukstis G. L., Janzen F. J. (2008). Does predator swamping promote synchronous emergence of turtle hatchlings among nests? Behav. Ecol. 19, 35–40. doi:10.1093/beheco/arm097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

USGCRP (U.S. Global Change Research Program) Wuebbles D. J., Fahey D. W., Hibbard K. A., Dokken D. J., Stewart B. C., Maycock T. K. (2017). Climate science special report: Fourth national climate assessment, volume I (U.S. Global Change Research Program). doi:10.7930/J0J964J6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warkentin K. M. (2005). How do embryos assess risk? vibrational cues in predator-induced hatching of red-eyed treefrogs. Anim. Behav. 70, 59–71. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warner D. A., Miller D. A. W., Bronikowski A. M., Janzen F. J. (2016). Decades of field data reveal that turtles senesce in the wild. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 6502–6507. doi:10.1073/pnas.1600035113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warner D. A., Mitchell T. S., Bodensteiner B. L., Janzen F. J. (2020). Sex and incubation temperature independently affect embryonic development and offspring size in a turtle with temperature-dependent sex determination. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 93, 62–74. doi:10.1086/706786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warner D. A., Mitchell T. S. (2013). Does maternal oviposition site influence offspring dispersal to suitable habitat? Oecologia 172, 679–688. doi:10.1007/s00442-012-2533-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisrock D. W., Janzen F. J. (1999). Thermal and fitness-related consequences of nest location in painted turtles (Chrysemys picta). Funct. Ecol. 13, 94–101. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2435.1999.00288.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willette D. A. S., Tucker J. K., Janzen F. J. (2005). Linking climate and physiology at the population level for a key life-history stage of turtles. Can. J. Zool. 83, 845–850. doi:10.1139/z05-078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Chrysemys picta, embryonic development, hatching synchrony, hatching success, survival, predator avoidance

Citation: Colbert PL, Spencer R-J and Janzen FJ (2022) Developmental asynchrony might not reduce fitness in early life in painted turtles. Front. Physiol. 13:923912. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.923912

Received: 19 April 2022; Accepted: 11 July 2022;
Published: 26 August 2022.

Edited by:

Edward M. Dzialowski, University of North Texas, United States

Reviewed by:

Jose Pablo Vazquez-Medina, University of California, Berkeley, United States
Masaki Shirai, Central Research Institute of Electric Power Industry (CRIEPI), Japan

Copyright © 2022 Colbert, Spencer and Janzen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: F. J. Janzen, janzenf1@msu.edu

Present address: P. L. Colbert, Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Organismal Biology, Iowa State University, Ames, IA, United States; R.-J. Spencer, Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Organismal Biology, Iowa State University, Ames, IA, United States; F. J. Janzen, Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Organismal Biology, Iowa State University, Ames, IA, United States

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.