Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neuroergonomics, 19 April 2022
Sec. Cognitive Neuroergonomics
Volume 3 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnrgo.2022.870362

Cognitive Performances: The Role of Digit Ratio (D2:D4) With a Protective Factor for Anxiety

Sergio Rinella1 Simona Massimino1 Alessia Sorbello1 Vincenzo Perciavalle2 Marinella Coco1*
  • 1Section of Physiology, Department of Biomedical and Biotechnological Sciences, University of Catania, Catania, Italy
  • 2Department of Sciences of Life, Kore University of Enna, Enna, Italy

This study aimed to identify a possible correlation between the D2:D4 ratio and state and/or trait anxiety in adult healthy subjects and, if so, whether it exists any difference between men and women. In addition, we also wanted to observe whether there is a relationship between participants' age and state and/or trait anxiety. The research involved 125 subjects of both sexes, who were calculated the D2:D4 ratio and were administered the self-assessment questionnaire State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-Y). Results show that there are positive significant correlations between the D2:D4 ratio and score at state anxiety and trait anxiety, in the total sample. However, if men are examined separately from women, it can be observed that only men have a statistically significant relationship between D2:D4 ratios and state anxiety and trait anxiety. Moreover, about possible relations between the age of participants and state and trait anxiety, a significant negative relationship was observed, without differences between men and women. However, only subjects with a D2:D4 ratio ≥ 1, without differences between men and women, showed a statistically significant negative linear correlation between their age and their state and trait anxiety. The present data allow us to conclude that a low D2:D4 ratio (<1) represents a protective factor against anxiety in both men and women and that this protection seems likely to act throughout life.

Introduction

Prenatal exposure to sex hormones determines important effects on the development and organization of the brain, which, consequently, will also influence its behavior (Collear and Hines, 1995). Subjects with high levels of testosterone present themselves as more prone to take risks, impulsive, with greater aggressiveness, while subjects with low levels of testosterone present themselves as more prone to reasoning and the study of logical subjects (Manning et al., 2000; Kilduff et al., 2013).

Several markers can detect the effects of prenatal androgens (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005), however, among the less invasive measures, the digit ratio allows to estimate prenatal exposure to androgens. It appears detectable in human fetuses as early as 10–40 weeks of gestation and assumes stable levels around 2 years of age (Manning et al., 1998; Manning, 2002a; Malas et al., 2006). It expresses the ratio of the length of the index finger to the ring finger of the right hand (Neyse and Brañas-Garza, 2014) which is measured from the midpoint of the lower crease (where the finger joins the hand) to the tip of the finger (D2:D4).

Generally, the digit ratio shows positive correlations with typical female behaviors and negative correlations with typical male behaviors (Putz et al., 2004). Specifically, it was observed that D2:D4 ratios appear to correlate with certain personological factors such as neuroticism, agreeableness, and openness (Fink et al., 2004, 2006). Moreover, D2:D4 can be a predictive factor of success among high financial brokers (Coates et al., 2009), of success in admission at Italian Medical Schools (Coco et al., 2011), of best performances in sports such as basketball (Tester and Campbell, 2007), skiing (Manning, 2002b), and soccer (Manning and Taylor, 2001), and of aggressive behavior in professional soccer players (Perciavalle et al., 2013) and of the risky decision making (Massimino et al., 2019; Rinella et al., 2019).

Many studies have attempted to verify possible correlations between digit ratio indices and impulsivity, while there are far fewer studies that have explored a possible correlation between the D2:D4 ratio and state and trait anxiety. Usually, the onset of anxiety disorders coincides with puberty, a period when the production of sex hormones is greatest (Yonkers and Kidner, 2002). It is shown that females are more than two times as likely as males to be afflicted with mood disorders (Shear et al., 2000; Pigott, 2003; Kessler et al., 2005; Bekker and van Mens-Verhulst, 2007; Fabio and Caprì, 2019). This disparity has been documented worldwide (Seedat et al., 2009) and would seem to suggest a potential role for gonadal hormones in the onset of anxiety and depressive disorders. In fact, several studies have confirmed that women are more likely to experience mood disorders during puberty, menopause, perimenstrual periods, and postpartum (Ahokas et al., 2001; Parker and Brotchie, 2004; Douma et al., 2005; Solomon and Herman, 2009).

Also, in women, an association is shown between D2:D4 and anxious behavior in childhood (Williams et al., 2003), and also with neuroticism (Fink et al., 2004), considered a precursor to anxiety (Ehrler et al., 1999; Khan et al., 2005).

Moreover, the relationship between testosterone levels, anxiety disorders, and major depressive disorder in humans is evident in males with hypogonadism, who exhibit a significantly higher prevalence of anxiety disorders and major depressive disorder, compared to those with normal physiological levels of androgens (Shores et al., 2004; Zarrouf et al., 2009). Despite a few inconsistent reports, the majority of studies support the case that testosterone yields beneficial effects on mood in men, especially in those with lower than normal levels (McHenry et al., 2014).

Even in the animal world, it has been shown that rhesus monkeys show sexually dimorphic D2:D4, with males having higher ratios than females (Baxter et al., 2017), and male rats deprived of androgens because of perinatal castration display female-typical patterns of anxious behavior (Lucion et al., 1996).

Few studies have used the D2:D4 ratio to explore a possible correlation between state and trait anxiety. Among these, it is possible to mention two. The first one actually identified a correlation between the D2:D4 ratio and anxiety in male university students, females excluded (Evardone and Alexander, 2009). The second instead investigated male subjects suffering from anxiety disorders (de Bruin et al., 2004). The results, whose scores indicated high values of the D2:D4 ratio, allowed us to suppose that these subjects may have been exposed to lower levels of prenatal testosterone. However, in this study female subjects were not assessed. From these observations, we hypothesize that there is a difference between males and females and that a higher digit ratio correlates with an increase in perceived state and trait anxiety.

Therefore, the main purpose of this study was to identify a possible correlation between the D2:D4 ratio and state and/or trait anxiety in adult healthy subjects, and, if so, whether it exists any difference between men and women. In addition, we also wanted to observe whether there is a relationship between participants' age and state and/or trait anxiety.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

The experimental sample was composed of 125 healthy adult subjects, of both sexes (66 male and 59 female), of an age group between 20 and 71 years (mean = 31.9; SD = 8.7). Male subjects had a mean age of 33.7 years (±10.1 SD), whereas women had a mean age of 29.9 years (±6.4 SD).

Information was collected on the participant's general condition, health status, and whether they were taking any drugs or substances. Volunteers signed informed consent and were also informed of their right to privacy, non-recognition, and anonymity.

The measurements were performed in agreement with the ethical standards of the Helsinki Declaration and approved by the Ethics Committee Catania 1 (authorization n. 113/2018/PO).

Procedures

To perform the study, the D2:D4 ratio was measured and a psychological assessment test for measures anxiety was performed (STAI-Y).

- D2:D4 ratio: The participants' right hand was photocopied and the length of the fingers from the metacarpophalangeal crease to the fingertip was measured. It was observed that this fold appears around the 9th week of gestation and is one of the primary folds of the hand (Kimura et al., 1990).

- The D2:D4 ratio has only been determined from the right hand, as right-hand finger ratios show stronger gender differences and appear to be more sensitive to prenatal androgens (Williams et al., 2000; Manning et al., 2007).

- To measure the D2:D4 ratio, we used the practical recommendations suggested by Voracek et al. (2007) and those recently described by Coates and Hebert (2008). In soft tissues, care must be taken to distinguish regular folds from irregular or secondary folds. Irregular folds form later than regular folds, after the 11th week of gestation when the fingers begin to bend, disrupting the dermal surface. Participants' hand prints were measured to determine the D2:D4 ratio by one of the authors using calipers accurate to 0.2 mm.

- State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, 1983): It is composed of 40 items and 2 scales (20 items per scale). On the state scale, participants were asked to describe how they were feeling “right now, that is, at this moment.” Responses were on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 = “Not at all” to 4 = “Very Much So.” On the trait scale, participants were asked to describe how they “generally feel.” Responses were on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 = “Almost Never” to 4 = “Almost Always.” For example, state anxiety items include: “I am tense; I am worried” and “I feel calm; I feel secure.” Trait anxiety items include: “I worry too much over something that really doesn't matter” and “I am content; I am a steady person.” Scores on the 20 items were summed to provide an overall scale score. Higher scores are positively correlated with higher levels of anxiety. Its most current revision is Form Y. Internal consistency coefficients for the scale have ranged from 0.86 to 0.95; test-retest reliability coefficients have ranged from 0.65 to 0.75 over a 2-month interval. Considerable evidence attests to the construct and concurrent validity of the scale (Spielberger, 1989).

Statistical Analysis

Data were collected and averaged and then compared with the unpaired t-test (two-tailed); multiple linear regression and the correlation coefficient of Pearson were also calculated. Significance was set at p < 0.05. All descriptive statistics are described as mean _ SD. All analyses were performed by means of GraphPad Prism version 6.00 for Windows (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, California, USA).

Results

D2:D4 Ratio

Figure 1 shows the values of D2:D4 ratios measured by the participants in the study. On the left of the figure, it can be seen the mean values (±SD) observed in the entire sample, and separately for men and women. Instead, the distributions of observed values for the total sample, and separately for men and women, are shown on the right.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. On the left are the mean values of the D2:D4 ratios and on the right is the distribution of the observed values, both of the entire sample, and separately for men and women; **p < 0.01, moderate significant.

As can be seen, measurement of the D2:D4 ratio in participants of the study showed differences between men and women. In fact, whereas in the entire sample the mean value was 0.98 (±0.041 SD), in men the mean value of the ratio was 0.97 (±0.039 SD) whereas in women it was 0.99 (±0.039 SD).

Comparison of these two latter sets of values using an unpaired t-test showed that the difference was statistically significant (p = 0.0028).

In contrast, no significant differences were observed in the distribution of observed D2:D4 ratios between men and women.

State and Trait Anxiety

Figure 2 shows the values of state anxiety (on the right) and trait anxiety (on the left) measured by the participants in the study. For both state anxiety and trait anxiety, the figure shows the values measured in the entire sample, and separately for men and women.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. State and trait anxiety values, both of the total sample, and separately for men and women.

Concerning the state anxiety, in the entire sample, the mean value was 47.42 (±11.01 SD), in men the mean value was 46.32 (±11.35 SD) whereas in women it was 48.66 (±10.56 SD).

Concerning the trait anxiety, in the whole sample, the mean value was 48.10 (±11.84 SD), in men the mean value was 47.53 (±12.68 SD) whereas in women it was 48.75 (±10.89 SD).

For both state anxiety and trait anxiety, the unpaired t-test showed no statistically significant differences between men and women.

Correlation Between D2:D4 Ratio and Anxiety

Figure 3 shows the correlations between D2:D4 ratios and state anxiety (on the left) and trait anxiety (on the right) at in the entire sample of subjects participating in the study, and separately for men and women.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Positive correlations between D2:D4 ratios and state and trait anxiety scores, both of the total sample, and separately for men and women.

As can be seen for the entire sample, there is a positive significant relationship between the D2:D4 ratio and score at STAI for both state anxiety (p = 0.0146) and trait anxiety (p = 0.0024).

However, if men are examined separately from women, it can be observed that only men have a statistically significant relationship between D2:D4 ratios and state anxiety (p = 0.0478) and trait anxiety (P = 0.0186).

Correlation Between age and Anxiety

Figure 4 shows the correlations between the age of participants and state anxiety (on the left) and trait anxiety (on the right) measured in the entire sample of subjects participating in the study, and separately for men and women.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Negative correlations between the ages of participants and state and trait anxiety scores, both of the total sample, and separately for men and women.

As can be seen, a significant negative relationship was observed for both state and trait anxiety with age, without differences between men and women.

However, if participants (both the men and women) are separated into individuals with a D2:D4 ratio less than 1 (N = 79) and subjects with a D2:D4 ratio equal or greater than 1 (N = 46), the situation changes significantly.

In fact, as can be seen in Figure 5, individuals with a D2:D4 ratio < 1 did not show a significant correlation between their age and their state anxiety and trait anxiety.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlations between the ages of participants and state and trait anxiety scores of the individuals with a D2:D4 ratio ≥ 1.

In contrast, individuals with a D2:D4 ratio ≥1 showed a statistically significant negative linear correlation between their age and their state anxiety and trait anxiety.

Discussion

Anxiety is an uncomfortable feeling, such as worry or fear that can be mild or severe. Everyone experiences anxiety in their life, as for a job interview or an exam or a medical test. Some individuals cope with these situations without great difficulty, while others find it difficult to control their worries. In these individuals, their feelings of anxiety are more constant and can often affect their daily lives.

As said above, the main purpose of this study was to identify a possible correlation between the D2:D4 ratio and state and/or trait anxiety in adult healthy subjects, and, if so, whether it exists any difference between men and women. In addition, we also wanted to observe whether there is a relationship between participants' age and state and/or trait anxiety.

The observed results can be summarized as follows:

• It should first be pointed out that the D2:D4 ratio measured in men was slightly lower (0.97 ± 0.039 SD) than that observed in women (0.99 ± 0.039 SD), and that this difference was statistically significant (P = 0.0028).

• Regarding anxiety levels in participants, it should also be noted that, for both state anxiety and trait anxiety, no statistically significant differences were observed between men and women.

• Concerning possible correlations between the D2:D4 ratio and anxiety (summarized in Figure 3), there is a positive significant relationship between the D2:D4 ratio and score at STAI for both state anxiety (P = 0.0146) and trait anxiety (P = 0.0024). However, if men are examined separately from women, it can be observed that only men have a statistically significant relationship between D2:D4 ratios and state anxiety (P = 0.0478) and trait anxiety (P = 0.0186).

• About possible relations between the age of participants and state and trait anxiety, a significant negative relationship was observed, without differences between men and women (Figure 4). However, if study participants (both men and women) were separated into individuals with a D2:D4 ratio < 1 (N = 79) and with a D2:D4 ratio ≥ 1 (N = 46), only subjects with a D2:D4 ratio ≥ 1 showed a statistically significant negative linear correlation between their age and their state and trait anxiety (Figure 5).

Conclusion

This study confirms what has already been highlighted in the literature (see Manning et al., 2014b), namely, that the D2:D4 ratio is related to the individual's ability to cope with unexpected and/or negative situations, the main cause of anxiety. This ability, although more evident in men, is also detectable in women. This well accords with findings in studies that have emphasized that the D2:D4 ratio tends to be negatively correlated with rates of female workforce participation and that this effect was greatest for the right hand (Manning et al., 2014a).

In summary, the present data allow us to conclude that a low D2:D4 ratio (<1) represents a protective factor against anxiety in both men and women and that this protection seems likely to act throughout life. Certainly, it would be interesting in the future to investigate the correlation of digit ratio with both cognitive performance and psychological protective variables of anxiety, to confer to an anthropometric variable such as digit ratio a predictive value with a social impact.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Catania. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

SR, SM, MC, and VP contributed to the conception and design of the study. SR and SM were responsible for data collection and VP for statistical analysis. All the authors were responsible for the drafting and finalization of the manuscript, contributed to manuscript revision, and approved the submitted version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ahokas, A., Kaukoranta, J., Wahlbeck, K., and Aito, M. (2001). Estrogen deficiency in severe postpartum depression: successful treatment with sublingual physiologic 17beta-estradiol: a preliminary study. J.Clin.Psychiatry. 62, 332–336. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v62n0504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Baxter, A., Wood, E. K., Capitanio, J. P., and Higley, J. D. (2017). Sexual dimorphism in 2D:4D digit ratio is linked to anxiety in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). FHSS Mentor. Res. Conferen. 17, 304. Retrieved from: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/fhssconference_studentpub/304

Google Scholar

Bekker, M. H., and van Mens-Verhulst, J. (2007). Anxiety disorders: sex differences in prevalence, degree, and background, but gender-neutral treatment. Gend. Med. 4, S178–193. doi: 10.1016/S1550-8579(07)80057-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coates, J. M., Gurnell, M., and Rustichini, A. (2009). Second-to-fourth digit ratio predicts success among high-frequency financial traders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 106, 623–628. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810907106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coates, J. M., and Hebert, J. (2008). Endogenous steroids and financial risk taking on a London trading floor. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA.105, 6167–6172. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704025105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coco, M., Perciavalle, V., Maci, T., Nicoletti, F., Di Corrado, D., and Perciavalle, V. (2011). The second-to-fourth digit ratio correlates with the rate of academic performance in medical school students. Mol. Med. Rep. 4, 471–476. doi: 10.3892/mmr.2011.456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen-Bendahan, C. C., van de Beek, C., and Berenbaum, S. A. (2005). Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: methods and findings. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 353–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collear, M., and Hines, M. (1995). Human behavioral sex differences: a role for gonadal hormones during early development? Psychol. Bul. 118, 55–107. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Bruin, E. I., Verheij, F., Wiegman, T., and Ferdinand, R. F. (2004). Differences in finger length ratio between males with autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, ADHD, and anxiety disorders. Dev. Med. ChildNeurol. 48, 962–965. doi: 10.1017/S0012162206002118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douma, S. L., Husband, C., O'Donnell, M. E., Barwin, B. N., and Woodend, A. K. (2005). Estrogen-related mood disorders: reproductive life cycle factors. ANS Adv Nurs. Sci. 28, 364–375. doi: 10.1097/00012272-200510000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrler, D. J., Evans, J. G., and Mcghee, R. L. (1999). Extending big-five theory into childhood: A preliminary investigation into the relationship between big-five personality traits and behavior problems in children. Psychol. Sch. 36, 451–458.

Google Scholar

Evardone, M., and Alexander, G. M. (2009). Anxiety, sex-linked behaviors, and digit ratios (2D:4D). Arch. Sex. Behav. 38, 442–455. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9260-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabio, R. A., and Caprì, T. (2019). Automatic and controlled attentional capture by threatening stimuli. Heliyon 5, 17–52. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2019.e01752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, B., Manning, J. T., and Neave, N. (2004). Second to fourth digit ratio and the ‘big five’ personality factors. Pers. Individ. Differ. 37, 495–503. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fink, B., Neave, N., Laughton, K., and Manning, J. T. (2006). Second to fourth digit ratio and sensation seeking. Pers. Individ. Differ. 41, 1253–1262. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., and Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 62, 593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, A. A., Jacobson, K. C., Gardner, C. O., Prescott, C. A., and Kendler, K. S. (2005). Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders. Br. J. Psychiatry. 186, 190–196. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.3.190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilduff, L. P., Hopp, R. N., Cook, C. J., Crewther, B. T., and Manning, J. T. (2013). Digit ratio (2D: 4D), aggression, and testosterone in men exposed to an aggressive video stimulus. Evol. psychol. 11:147470491301100502. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimura, S., Schaumann, B. A., Plato, C. C., and Kitagawa, T. (1990). Embryological development and prevalence of digital flexion creases. Anat. Rec. 226, 249–257. doi: 10.1002/ar.1092260214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucion, A. B., Charchat, H., Pereira, G. A. M., and Rasia-Filho, A. A. (1996). Influence of early postnatal gonadal hormones on anxiety in adult male rats. Physiol. Behav. 60, 1419–1423. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00246-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malas, M. A., Dogan, S., Evcil, E. H., and Desdicioglu, K. (2006). Fetal development of the hand, digits, and digit ratio. EarlyHum. Dev. 82, 469–475. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J. T. (2002a). Digit Ratio: A Pointer to Fertility, Behavior, and Health. New Brunswick, NJ: RutgersUniversityPress.

Google Scholar

Manning, J. T. (2002b). The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length and performance in skiing. J SportsMed. Phys. Fitness. 42, 446−450. Retrieved from: https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/ratio-2nd-4th-digit-length-performance-skiing/docview/202673391/se-2?accountid=15599

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Manning, J. T., Barley, L., Walton, J., Lewis-Jones, D. I., Trivers, R. L., Singh, D., et al. (2000). The 2nd: 4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success: evidence for sexually antagonistic genes? Evol. Hum. Behav. 21, 163–183. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00029-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J. T., Churchill, A. J., and Peters, M. (2007). The effects of sex, ethnicity, and sexual orientation on self-measured digit ratio (2D:4D). Arch. Sex. Behav. 36, 223–233. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9171-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J. T., Fink, B., and Trivers, R. (2014a). Digit Ratio (2D:4D) and Gender Inequalities Across Nations. Evol. Psychol. 12, 757–768. doi: 10.1177/147470491401200406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J. T., Scutt, D., Wilson, J., and Lewis-Jones, D. I. (1998). The ratio of the 2nd and 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone, and oestrogen. Hum. Reprod. 13, 3000–3004. doi: 10.1093/humrep/13.11.3000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J. T., and Taylor, R. P. (2001). Second to fourth digit ratio and male ability in sport: implications for sexual selection in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 61–69. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00063-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manning, J., Kilduff, L., Cook, C., Crewther, B., and Fink, B. (2014b). Digit Ratio (2D:4D): A Biomarker for Prenatal Sex Steroids and Adult Sex Steroids in Challenge Situations. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). 30, 9. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Massimino, S., Rinella, S., Buscemi, A., Similia, E., Perciavalle, V., Perciavalle, V., et al. (2019). Digit ratio, personality and emotions in skydivers. Biomed. Rep.10, 39–46. doi: 10.3892/br.2018.1174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHenry, J., Carrier, N., Hull, E., and Kabbaj, M. (2014). Sex differences in anxiety and depression: role of testosterone. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 35, 42–57. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neyse, L., and Brañas-Garza, P. (2014). Digit Ratio Measurement Guide. Kiel Institute for the World Economy.

Google Scholar

Parker, G. B., and Brotchie, H. L. (2004). From diathesis to dimorphism: the biology of gender differences in depression. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 192, 210–216. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000116464.60500.63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perciavalle, V., Di Corrado, D., Petralia, M. C., Gurrisi, L., Massimino, S., and Coco, M. (2013). The second-to-fourth digit ratio correlates with aggressive 103 behavior in professional soccer players. Mol. Med. Rep. 7, 1733–1738. doi: 10.3892/mmr.2013.1426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pigott, T. A. (2003). Anxiety disorders in women. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 26, 621–672. doi: 10.1016/S0193-953X(03)00040-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putz, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., Sporter, R. J., and McBurney, D. H. (2004). Sex hormones and finger length: What does 2D:4D indicate? Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 182–199. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinella, S., Buscemi, A., Massimino, S., Perciavalle, V., Tortorici, M. M., Ghiunè Tomaselli, D., et al. (2019). Risk-taking behavior, the second-to-fouth digit ratio and psychological features in a sample of caves. PeerJ. 10, 8029. doi: 10.7717/peerj.8029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seedat, S., Scott, K. M., Angermeyer, M. C., Berglund, P., Bromet, E. J., Brugha, T. S., et al. (2009). Cross-national associations between gender and mental disorders in the world health organization world mental health surveys. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 66, 785–795. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.36

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shear, M. K., Feske, U., and Greeno, C. (2000). “Gender differences in anxiety disorders: Clinical implications,” in: Gender and its Effects on Psychopathology, ed Frank E. (Washington, DC: American PsychiatricPress, Inc), pp. 151–165.

Google Scholar

Shores, M. M., Sloan, K. L., Matsumoto, A. M., Moceri, V. M., Felker, B., and Kivlahan, D. R. (2004). Increased incidence of diagnosed depressive illness in hypogonadal older men. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 61, 162–167. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solomon, M. B., and Herman, J. P. (2009). Sex differences in psychopathology: of gonads, adrenals and mental illness. Physiol. Behav. 97, 250–258. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y) Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D. (1989). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography (2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Tester, N., and Campbell, A. (2007). Sporting achievement: what is the contribution of digit ratio? J. Pers. 75, :663–677. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00452.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voracek, M., Bagdonas, A., and Dressler, S. G. (2007). Digit ratio (2D:4D) in Lithuania once and now: testing for sex differences, relations with eye and hair color, and a possible secular change. Coll. Antropol. 31, 863–868. Available online at: https://hrcak.srce.hr/26901

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Williams, J. H. G., Greenhalgh, K. D., and Manning, J. T. (2003). Second to fourth finger ratio and the possible precursors of developmental psychopathology in preschool children. EarlyHum. Dev. 72, 57–65. doi: 10.1016/S0378-3782(03)00012-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, T. J., Pepitone, M. E., Christensen, S. E., Cooke, B. M., Huberman, A. D., Breedlove, N. J., et al. (2000). Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation. Nature. 404, 455–456. doi: 10.1038/35006555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yonkers, K. A., and Kidner, C. L. (2002). “Sex differences in anxiety disorders,” in: Psychiatric Illness in Women: Emerging Treatments and Research, eds Lewis-Hall F, Williams TS, et al., (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing), pp. 5–30.

Google Scholar

Zarrouf, F. A., Artz, S., Griffith, J., Sirbu, C., and Kommor, M. (2009). Testosterone and depression: systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Psychiatr. Pract. 15, 289–305. doi: 10.1097/01.pra.0000358315.88931.fc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: STAI-Y2, testosterone, personological factors, sex-linked behaviors, affective state

Citation: Rinella S, Massimino S, Sorbello A, Perciavalle V and Coco M (2022) Cognitive Performances: The Role of Digit Ratio (D2:D4) With a Protective Factor for Anxiety. Front. Neuroergon. 3:870362. doi: 10.3389/fnrgo.2022.870362

Received: 06 February 2022; Accepted: 02 March 2022;
Published: 19 April 2022.

Edited by:

David Perpetuini, University of Studies G. d'Annunzio Chieti and Pescara, Italy

Reviewed by:

Antonio Aquino, University of Studies G. d'Annunzio Chieti and Pescara, Italy
Tindara Caprì, National Research Council of Italy (CNR), Italy

Copyright © 2022 Rinella, Massimino, Sorbello, Perciavalle and Coco. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marinella Coco, marinella.coco@gmail.com

Download