Skip to main content

METHODS article

Front. Pediatr., 16 January 2023
Sec. Neonatology
Volume 10 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2022.913396

Protocol for a prospective multicenter longitudinal randomized controlled trial (CALIN) of sensory-tonic stimulation to foster parent child interactions and social cognition in very premature infants

Cassandre Guittard1,† Alexandre Novo2,† Julien Eutrope3 Corinne Gower4 Coralie Barbe5 Nathalie Bednarek6 Anne-Catherine Rolland3 Stéphanie Caillies1 Gauthier Loron6*
  • 1Université Reims Champagne-Ardenne, C2S, Reims, France
  • 2CHU Nantes, Département de Psychiatrie, Les Apsyades, Nantes, France
  • 3Université de Reims Champagne-Ardenne, C2S, CHU Reims, Service de Pédopsychiatrie, Reims, France
  • 4CHU Reims, Unité d’Aide Méthodologique, Reims, France
  • 5Université de Reims Champagne-Ardenne, Research on Health University Department, C2S, Reims, France
  • 6Université de Reims Champagne-Ardenne, CReSTIC, CHU Reims, Service de Médecine Néonatale et de Réanimation Pédiatrique, Reims, France

Introduction: Premature birth is associated with long-term somatic and neurological disorders, including cognitive, social and behavioral impairments. Moreover, the mothers of infants born preterm exhibit a higher prevalence of anxiety and depressive symptoms after birth. Early rehabilitation, developmental care, and parenting support have already been shown to have a positive impact on neurological outcome. However, no randomized controlled study has so far assessed the effects on parenting and long-term neurological outcomes of proprioceptive stimulation to trigger positive brain plasticity in very preterm babies. The CALIN project will therefore investigate the impact of sensory-tonic stimulation (STS) of extremely preterm infants by their parents on child parent interactions, infants' morphological and functional brain development and subsequent cognition (including social cognition), and parents' anxiety and depressive symptoms in the postpartum period.

Methods and analysis: Infants born between 25 and 32 weeks of gestation will be randomly assigned to the “STS + Kangaroo care” or “Kangaroo care” group. The primary endpoint, child and parent interactions, will be rated at 12 months corrected age using the Coding Interactive Behavior system. Secondary endpoints include: 1/functional and anatomical brain maturation sequentially assessed during neonatal hospitalization using electroencephalogram (EEG), amplitude-integrated EEG (aEEG), cranial ultrasound and MRI performed at term-corrected age, 2/social and cognitive outcomes assessed at 15 months, 2, 4 and 6 years, and 3/parents' anxiety and depressive symptoms assessed at 7 ± 1 weeks after birth, using dedicated questionnaires.

Ethics and dissemination: This study was approved by the French Ethics Committee for the Protection of Persons on 18 October 2021. It is registered with the French National Agency for the Safety of Medicines and Health Products (ANSM; no. 2020-A00382–37). The registry number on ClinicalTrials.gov is NCT04380051.

1. Introduction

1.1. Background

In 2018, the World Health Organization (WHO) estimated that 15 million children are born prematurely each year worldwide. The number of newborns surviving a premature birth has gradually increased over the years (1, 2). However, many of those will exhibit varying degree of neurological impairment. In the recent decades, the clinical and morphological picture of brain insults due to prematurity has changed. Classically, extensive clastic injuries (e.g., cystic necrosis of white matter, large infarction of brain parenchyma) most often led to cerebral palsy and/or moderate to severe cognitive impairment. Since the 1990s, however, their incidence has dwindled, and they have been supplanted by less prominent and more diffuse brain damage leading to the loss of vulnerable cells and impaired brain development (35).

Nowadays, the spectrum of neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm babies broadly includes deficits in language, gross and fine motor skills, behavior, and cognition (especially executive functions and social cognition). The severity of the clinical picture is related to the degree of prematurity (612). Many brain areas undergo a sensitive period of development during the third trimester of gestation and after birth, during which sensory input shapes neurological maturation and future function. This is the case of the median prefrontal cortex, temporoparietal junction and posterior temporal sulcus (involved in cognition and social cognition) (1315), the cerebellum, as well as visual, olfactory and somatosensory pathways (16). Premature birth dramatically modifies the context of this developmental window. During hospitalization, the developing brains of infants born preterm are exposed to stimuli that may be detrimental to their maturation (17) with either too much or too little sensorial input (18). This is referred to as dystimulation.

Furthermore, premature birth disturbs early infant-parent interactions, and ultimately relationships with others. Preterm infants are not only separated from their parents, but placed in a stressful, technical, and potentially painful environment. Comorbidities and sedation have a negative physiological impact and reduce their availability for interaction. On the parental side, the idealized postnatal period is replaced by an anxious–and even traumatic–experience (1921). Parents often express guilt and anxiety about the survival of their child and their parenting skills. A higher prevalence of parental anxiety, postnatal depression and posttraumatic stress disorder has been observed in the mothers of infants born preterm (22), even up to 18 months after birth (23).

Finally, prematurity severely disturbs the ability of both parents and newborns to interact and find reassurance. More than 35% of children born preterm subsequently exhibit insecure attachment behavior in relationships with others (24). Given that studies have demonstrated a longitudinal link between attachment security and cognitive development (2526), specially social cognition (27, 28), it seems important to study the effects of disturbed early interactions on child development, and to test the potential of intervention programs to efficiently minimize these effects.

Developmental care has been implemented in many neonatal intensive care units (NICUs) to overcome the difficulties described above. The “Kangaroo mother care” and the “Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program” (NIDCAP) are among the most studied and practiced in the world. Fundamentally, those approaches place the family at the center of the newborn care, promoting affective contact with parents, and aiming at reducing dystimulation and sleep wake cycle interruptions (29). Those methods have shown positive effects on weight gain, sleep wake cycle interruptions electroencephalographic (EEG) activity, duration of hospitalization, some items of long term cognitive development and maternal anxio-depressive symptomatology (3034). The involvement of parents in care is associated with many benefits: for the parents, it enhances the parenting process, their perception of their child, their sense of competence, and their attachment; for the child, it has a positive impact on cognitive and motor development, on executive function disorders and psychological disorders (31, 3537).

Several teams are now investigating whether the focused enrichment of infants' sensorial input provides an additional effect to developmental care. Exposure to sounds such as maternal heartbeat, voices and music are promising avenues (3842). Some studies have documented effect of somesthetic stimulation (43, 44). These interventions variably associate skin contact, massage (sliding on the skin), kinesthetic stimulation (movement of the child's limbs) and positioning. In most experimental protocols, those interventions have been performed one to three times a day, after feeding, during 5 to 10 days, on moderately preterm babies (39). Those interventions were associated with an increase in weight gain, lymphocyte natural killer activity (associated with a reduction of the incidence of late neonatal sepsis), as well as a decrease in pain response. A reduced length of stay in hospital and a better developmental score at 12 and 24 months were reported (434547). However, studies evaluating longitudinally the effectiveness of early tactile and kinesthetic interventions on several key aspects of extremely preterm children development are still sparse.

1.2. Objectives and hypotheses

The overall aim of this study is to assess effect of a sensori-tonic stimulation (STS) provided by parents, associated with kangaroo care, vs. kangaroo care alone, on the quality of parent-infant interactions, parental wellbeing, and ultimately brain maturation and cognitive outcome of infants born preterm.

1.2.1. Primary objective

This study will investigate the benefits of early STS, provided by one of the parents on its infant born preterm on the developing interactions between them. We expect to observe stronger interactions in the dyads who practiced STS + kangaroo care, than in the control group (kangaroo care only).

1.2.2. Secondary objectives

The present work will assess, in children born very preterm, the effect of “STS + kangaroo care” vs. “kangaroo care” alone on: 1/the morphological and functional brain maturation, 2/the precursors of cognitive development at 15 months, 3/psychomotor development at 2 and 4 years, and 4/cognition including social cognition assessed at 6 years of age.

Moreover, this study will assess the impact of the intervention on symptoms of anxiety and depression experienced by the parents of children born preterm.

2. Methods

2.1. Design

The CALIN study is a prospective, multicentric randomized study. The clinicians who carry out the assessments will be blind to the result of the randomization. Participants will be recruited in French tertiary neonatal centers equipped with Neonatal Intensive Care Units.

2.2. Participants

2.2.1. Study population

Children born between very preterm and their parents will be eligible if they meet the following inclusion criteria: 1/the child is inborn, 2/birth between 25 and 32 weeks of gestation and weight over 600 g at birth, 3/the child's hospitalization at the recruiting center is planned until 36 weeks corrected age, 4/the child's parents have parental authority and have agreed to participate in the study by signing the informed consent, 5/the child's parents are available physically and mentally to participate in the study and 6/the child and his/her parents are affiliated to the social security system.

The exclusion criteria are the following: 1/the child is hemodynamically unstable, 2/the child has a suspected comorbidity (e.g., genetic syndrome, congenital malformation, brain injury, skin pathology), 3/the child was born from a multiple pregnancy, 4/the child was born anonymously, 5/the child is to be separated from his/her parents (e.g., assigned to infancy protection services).

If the child's condition does not allow the practice of STS (for example: hemodynamic compromise, enterocolitis), the intervention may be temporarily discontinued until the condition improves. The discontinuation and the resumption of the intervention are decided by the medical staff in charge of the child, who informs and explains the decision to the parents. Newborns will not be excluded from the intervention group.

2.2.2. Sample size calculation

The sample size has been calculated so that a two-tailed test will detect a significant difference in the primary endpoint (Coding infant behavior scale at 12 months corrected age, see below), with a power of 90% and a significance level of 0.05 (alpha value). Forty-eight infants would be required in each group (NQuery 4.0® software). In anticipation of loss to follow-up, the target size has been increased by 20%, such that 60 children will be included in each group, resulting in inclusion of a total of 120 infants.

2.2.3. Randomization

The newborns and their parents will be randomly assigned to either the experimental group or the control group. Those in the experimental group will participate in the STS intervention, in addition to kangaroo care. The control group will benefit from kangaroo care alone. Besides that, the medical and nursing care will be the same, regardless of the outcome of the randomization.

2.3. Intervention

Sensory-tonic stimulation (STS) refers to a protocolized, tactile, vestibular and kinesthetic stimulation, provided by a parent to her/his baby born preterm. This proprioceptive stimulation is carried out using an enveloping and continuous touch with a moderate pressure, as described in the literature (48, 49).

STS will be adapted to the newborn's term and level of development, broadly stratified into three stages (25-30, 31–34 and 35–36 corrected age, respectively). The preterm infant is either installed in a microbead baby nest in the incubator or on the changing table, depending on the newborn's usual arrangement and maturity. This position facilitates the sensory-tonic care, the observation of the behavior of the newborn, her/his signs of comfort and discomfort, and her/his state of alertness. At each stage, the stimulation begins with a two-handed enveloping contact with the child lying on his or her back: one hand on the trunk, the other hand supporting the pelvis, with the legs folded. Then, the hands slowly slide under the child's head/neck and pelvis, to lift the child and perform vestibular stimulation with gentle rocking motions. As the child matures, the parent adds hand movements along the child's limbs, then slow flexion-extension movements. The approximate duration of each step is between fifteen seconds and one minute, depending on the baby's tolerance and the step considered. Moving on to each next step requires a favorable state of alertness and the absence of signs of discomfort. As much as possible, the contact and gentle pressure between the hands and the baby's skin are continuously maintained. At all times, if signs of discomfort are observed: a comfortable and calming position is maintained (usually lateral roll-up).

Parents will be coached by a trained, designated professional, initially using a mannequin. They will also be shown a training video and given an illustrated training booklet covering all the steps of STS. They will begin from the 10th day after birth and when the parents feel ready to perform it and will continue until discharge from hospital or 36 weeks corrected age. STS should be carried out 5 times per week, 5-15 min each time. STS will be performed when the newborn exhibits quiet alertness (State 3 or 4 according to Brazelton (50)). The intervention will be interrupted if signs of discomfort are detected in the newborn or his/her parents. Once a week, a trained psychologist or psychomotor therapist will supervise the parent's gestures and discuss his/her experience.

“Skin to skin care” will be practiced without any restriction in both groups, following local policies. In the experimental group, newborns and their parents will participate in STS in addition to kangaroo care. The time, number, duration of each skin-to-skin care or STS will be collected, as well as somatic stability, initial and maximal state of alertness of the baby (51).

Preterm babies will benefit from the usual neonatal intensive care, following the local policies of each center.

2.4. Assessments and outcomes

An overview of the different assessments and outcomes is provided in Figure 1.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Design of the study, overview of assessments and outcomes.

2.4.1. Primary outcome

The quality of interactions between the very premature infants and their parents will be scored at 12 months corrected age using the Coding Infant Behavior system (CIB) (52). The latter assesses the quality of child-parent interactions by observing the occurrence of behaviors along six dimensions: parental sensitivity, parental intrusion, child's social engagement, child's negative emotionality and engagement, dyadic reciprocity, and negative dyad states. This scale is validated from birth to 3 years of age and has shown a great sensitivity in scoring interactions in many social and/or pathological situations (53). Early CIB scores have previously been correlated with cognitive development (54). We will record two 15 min sequences of child-parent interactions during free play. Observations will be double-quoted by professionals trained to use the CIB, blinded to the randomization group. The main judgment criteria will be the score in “child's social engagement” dimension of the CIB system. The Secondary judgment criteria will be the scores in the other dimensions of the CIB system.

2.4.2. Secondary outcomes

2.4.2.1. Morphological and functional development of the brain

All the data yielded by the following paraclinical examinations will be anonymized and sent to the main investigating center for centralized review. They will be interpreted by two experts for each technique used, blinded to the randomization group.

Serial cerebral ultrasound scans will be performed during the infants' NICU stay, following a standardized protocol. Lesions will be classified: 1/according to the Papille classification for intraventricular hemorrhages, 2/according to the De Vries classification for periventricular leukomalacia (55). Moreover, several two-dimensional measurements will be performed on ultrasound scan to assess brain growth: interhemispheric distance, ventricular width, thickness of motor and somatosensory cortex, diameter of thalami and cerebellum.

Brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) will be performed at term-corrected age including a diffusion sequence, following a standardized protocol of acquisition. Imaging will be interpreted according to Kidokoro (56). Moreover, regional brain volumes will be quantified using semi-automatic, post-hoc segmentation.

EEG and amplitude-integrated EEG will be sequentially recorded. Timing of the EEG recordings and their interpretation will follow the guidelines of the French society of electrophysiology (57).

2.4.2.2. Sequential assessment of cognition and psychomotor development

Precursors to cognitive development will be assessed at 15 months corrected age using the “Batterie d’Évaluation Cognitive et Socio-émotionnelle” (58). This psychometric tool assesses cognitive and socio-emotional development in young children up to 24 months, based on standardized activities and standardized sequences of interactions between the psychologist and the child.

Child development at 2 and 4 years will be measured using the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ 24 and ASQ 48) (59). Those questionnaires, filled by parents, include 30 items assessing five areas of development: communication, gross motor skills, fine motor skills, problem solving, and individual or social skills. A French, validated translation is available, featuring age-appropriate questions corresponding to the child's expected level of development and normative data.

Cognitive and social development will be assessed at 6 years using an extensive set of neuropsychological tests and theory of mind tasks: 1/Global intellectual abilities will be assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-V). This scale has five main indices: Verbal Comprehension, Visual Spatial, Fluid Reasoning, Working Memory, and Processing Speed and allows calculation of a global intellectual quotient. 2/To assess attention, inhibition and mental flexibility, The NEPSY's Auditory Attention and Associated Responses subtest will performed (60). 3/The NEPSY's Sentence Repetition subtest will be used to assesses phonological working memory abilities through a task where participants have to repeat sentences of increasing length and complexity (60). 4/The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-CH)'s Opposite Worlds subtest will attention and inhibition abilities through a pointing task in response to a verbal cue (61). 5/The Raven Matrix Test assesses nonverbal intellectual efficiency through a reasoning by analogy task (62). 6/Social cognition will be assessed using Cognitive and Affective theory of mind abilities tasks (6369).

2.4.2.3. Parental anxiety and depressive symptoms

Anxiety and depressive symptoms will be assessed in both parents at 7 weeks ± 1 after birth. Maternal anxiety and depressive symptoms will be measured using the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-Y), Parental Stress Index (PSI), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale, and Modified Perinatal Post Traumatic Stress Disorder Questionnaire. As some of these scales are not suitable for rating fathers, they will be replaced with non-gender specific scales. Paternal anxiety and depressive symptoms will therefore be measured using the STAI-Y, PSI, BDI, and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist for DSM-5 (7075).

2.5. Data analysis

First, we will carry out a descriptive analysis of the data, with means and standard deviations, and counts and percentage for the quantitative and qualitative variables, respectively. For all subsequent analysis, the group (experimental vs. control) will be the independent variable. The scores obtained on each scale will be the dependent variable.

2.5.1. Statistical analysis of primary outcome

The main judgment criteria (score in the “child's social engagement” dimension of the CIB at 12 months) will be compared between the experimental group and the control group using a Student or Mann-Whitney test, depending on the application conditions.

Secondary judgment criteria (scores in the other dimensions of the CIB at 12 months) will be compared between the experimental group and the control group using a Student or Mann-Whitney test, depending on the application conditions.

2.5.2. Statistical analysis of secondary outcomes

All secondary outcomes are quantitative measures. For all these measures, we will compare the scores of the experimental group with those of the control group, according to the distribution of the data, with parametric or nonparametric tests (Student's t, Mann-Whitney, chi2 or Fisher's exact tests), depending on the application conditions. Correlations will be calculated between quantitative measures.

A p value of < 0.05 will be considered statistically significant.

Statistical analysis will be performed with software “R®”(76).

3. Discussion

Although prematurity has been identified as a public health problem by the WHO since 2010, studies assessing the efficiency of early intervention programs are still sparse.

Developmental care and the kangaroo method (also called skin-to-skin care) have been implemented in many neonatal intensive care units (NICUs). These programs aim to reduce dystimulation, sleep-wake cycle interruptions and enhance affective contact with parents. They have been shown to have many positive effects on weight gain, somatic development, electroencephalographic (EEG) activity (77), medical complications, and duration of hospitalization (30, 78, 79).

Several teams are now investigating whether the focused enrichment of infants' sensory environment can improve sensory input and, ultimately, brain development. Massage and exposure to sounds such as maternal heartbeat, voices and music are promising avenues that are currently being explored (39, 40, 80, 81).

The present study will focus on sensory-tonic stimulation (STS). Complementing the standard care designed to protect premature newborns from all the usual dystimulation, the purpose of this STS intervention will be to improve proprioceptive input, which is generally lacking in NICUs. The sensorimotor cortex exhibits activity-dependent plasticity during this crucial period of development (82, 83). In the past, tactile stimulation of very premature infants in the form of massage has had a positive impacts on physical growth (e.g., weight, height) (48, 84), electroencephalographic (EEG) activity maturation [35,37], sleep quality and stress behaviors (e.g., crying, motor agitation) (43, 48), physiological measurements (e.g., IGF binding protein-3, glucose, insulin, cortisol and thyroid hormones) (84), and early psychomotor development (85, 86). Interestingly, this kind of intervention may also have a positive impact on parents’ moods and skills. Tactile, tender and affectionate contact increases plasma levels of oxytocin and endorphins (associated with feelings of wellbeing and happiness) in both the infants and their parents (43). However, these studies are based on different protocols, sample sizes, participants (parents vs. caregivers), and stimulation duration.

One of the strengths of the CALIN study is to consider different dimensions of the child's development within its family, analyzing the parents' mood, early interactions, psychomotor and long-term cognitive development, and brain maturation. These different dimensions seem to interact with each other over time.

We are aware that the CALIN protocol has potential limitations and biases. We may be challenged by potential variability in stimulation time if parents continue to practice STS after the infant has left hospital. We also expect to a risk of loss to follow-up due to the longitudinal design of this study.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT04380051.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by French Ethics Committee for the Protection of Persons.

Author contributions

GL, AN and CG conceived the study and coordinated its design. CG drafted the manuscript. SC supervised the writing of the manuscript. CB and CG helped with implementation of the study. GL is the principal investigator of the protocol. The lead investigators are CG, GL, JE and AN. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The present research was funded by the French Interregional Clinical Research Hospital Program (PHRC-I). C. Guittard's PhD research is funded by the American Committee of the American Memorial Hospital of Reims.

Acknowledgments

We thank the American Committee for supporting the activities of the American Memorial Hospital in Reims for over 100 years. We thank the families who will participate in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Larroque B, Ancel PY, Marret S, Marchand L, André M, Arnaud C, et al. Neurodevelopmental disabilities and special care of 5-year-old children born before 33 weeks of gestation (the EPIPAGE study): a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet. (2008) 371(9615):813–20. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60380-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Ancel PY, Goffinet F. and the EPIPAGE-2 Writing Group. Survival and morbidity of preterm children born at 22 through 34 Weeks’ gestation in France in 2011: results of the EPIPAGE-2 cohort study. JAMA Pediatr. (2015) 169(3):230–8. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3351

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Penn AA, Gressens P, Fleiss B, Back SA, Gallo V. Controversies in preterm brain injury. Neurobiol Dis. (2016) 92:90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.10.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Volpe JJ. The encephalopathy of prematurity–brain injury and impaired brain development inextricably intertwined. Semin Pediatr Neurol. (2009) 16(4):167–78. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2009.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Back SA. Brain injury in the preterm infant: new horizons for pathogenesis and prevention. Pediatr Neurol. (2015) 53(3):185–92. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2015.04.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Anderson PJ, Doyle LW. Executive functioning in school-aged children who were born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight in the 1990s. Pediatr. (2004) 114(1):50–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.114.1.50

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Arpi E, Ferrari F. Preterm birth and behaviour problems in infants and preschool-age children: a review of the recent literature. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2013) 55(9):788–96. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12142

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Johnson S, Hollis C, Kochhar P, Hennessy E, Wolke D, Marlow N. Psychiatric disorders in extremely preterm children: longitudinal finding at age 11 years in the EPICure study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2010) 49(5):453–463.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.02.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. MSc HM, Pitchford NJ, Hagger MS, Marlow N. Development of executive function and attention in preterm children: a systematic review. Dev Neuropsychol. (2009) 34(4):393–421. doi: 10.1080/87565640902964524

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Baron IS, Kerns KA, Müller U, Ahronovich MD, Litman FR. Executive functions in extremely low birth weight and late-preterm preschoolers: effects on working memory and response inhibition. Child Neuropsychol. (2012) 18(6):586–99. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2011.631906

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM, Anand KJS. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born PretermA meta-analysis. JAMA. (2002) 288(6):728–37. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.6.728

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Sun J, Mohay H, O’Callaghan M. A comparison of executive function in very preterm and term infants at 8 months corrected age. Early Hum Dev. (2009) 85(4):225–30. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.10.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Soto-Icaza P, Aboitiz F, Billeke P. Development of social skills in children: neural and behavioral evidence for the elaboration of cognitive models. Front. Neurosci. (2015) 9:333. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00333

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Saxe R, Kanwisher N. People thinking about thinking people: the role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.”. NeuroImage. (2003) 19(4):1835–42. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Saxe RR, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Scholz J, Pelphrey KA. Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children. Child Dev. (2009) 80(4):1197–209. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01325.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Stoodley CJ, Limperopoulos C. Structure-function relationships in the developing cerebellum: evidence from early-life cerebellar injury and neurodevelopmental disorders. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. (2016) 21(5):356–64. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2016.04.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Philbin MK, Ballweg DD, Gray L. The effect of an intensive care unit sound environment on the development of habituation in healthy avian neonates. Dev Psychobiol. (1994) 27(1):11–21. doi: 10.1002/dev.420270103

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Borghini A, Forcada-Guex M, Nix CM. XIII. Prématurité et interventions précoces [Internet]. Recherches en périnatalité. Presses Universitaires de France; (2014) [cited 2021 May 11]. Available at: https://www.cairn.info/recherches-en-perinatalite–9782130628545-page-247.htm

19. Lahouel-Zaier W. Impact de l’hospitalisation périnatale sur l’établissement du lien d’attachement entre le bébé et sa mère. Devenir. (2017) 29(1):27–44. doi: 10.3917/dev.171.0027

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Rosenblum LA, Andrews MW. Influences of environmental demand on maternal behavior and infant development. Acta Paediatr. (1994) 83(s397):57–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1994.tb13266.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Goutaudier N, Séjourné N, Bui É, Cazenave N, Chabrol H. L’accouchement prématuré: une naissance traumatique? Symptômes de stress posttraumatique et variables associées. Gynécologie Obstétrique & Fertilité. (2014) 42(11):749–54. doi: 10.1016/j.gyobfe.2014.05.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gangi S, Dente D, Bacchio E, Giampietro S, Terrin G, De Curtis M. Posttraumatic stress disorder in parents of premature birth neonates. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. (2013) 82:882–5. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.06.365

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Brunson E, Thierry A, Ligier F, Vulliez-Coady L, Novo A, Rolland AC, et al. Prevalences and predictive factors of maternal trauma through 18 months after premature birth: a longitudinal, observational and descriptive study. PLOS ONE. (2021) 16(2):e0246758. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0246758

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. López-Maestro M, Sierra-Garcia P, Diaz-Gonzalez C, Torres-Valdivieso MJ, Lora-Pablos D, Ares-Segura S, et al. Quality of attachment in infants less than 1500 g or less than 32weeks. Related factors. Early Hum Dev. (2017) 104:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2016.11.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Bernier A, Calkins SD, Bell MA. Longitudinal associations between the quality of mother–infant interactions and brain development across infancy. Child Dev. (2016) 87(4):1159–74. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Bornstein MH, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Maternal responsiveness and cognitive development in children. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (1989) 1989(43):49–61. doi: 10.1002/cd.23219894306

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wellman HM. Theory of mind: the state of the art. Eur J Deve Psychol. (2018) 15(6):728–55. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2018.1435413

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hughes C, Leekam S. What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review. Reflections and New Directions for Studies of Typical and Atypical Development. Social Deve. (2004) 13(4):590–619. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00285.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Als HB, McAnulty G. The newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program (NIDCAP) with kangaroo mother care (KMC): comprehensive care for preterm infants. Current Women's Health Rev. (2011) 7(3):288–301. doi: 10.2174/157340411796355216

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Westrup B, Kleberg A, Eichwald von K, Stjernqvist K, Lagercrantz H. A randomized, controlled trial to evaluate the effects of the newborn individualized developmental care and assessment program in a Swedish setting. Pediatr. (2000) 105(1):66–72. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.1.66

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Klein V, Zores-Koenig C, Dillenseger L, Langlet C, Escande B, Astruc D, et al. Changes of infant- and family-centered care practices administered to extremely preterm infants during implementation of the NIDCAP program. Front Pediatr. (2021) 9:718813. doi: 10.3389/fped.2021.718813

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Ohlsson A, Jacobs SE. NIDCAP: a systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials. PEDIATR. (2013) 131(3):e881–93. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-2121

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Aita M, De Clifford Faugère G, Lavallée A, Feeley N, Stremler R, Rioux É, et al. Effectiveness of interventions on early neurodevelopment of preterm infants: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Pediatr. (2021) 21(1):210. doi: 10.1186/s12887-021-02559-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Héon M, Aita M, Lavallée A, De Clifford-Faugère G, Laporte G, Boisvert A, et al. Comprehensive mapping of NICU developmental care nursing interventions and related sensitive outcome indicators: a scoping review protocol. BMJ Open. (2022) 12(1):e046807. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-046807

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Tessier R, Cristo M, Velez S, Giron M, de Calume ZF, Ruiz-Palaez JG, et al. Kangaroo mother care and the bonding hypothesis. Pediatr. (1998) 102(2):e17. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.2.e17

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Charpak N, Tessier R, Ruiz JG, Hernandez JT, Uriza F, Villegas J, et al. Twenty-year follow-up of kangaroo mother care versus traditional care. Pediatr. (2017) 139(1):e20162063. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2063

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Feldman R, Eidelman AI, Sirota L, Weller A. Comparison of skin-to-skin (kangaroo) and traditional care: parenting outcomes and preterm infant development. Pediatr. (2002) 110(1 Pt 1):16–26. doi: 10.1542/peds.110.1.16

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Gaden TS, Ghetti C, Kvestad I, Bieleninik Ł, Stordal AS, Assmus J, et al. Short-term music therapy for families with preterm infants: a randomized trial. Pediatr. (2022) 149(2):e2021052797. doi: 10.1542/peds.2021-052797

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Webb AR, Heller HT, Benson CB, Lahav A. Mother's voice and heartbeat sounds elicit auditory plasticity in the human brain before full gestation. PNAS. (2015) 112(10):3152–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414924112

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Lordier L, Meskaldji DE, Grouiller F, Pittet MP, Vollenweider A, Vasung L, et al. Music in premature infants enhances high-level cognitive brain networks. PNAS. (2019) 116(24):12103–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1817536116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Filippa M, Lordier L, De Almeida JS, Monaci MG, Adam-Darque A, Grandjean D, et al. Early vocal contact and music in the NICU: new insights into preventive interventions. Pediatr Res. (2020) 87(2):249–64. doi: 10.1038/s41390-019-0490-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Daneshvarfard F, Abrishami Moghaddam H, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Kongolo G, Wallois F, Mahmoudzadeh M. Neurodevelopment and asymmetry of auditory-related responses to repetitive syllabic stimuli in preterm neonates based on frequency-domain analysis. Sci Rep. (2019) 9(1):10654. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-47064-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Field T. Massage therapy research review. Complement Ther Clin Pract. (2016) 24:19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2016.04.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Álvarez MJ, Fernández D, Gómez-Salgado J, Rodríguez-González D, Rosón M, Lapeña S. The effects of massage therapy in hospitalized preterm neonates: a systematic review. Int J Nurs Stud. (2017) 69:119–36. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2017.02.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Wang L, He J, Zhang X. The efficacy of massage on preterm infants: a meta-analysis. Am J Perinatol. (2013) 30(09):731–8. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1332801

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Procianoy RS, Mendes EW, Silveira RC. Massage therapy improves neurodevelopment outcome at two years corrected age for very low birth weight infants. Early Hum Dev. (2010) 86(1):7–11. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.12.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Abdallah B, Badr LK, Hawwari M. The efficacy of massage on short and long term outcomes in preterm infants. Infant Behavior and Development. (2013) 36(4):662–9. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.06.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Field T, Diego MA, Hernandez-Reif M, Deeds O, Figuereido B. Moderate versus light pressure massage therapy leads to greater weight gain in preterm infants. Infant Behavior and Deve. (2006) 29(4):574–8. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.07.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Guzzetta A, D’acunto MG, Carotenuto M, Berardi N, Bancale A, Biagioni E, et al. The effects of preterm infant massage on brain electrical activity. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2011) 53(s4):46–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04065.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Brazelton TB, Nugent JK. Neonatal behavioral assessment scale. London: Cambridge University Press; (1995). 168 p.

51. Martinet M, Borradori Tolsa C, Rossi Jelidi M, Bullinger A, Perneger T, Pfister RE. Élaboration et validation de contenu d’une grille d’observation du comportement sensorimoteur du nouveau-né à l’usage du personnel soignant. Archives de Pédiatrie. (2013) 20(2):137–45. doi: 10.1016/j.arcped.2012.11.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Feldman R, Eidelman AI. Neonatal state organization, neuromaturation, mother-infant interaction, and cognitive development in small-for-gestational-age premature infants. Pediatrics. (2006) 118(3):e869–78. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Feldman R, Masalha S. Parent–child and triadic antecedents of children's Social competence: cultural specificity, shared process. Dev Psychol. (2010) 46(2):455–67. doi: 10.1037/a0017415

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J Child Psychol and Psychiatry. (2007) 48(3–4):329–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Guillot M, Chau V, Lemyre B. Routine imaging of the preterm neonatal brain. Paediatr Child Health. (2020) 25(4):249–55. doi: 10.1093/pch/pxaa033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kidokoro H, Neil JJ, Inder TE. New MR imaging assessment tool to define brain abnormalities in very preterm infants at term. Am J Neuroradiol. (2013) 34(11):2208–14. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3521

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lamblin MD, André M, Challamel MJ, Curzi-Dascalova L, d’Allest AM, De Giovanni E, et al. [Electroencephalography of the premature and term newborn. Maturational aspects and glossary]. Neurophysiol Clin. (1999) 29(2):123–219. doi: 10.1016/S0987-7053(99)80051-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Adrien JL. BECS: Batterie d’évaluation cognitive et socio-émotionnelle. ECPA, les Éditions du centre de psychologie appliquée. Paris (2007).

59. Squires J, Bricker D, Twombly E, Nickel R, Clifford J, Murphy K. Ages & stages English questionnaires, (ASQ-3): a parent-completed, child-monitoring system. MD: paul H: Baltimore (2009).

60. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp S. Tests d’évaluation de l’attention chez l’enfant (NEPSY)–classeur d’administration. Paris: ECPA (2003).

61. Manly T, Robertson I, Anderson V, Nimmo-Smith I, Lussier F, Flessas J. TEA-Ch: test d’Evaluation de l’Attention chez l’enfant. Éditions du Centre de psychologie appliquée (ECPA). Paris (2006).

62. Raven J, Court J, Raven J. Progressive matrices couleur. Paris: Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée (1998).

63. Hutchins TL, Bonazinga LA, Prelock PA, Taylor RS. Beyond false beliefs: the development and psychometric evaluation of the perceptions of Children's Theory of mind measure—experimental version (PCToMM-E). J Autism Dev Disord. (2008) 38(1):143–55. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0377-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Perner J, Wimmer H. “John thinks that mary thinks that…” attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. J Exp Child Psychol. (1985) 39(3):437–71. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Sullivan K, Zaitchik D, Tager-Flusberg H. Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs. Dev Psychol. (1994) 30(3):395–402. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.3.395

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Wimmer H, Perner J. Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's Understanding of deception. Cognition. (1983) 13(1):103–28. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Harwood MD, Farrar MJ. Conflicting emotions: the connection between affective perspective taking and theory of mind. Br J Deve Psychol. (2006) 24(2):401–18. doi: 10.1348/026151005X50302

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Parker JR, MacDonald CA, Miller SA. “John thinks that mary feels …” false belief in children across affective and physical domains. J Genet Psychol. (2007) 168(1):43–62. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.168.1.43-62

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Baron-Cohen S, O’Riordan M, Stone V, Jones R, Plaisted K. Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (1999) 29(5):407–18. doi: 10.1023/A:1023035012436

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Bruchon-Schweitzer M, Paulhan I. L’inventaire d’anxiété-trait et d’anxiété-état. Adaptation française et validation du STAI-Y de CD Spielberger. 1993.

71. Abidin RR. Parenting stress Index: manual, administration booklet, [and] research update. Pediatric Psychology Press, 2915 Idlewood Drive, Charlottesville, VA 22901 ($18; 1983.

72. Beck AT, Ward C, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. Beck depression inventory (BDI). Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1961) 4(6):561–71. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Adouard F, Glangeaud-Freudenthal NMC, Golse B. Validation of the Edinburgh postnatal depression scale (EPDS) in a sample of women with high-risk pregnancies in France. Arch Womens Ment Health. (2005) 8(2):89–95. doi: 10.1007/s00737-005-0077-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Pierrehumbert B, Borghini A, Forcada-Guex M, Jaunin L, Müller-Nix C, Ansermet F. Validation française d’un questionnaire de stress post-traumatique destiné aux parents d’enfants présentant un risque périnatal élevé. Annales Médico-Psychologiques, Revue Psychiatrique. (2004) 162(9):711–21. doi: 10.1016/j.amp.2003.10.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Ashbaugh AR, Houle-Johnson S, Herbert C, El-Hage W, Brunet A. Psychometric validation of the English and French versions of the posttraumatic stress disorder checklist for DSM-5 (PCL-5). PLoS ONE. (2016) 11(10):e0161645. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161645

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Stéfaniak N. easieR: A GUI R metapackage. 2018. Available at: https://theeasierproject.wordpress.com/.

77. Als H, Duffy FH, McAnulty G, Butler SC, Lightbody L, Kosta S, et al. NIDCAP Improves brain function and structure in preterm infants with severe intrauterine growth restriction. J Perinatol. (2012) 32(10):797–803. doi: 10.1038/jp.2011.201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Als H, Lawhon G, Duffy FH, McAnulty GB, Gibes-Grossman R, Blickman JG. Individualized developmental care for the very low-birth-weight preterm infant: medical and neurofunctional effects. JAMA. (1994) 272(11):853–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.1994.03520110033025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Brown LD, Heermann JA. The effect of developmental care on preterm infant outcome. Appl Nurs Res. (1997) 10(4):190–7. doi: 10.1016/S0897-1897(97)80572-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Dehaene-Lambertz G, Montavont A, Jobert A, Allirol L, Dubois J, Hertz-Pannier L, et al. Language or music, mother or mozart? Structural and environmental influences on infants’ language networks. Brain Lang. (2010) 114(2):53–65. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.09.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Beltrán MI, Dudink J, de Jong TM, Benders MJNL, van den Hoogen A. Sensory-based interventions in the NICU: systematic review of effects on preterm brain development. Pediatr Res. (2022) 92(1):47–60. doi: 10.1038/s41390-021-01718-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Hadders-Algra M. Early brain damage and the development of motor behavior in children: clues for therapeutic intervention? Neural Plast. (2001) 8(1–2):31–49. doi: 10.1155/NP.2001.31

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Ismail FY, Fatemi A, Johnston MV. Cerebral plasticity: windows of opportunity in the developing brain. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. (2017) 21(1):23–48. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2016.07.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Guzzetta A, Baldini S, Bancale A, Baroncelli L, Ciucci F, Ghirri P, et al. Massage accelerates brain development and the maturation of visual function. J Neurosci. (2009) 29(18):6042–51. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5548-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Pelc K, Daniel I, Wenderickx B, Dan B. Multicentre prospective randomised single-blind controlled study protocol of the effect of an additional parent-administered sensorimotor stimulation on neurological development of preterm infants: primebrain. BMJ Open. (2017) 7(12):e018084. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018084

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Cadet IG. Impact du massage sur les fonctions physio et psychologiques des bébés nés prématurément et sur leur développement. 2015 [cited 2021 Nov 15]; Available at: https://corpus.ulaval.ca/jspui/handle/20.500.11794/25807

Keywords: neurodevelopment, preterm, brain, proprioception, interactions, clinical trial, brain imaging, parenting (MeSH)

Citation: Guittard C, Novo A, Eutrope J, Gower C, Barbe C, Bednarek N, Rolland A-C, Caillies S and Loron G (2023) Protocol for a prospective multicenter longitudinal randomized controlled trial (CALIN) of sensory-tonic stimulation to foster parent child interactions and social cognition in very premature infants. Front. Pediatr. 10:913396. doi: 10.3389/fped.2022.913396

Received: 5 April 2022; Accepted: 15 December 2022;
Published: 16 January 2023.

Edited by:

Stéphane Marret Rouen University Hospital, France

Reviewed by:

Alexandre Netto, Santa Casa of Sao Paulo, Brazil
Aude Buil, Université Sorbonne Paris Cité, France
Claire Zores, Hôpitaux Universitaires de Strasbourg, France

© 2023 Guittard, Novo, Eutrope, Gower, Barbe, Bednarek, Rolland, Caillies and Loron. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gauthier Loron gloron@chu-reims.fr

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Specialty Section: This article was submitted to Neonatology, a section of the journal Frontiers in Pediatrics

Download