Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 03 November 2023
Sec. Neurodevelopment
This article is part of the Research Topic Education and Health as Social Determinants: The Econeurobiology of Brain Development View all 10 articles

Positive or negative environmental modulations on human brain development: the morpho-functional outcomes of music training or stress

  • 1Department of Molecular Medicine, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 2Department of Mother and Child Health, University of Padova, Padova, Italy

In the last couple of decades, the study of human living brain has benefitted of neuroimaging and non-invasive electrophysiological techniques, which are particularly valuable during development. A number of studies allowed to trace the usual stages leading from pregnancy to adult age, and relate them to functional and behavioral measurements. It was also possible to explore the effects of some interventions, behavioral or not, showing that the commonly followed pathway to adulthood may be steered by external interventions. These events may result in behavioral modifications but also in structural changes, in some cases limiting plasticity or extending/modifying critical periods. In this review, we outline the healthy human brain development in the absence of major issues or diseases. Then, the effects of negative (different stressors) and positive (music training) environmental stimuli on brain and behavioral development is depicted. Hence, it may be concluded that the typical development follows a course strictly dependent from environmental inputs, and that external intervention can be designed to positively counteract negative influences, particularly at young ages. We also focus on the social aspect of development, which starts in utero and continues after birth by building social relationships. This poses a great responsibility in handling children education and healthcare politics, pointing to social accountability for the responsible development of each child.

1. Introduction

The study of brain development from pregnancy to adult age has been devoted for years to establishing a fix sequence of events in the morpho-functional development. This vision was the consequence of two concurrent causes, in different fields of knowledge. First, the development of theories of cognitive development in the field of child psychology, during the last century, that described a rather fixed sequence of functional acquisitions from birth onwards, during typical development. This brought forward the underlying idea that both the sequence and timing of acquisition were rather stable and culture-independent, at least in the first phases of development. A revision of the literature on these topics is outside the scope of this review: the readers can refer to the seminal works of leaders in the field, like Jean Piaget and Lev Semënovič Vygotskij. Second, in the emerging field of neuroscience, most data on brain development were from histology, while physiological recordings were limited to certain ages and were mostly not intended to detect lifelong changes. This had the consequence of transmitting the idea that fixed steps of structural and functional development are reached at precise times, with an invariant sequence and limited variability in timing. Some notable exceptions included the studies on plasticity of the visual cortex, that led David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel to share the Nobel prize in 1991. However, technical advances like functional neuroimaging and powerful electrophysiological registration, coupled to the enhanced information processing and miniaturization of devices, led in the last three decades to a paradigm shift in neurosciences. This happened for the inspiring works of Penfield on cortical registrations (Penfield and Boldrey, 1937; Penfield and Jasper, 1954), leading to the definition of brain functional maps (the so-called ‘homunculus’), that now appear more and more plastic even in adults. It is becoming increasingly clear that any function of the brain emerges from a complex network of interactions between gene expression and environmental inputs, at the micro- and macro-scale. Under usual circumstances, moving from one step to the other occurs along a phylogenetically defined best-fit pathway, common to most mammals, that goes from sensory-motor to social and cognitive development, linked to the maturation of specific brain areas and networks. As such, any anomaly may hamper the typical developmental scheme (see Figure 1) and triggers a wealth of downstream effects aimed at fixing the path, with outcomes that may be fitting or not.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The timeline shows some acquisitions during typical development. EEG, electroencephalographic; EM, embryonic month; EW, embryonic week; PM, postnatal month; Yrs, years.

However, during the developmental path (summarized in Table 1) some critical periods of particular sensitivity and plasticity may be delimited, which span through infancy (for example: language acquisition) and extend to adolescence, in particular for some functions like memory and social stress management (Fuhrmann et al., 2015). Actually, time is a critical factor in development, since the susceptibility to some influences may dramatically vary, as well as the consequences at different time frames, including biochemical, electrical, genomic and epigenetic mechanisms, up to the effects at the level of development of the organism, which may be lifelong (Boyce et al., 2020).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of the main developmental steps during infancy.

Here we review the recent literature on the development of the human brain and its susceptibility to both negative and positive environmental influences. A search on PubMed was done on March 24, 2023 with the following terms: BRAIN and PLASTICITY and CHILD and DEVELOPMENT, in any field, with no filters for language or year of publication: 1705 papers were retrieved. By applying the filters: Meta-Analysis, Review and Systematic Review 573 papers were excluded to give 1,132 papers. The goal was to focus on healthy brain development, hence abstracts were read and evaluated to exclude papers exclusively or mainly related to Autism (n = 75), other diseases (n = 376) or out of focus, including studies done on cells or animals, or retracted studies (n = 311). The resulting 370 articles were evaluated, sorting out those describing studies on the morphological and functional normal development of the brain, stress effects on the brain and music training effects on the brain. Articles related to second language learning or other manipulations were excluded, as well as comment articles or introduction to issues, or duplicate publications (same authors, title or content, also in different languages). The 125 resulting relevant articles are reviewed here.

2. Morphological and functional development of the brain

2.1. Structural changes across development

A large longitudinal study addressed the question of whether cognitive improvement preceded, accompanied or followed the changes in the thickness and surface area of the cortex from infancy to adulthood, when an association between cortical measurements and cognitive performance is apparent: being the rate of change at each measurement predictive of subsequent changes, it was concluded that structural changes in the cortex are related to cognitive performance and vice-versa, without a clear sequence in any measure (Estrada et al., 2019). Cortical thickness has been also specifically linked to different neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders (Patel et al., 2021).

Normative structural data in the first 6 years of life highlight a generalized thinning of cortex, with the exception of the occipital areas, which first decrease and then become thicker (Remer et al., 2017). Cortical thinning is mostly related to pyramidal neurons, astrocyte and microglia marker genes (Shin et al., 2018). Also, during the first 5 years of life, genes in cortical neurons change dramatically their methylation status, while later changes are much reduced (Price et al., 2019).

Structural maturation of the frontal cortex, crucial for executive functions, requires a prolonged postnatal development, which involves also the migration and integration of newly formed inhibitory interneurons (Paredes et al., 2016). Interestingly, inhibitory control training in children shows larger effects that in adolescent, both on the efficiency of behavioral control and in the structure of some subdivisions of the inferior frontal gyrus in the prefrontal cortex (Delalande et al., 2020). Maturation of cortical circuits are also echoed by biochemical fingerprinting in specific cell types. Neural cell adhesion molecule (NCAM) isoforms are implicated in cell migration, axonal growth and synaptic plasticity, besides schizophrenia. As determined in post-mortem samples, they peak at different times during development in the various areas, from fetal/early infancy to late adolescence, supporting specific roles in neural circuitry formation, emerging at precise developmental timepoints (Cox et al., 2009). Also, connections between areas mature with time: for example, at 9 months the connections over longer distance are emerging, while local network connectivity decreases (Damaraju et al., 2014). Throughout development, structural and functional connectivity develop with non-linear trajectories, which also differ for the various areas, starting in utero and extending to late adolescence (Vandewouw et al., 2021). Increased connectivity boosts the emergence of executive and cognitive functions, with a differential contribution of the striatum, which improves cognitive functions in childhood through increase in the cortico-cortical connections, while its effects on the executive functions appear less age-related (Darki et al., 2020). During adolescence, myelin microstructure refines to increase the speed of electrical signal transmission, at the expenses of diminished plasticity, and mature first in sensorimotor areas and then in associative areas (Baum et al., 2022).

2.2. Development of sensory-motor functions

In the striate cortex, functional plasticity mirrors the number of synapses that peaks in the first year of age and is rapidly refined and reduced in the subsequent pre-school years (Huttenlocher and de Courten, 1987). Early imaging studies reported a decrease in grey matter of frontal and parietal cortices during adolescence (Jernigan et al., 1991), while enzymes related to cholinergic and glutamatergic neurotransmission vary across the entire lifespan, in specific areas (Court et al., 1993). Notably GABA-ergic neurotransmission show a protracted period of postnatal refinement, spanning the first years of life and possibly accounting for the protracted plasticity of visual areas, which allows therapeutic interventions (Murphy et al., 2005).

Also, in the primary motor cortex GABAergic interneurons mature after childhood, fostering plasticity and motor learning (Walther et al., 2009). Contrary to findings in monkeys, in humans the corticospinal projections start connecting with spinal cord at 24 post-conception weeks, between 2 and 4 postnatal months the spontaneous activity becomes more coordinated between limbs, yet functional control of distal effectors is reached much later, between 6 and 12 months of age, to support goal-directed movements (Eyre et al., 2000; Kanemaru et al., 2012).

Movement of the hand requires the identification of targets, which is usually based on vision: already 2 days after birth, newborns may be trained to discriminate kinematic patterns of biological movement, characterized by subsequent acceleration/deceleration, even if they spontaneously do not (Craighero et al., 2020). Already at 5–6 months, the movement of the hand is typically directed to a person or to an object: this specificity is missing in children not sharing typical developmental paths (Ouss et al., 2018). Sensorimotor coordination accuracy improves in primary school children while sensorimotor integration relies on subcortical circuits maturing at a later stage, towards adulthood (Savion-Lemieux et al., 2009). On the other hand, in children and adults, observation of an action activates the same mirror-neurons system (premotor cortex-inferior frontal gyrus and posterior parietal lobe), but with a more widespread and more bilateral activation in children than in adults (Biagi et al., 2016). Human movement develops through the progressive control of tools use, which requires a complex dynamic between body size representation and sensory inputs, so that only in late puberty the body representation acquires adult features and may rely on proprioception instead of visual perception (Martel et al., 2021). Of note, children do not adapt as adults to tactile stimuli, until 8–10 years of age, suggesting a different sensory experience in addition to a different stimulus processing (Domenici et al., 2022).

Expert visual processing of face is lateralized to the right hemisphere and requires visual input during infancy to become fully operational, suggesting protracted need for stimulation (Le Grand et al., 2003), yet face specialization starts before reading acquisition an impinges on the decreased cortical responses to the other stimuli (Cantlon et al., 2011), as well as on higher glutamate relative to GABA levels in the inferior frontal gyrus (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2015). Similarly, movement-directed visual attentional shifts for actions appears already at 7 months, suggesting concomitant maturation of visual and attentional systems (Daum et al., 2016). Later on, during school-age period, the increase in activity of the adrenal gland with dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) surge, promotes the concomitant maturation of amygdala with occipital lobe, related to visual awareness, parietal lobe, related to visuomotor abilities, and frontal lobe, related to attention (Nguyen et al., 2016). Apparently, DHEA in childhood helps optimization of attentional and working memory functions but may impair the processing of spatial cues by reducing the connections from hippocampus to cortex (Nguyen et al., 2017).

Functional lateralization requires the maturation of corpus callosum, which has a critical refinement period after 6 years of age, thus affecting language transfer between the two hemispheres (Westerhausen et al., 2011). Myelinization is indeed critical for full functional maturation, but while myelin turnover is fast, the number of oligodendrocytes in the corpus callosum is stable from childhood, with a yearly exchange rate of only 1 out of 300 (Yeung et al., 2014). Prolonged maturation and myelin plasticity appear related also to increased functional cognitive ability already by 3 years of age (Deoni et al., 2016). In the first 2 years of life myelination and microstructural properties of glia appear related to cognitive abilities, with protracted development associated to better performance in cognitive and language tasks (Girault et al., 2019). Also, cortical thickness in the first 2 years appears related to cognitive abilities, yet the contribution of gestational age and maternal education may overcome structural differences (Girault et al., 2020).

More complex sensory functions, including multisensory integration, appear subsequently after middle childhood (Ernst, 2008), and their fine-tuning appears complete by 14 years (Brandwein et al., 2011). Cognitive mathematical abilities may be related to white matter in the left parietal lobe (Matejko et al., 2013) and better outcomes of intense math training rely on left perisylvian tracts plasticity (Jolles et al., 2016a), with a specific effect on connectivity of the intraparietal sulcus, but not of the angular gyrus, with hippocampus, lateral prefrontal and ventral temporo-occipital cortex (Jolles et al., 2016b). The intraparietal sulcus content in glutamate and GABA appears developmentally linked to math abilities (Zacharopoulos et al., 2021). Similarly, abacus training in primary school children improves performance and executive functions by modulating frontoparietal activation (Wang et al., 2017), and appears linked to the volume of fusiform gray matter (Zhou et al., 2022). In the first 2 years of primary school, better outcomes in mathematical abilities are associated with specific changes in some cortical areas, in detail: arithmetic abilities are linked to folding change in the right intraparietal sulcus, and thickness changes in right temporal lobe and left middle occipital gyrus, while visuospatial abilities are linked to right superior parietal thickness, and other frontal areas in the right hemisphere (Kuhl et al., 2020). However, complex visuospatial tasks elicit strong bilateral parietal activation in both adults and children from age 5 onwards (Ferrara et al., 2021).

Quantitative differences in cognitive abilities, operationally defined as a higher IQ, appear linked to a prolonged sensitivity to environmental influences (Brant et al., 2013) while its relationships to cortical thinning and surface area in late childhood appears controversial (Burgaleta et al., 2014; Schnack et al., 2015). Functional control for cognitive functions emerges at a later time, for example executive attention is linked to planning and inhibitory control, and allows the development of self-regulation (Rueda et al., 2005). Functional plasticity appears high in late childhood also for memory function (Brehmer et al., 2007). Emotional regulation steers impulsivity in the context of prospective thinking: it appears in late childhood and is related to insula thickness (Churchwell and Yurgelun-Todd, 2013). As outlined above, amygdala development is crucial in managing the emotional reactivity: it is noteworthy that the paralaminar nuclei of amygdala host a population of immature cells that slowly develops through childhood and adolescence into excitatory neurons but still persists even in old age, suggesting a protracted plasticity in this area (Sorrells et al., 2019).

The developmental trajectory goes through a reduction in modularity and local efficiency of brain processing, while increasing global efficiency. This increase in the efficiency of global processing, linked to functional maturation, initially affects sensorimotor areas. At variance, associative and paralimbic areas show a protracted plasticity during late childhood, which may account for peripubertal behavioral modulation (Khundrakpam et al., 2013). In school-age children, enhanced brain modularity may also prepare for disclosing effects of physical activity on cognitive and executive functions (Chaddock-Heyman et al., 2020).

2.3. Sleep to grow

Already during the first year of life, the pattern of night sleep and awakening may predict typical and atypical cognitive trajectories (Pisch et al., 2019), while slow waves propagation during the night, which depends on brain connectivity, is reduced in toddlers compared to older children (Schoch et al., 2018). Also, the decline in slow-wave non-REM sleep activity is steeper during adolescence, in caudal-rostral direction, suggesting late functional reorganization following structural synaptic pruning (Feinberg et al., 2011). At variance, local increase in slow-wave activity over the right parietal areas, related to visuomotor-dependent plasticity, is higher in children (Wilhelm et al., 2014), as it is the slow-wave increase in left frontoparietal areas after working memory training (Pugin et al., 2015). Around 1 year of age, sleep spindles appear to be related to semantic generalization of words (Friedrich et al., 2015), while in school-age children the learning-dependent hippocampal activity and sleep-related frontal activity do change at a faster rate than in adults (Urbain et al., 2016). Children also show the largest overnight slope change in slow waves, which may be related to the increased plasticity of children brain (Jaramillo et al., 2020). Slow waves are generated by corticocortical connections while spindles results from thalamocortical activity and in adolescents appear modulated by genetic background in posterior areas, while spindles in anterior areas are more sensitive to environmental factors (Rusterholz et al., 2018). Interestingly, the larger modulation of slow-wave activity during night in children is not accompanied by the change in glutamate/glutamine which is apparent in adults, pointing to different biochemical pathways in children (Volk et al., 2019). Another feature of human EEG activity is the alpha oscillation, which shows a maturation during childhood, most apparent for the aperiodic component: this is related to increased thalamocortical connections and attentional performance (Tröndle et al., 2022).

3. How environment may interact with developmental trajectories

The effect of environment on development can be positive or negative from the very beginning of pregnancy throughout postnatal life. Among negative environmental regulations with a heavy societal impact, the effect of pre-natal alcohol exposure has long been studied in both animals and humans. Several studies have documented the long-lasting effects of maternal alcohol consumption on both the structure and functions of the developing brain and ultimately child fitness. Despite the detailed discussion of this topic is outside the scope of this Review, we highlight that early longitudinal studies proved the adverse effects of heavy drinking during pregnancy on the morpho-functional development, in particular in the parietal cortex (Lebel et al., 2012) and in the development of white matter in relation to executive functions (Gautam et al., 2014).

Also, other environmental stimuli or their absence may interfere with the development of structural features and functional acquisitions during postnatal development. In order to develop harmonic abilities, the interaction with environment may foster or hinder functions after their appearance. As an example, children with blind parents normally show the eye contact from birth, which usually is a common means of communication, but by age 6 months onwards they display progressively less attention to gaze processing, even if this is not related to impairments in social or cognitive abilities, suggesting that even ‘obvious’ abilities require a strong social/environmental input and practice to be fully operational (Senju et al., 2015). Hence, the neuroscientific literature provides several lines of evidence that support the steering role of environmental stimuli in early development, with durable effects in different areas, both anatomical and functional, including cognitive, emotional, and social abilities.

3.1. Case studies

Here we briefly introduce two examples, one of negative (stress) and one of positive (music training) environmental influences on the development of the brain and its functions, to highlight how much we as adults are responsible for the life paths of future generations. In order to include articles from different countries, socioeconomical status and cultures, we focused on two modulators that were less likely linked to these factors. Stressful environmental conditions like low socioeconomical status, trauma or neglect share similar characteristics among different cultures. Also, music and music training are widespread in all cultures, and include learning sensory-motor abilities which makes them less dependent on overall cognitive abilities or higher socioeconomical status than, for example second language training. Thus, music training appears a robust example to explore in this review.

3.2. The role of stress

Stress may profoundly impact on neurodevelopmental trajectories, at different ages, by means of the different neural, endocrine, neuroendocrine, metabolic and immune responses. Notably, the brain itself is the target of stress hormones, that shape the brain stress response, tuned to plasticity, a double-edged sword that may either blunt or enhance adaptive responses, modifying the vulnerability to early life stressors. Under this respect, the concept of resilience includes the processes leading to positive adaptation to relevant traumas or adverse challenges. Early rehabilitation and care, including proprioceptive stimulation as sensory-tonic stimulation and kangaroo care, coupled to parenting support may influence the development of very preterm infants (Guittard et al., 2023), while severe maltreatment or abuse impairs functional and structural brain development, thus representing a relevant threat for the single person and a significant cost for society. Hence, boosting resilient responses in high-risk persons may promote neural and neuroendocrine plasticity to decrease maladaptive or even psychopathological responses (Cicchetti, 2010). In the first 2 years of life, elicited imitation task as a tool to investigate declarative memory, reveals that neglected children do not receive maternal feedback while abused children do, leading to a loss of plasticity in neglected children, while increased imitation in abused infants possibly leads them to increased cognitive but decreased social competence (Cheatham et al., 2010). The brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is involved in synaptic plasticity and is differentially expressed in childhood (Sterner et al., 2012). Interestingly, the exposure to unfavorable environment leads to depression in persons carrying the Val66Met BDNF polymorphism (Comasco et al., 2013), with Val carriers of the same polymorphism more prone to self-injurious behavior (Bresin et al., 2013), while childhood abuse in Met carriers results in poorer cognitive performance and brain anomalies, including larger lateral ventricles and reduced right hippocampus (Aas et al., 2013). The same polymorphism appears to impact stress experience more at late stages (Lehto et al., 2016). BDNF methylation in adolescent brain is related to neighborhood disadvantage and thinner lateral orbitofrontal cortex (Wrigglesworth et al., 2019). Children outcome on different measures was linked to parenting quality, but also to BDNF status and genes involved in dopamine and serotonin neurotransmission, that appear to convey some vulnerability to environmental stress, fostering the vision of a continuum of general traits to describe the responses to stress, instead of two susceptibility traits (e.g., the orchid/dandelion duality) leading to different responses (Zhang et al., 2021). By widening the analysis to the BDNF gene network, it appeared that this network interacted with adverse prenatal conditions to affect later cognitive development, so that a high BDNF network score coupled to high prenatal adversity resulted in slower cognitive development and grey matter density in associative cortical areas (de Mendonça Filho et al., 2021).

Material hardship linked to poverty may lead to different amygdala-prefrontal cortex connectivity in late infancy, and leads to reduced amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex connections in adolescents, also related to anxiety and depression, indicating some preferential windows of plasticity for targeted supporting interventions (Hardi et al., 2022).

Low socioeconomic resources may impair visual working memory, as a proxy for cognitive abilities, and related brain activity in the left frontal cortex of children up to 4 years old (Wijeakumar et al., 2019). The socioeconomical status may result in differential exposure to language or stress, which may be the bases for the differences in hippocampus and amygdala seen in socioeconomically disadvantaged children, with an additional contribution of age, inducing additional differences in the left superior temporal and inferior frontal gyri (Noble et al., 2012). Similar reductions in amygdala and hippocampus were detected in children experiencing early life stress in the form of physical abuse, early neglect or low socioeconomic status (Hanson et al., 2015). Child abuse appears also to affect the morphological complexity of the prefrontal cortex and to increase recruitment of perineuronal networks, mediated by oligodendrocyte precursors, that lead to decreased plasticity (Tanti et al., 2022).

A large study confirmed that trauma exposure resulted in adolescent thinner superior frontal gyri and right amygdala and larger cingulate cortices (Jeong et al., 2021). Child maltreatment results in increased Cornu Ammonis (CA) 4 subfield of hippocampus, most apparent in males, while larger CA1 is associated with late-onset psychopathology, suggesting that maltreatment differentially affects hippocampal subfields, which may precede the appearance of psychopathology (Whittle et al., 2016). The level of self-perceived stress is also associated with smaller hippocampal volume in adolescents (Piccolo et al., 2018), and in a longitudinal study, attachment dimensions like anxiety and avoidance were linked to larger decreases in prefrontal and anterior temporal cortices in adolescent brain (Puhlmann et al., 2023).

In adolescents, post-traumatic stress disorder, as a result of altered fear regulation, is linked to decreased grey matter volume in the centromedial and basolateral amygdala, whose connectivity with left orbitofrontal and subcallosal cortices is increased, while connections to the right cingulate and prefrontal cortices appear less strong (Aghajani et al., 2016).

Volumetric correlations among different areas indicate that prenatal stress but not childhood trauma may de-couple amygdala growth from the development of other regions involved in emotional processing (Mareckova et al., 2022). Early childhood deprivation induces long-term modifications, apparent in adult white matter tracts, in particular of the limbic circuits and long-ranging association fibers, while the microstructural organization appears not altered (Mackes et al., 2022). Also, epigenetic changes in some genes associated to child abuse may enhance the risk of child depression (Weder et al., 2014), and regions of low methylation differentiated children receiving less tactile contact (Moore et al., 2017). Lastly, genome-wide mapping fished out some loci linked to postnatal stress and subcortical structures like caudate and accumbens nuclei, which have a role in neuronal plasticity and neurodevelopmental disorders, however causality remains to be ascertained (Bolhuis et al., 2022).

3.3. Music training – positively steering development

Highlights for positive experience-related brain plasticity stem from the effects of music training in infancy, because of the worldwide diffusion of music across all human cultures, paralleled by a widespread training at young age, which is however not diffuse to all persons. Because of the multimodal nature and prolonged practice of instrument or voice, music training appears well suited to exploit plastic capabilities of the brain.

Adult musicians show increased sound discrimination, as a result of training. Early music training enhances children performance in specific musical skills like melody discrimination (Ireland et al., 2019). After 2 years of training in school age children, an improvement in tonal discrimination and increased maturity of auditory processing are apparent (Habibi et al., 2016). In 9 to 15 years old trained or untrained subjects, cognitive flexibility was linked to sound discrimination performance, which was more apparent in music-trained group (Saarikivi et al., 2016).

Instrument practice allows a regional-specific increase in the organization of the pyramidal tract already in childhood (Bengtsson et al., 2005) and extend to cognitive abilities underlying musical training, initially on more closely related fields (Schlaug et al., 2005). Long lasting effects on motor performance appear stronger if the music practice starts before age seven, even if the amount of training was similar, pointing to the existence of a sensitive period in infancy (Penhune et al., 2005).

Interestingly, even relatively a short period (9 months) of music training may produce benefits for pitch processing in music but also in language, showing that cognitive benefits extend over different cognitive domains, by modifying their neural substrates and related pattern of brain activity (Moreno et al., 2009). Musical and linguistic syntactic abilities may be learned through similar processes in early infancy and about age 4–5, music training affects timbre identification and improve language abilities, like morphologic rule formation and memory for words, showing that training effects extend beyond the music domain (Marin, 2009). Notably, increased right brain activity due to human voice processing is related to intelligence in toddlers and school-age children (An et al., 2020).

Preschool children benefit even from short (20 days) music training, whose effects spill over to verbal intelligence and executive function tasks (Moreno et al., 2011). Over 2 years of training around 8 years old, speech segmentation skills improve more than in untrained children suggesting therapeutic strategies for children with language impairments and related learning difficulties (François et al., 2013).

In preschool children, music or second language training induce long-lasting improvement in processing of the trained sounds and increase suppression of untrained, non-relevant sounds, as shown by event-related potentials (Moreno et al., 2015).

By exploring the brain structure, aptitude to music in school-age children is related to pre-training structural organization of the right corticospinal tract while the corpus callosum structure appears more linked to tonal ability (Zuk et al., 2022).

Increased pitch discrimination is related to larger auditory regions in both untrained and music-trained adults and children, while in musicians it is also associated to larger inferior frontal gyrus (Palomar-García et al., 2020).

After only 15 months of practice, music training may induce structural changes in the brain, directly related to improvements in auditory and motor skills (Hyde et al., 2009). Apparently, starting musical training before age 7 changes white matter connectivity, more robustly in the isthmus of corpus callosum: therein, fractional anisotropy, related to myelinization, is linked to both age of starting the training and sensorimotor synchronization performance (Steele et al., 2013). On the other hand, the benefits of music appear to extend also to prenatal age, since in preterm infants exposed to musicotherapy, the maturation of white matter improved in acoustic radiations, claustrum and uncinate fasciculum, and also amygdala volumes increased, suggesting improved acoustic and emotional processing, compared to non-exposed preterm and full-term babies (Sa de Almeida et al., 2020). The age of onset is critical for structural changes to appear: focusing on exposure to a second language and music, it emerged that the arcuate fasciculus, which participates in both music and language activities by linking areas of the dorsal auditory pathway, is sensitive to second language in the left hemisphere, while in the right one it changes according to music exposure (Vaquero et al., 2020). Hence, the structure of different areas is selectively modified according to the type of experience mostly in early infancy.

4. Development is socially modulated

A relevant, yet underappreciated, issue in developmental neuroscience is the social nature of our species. In a study involving both parents and children, focused on the intergenerational transmission of sociality, the parents’ limbic, embodied simulation and mentalizing networks appeared linked to the use of strategies for children’s emotion regulation, suggesting a strong link between parent–child interactions and later child social life (Abraham et al., 2016). However maternal influence starts prenatally, since maternal stress (e.g., pandemic-related) affects 3 months infants’ regulatory capacity (Provenzi et al., 2021), and also extends to calibration of growth rate and timing of sexual development, by affecting postnatal testosterone levels in infants (Corpuz, 2021). Maternal depression during pregnancy appears to influence the development of amygdala, by interacting with the canonical transforming growth factor-beta (TGF-β) signaling pathway (Qiu et al., 2021).

In school-age children, parental praise as a positive parenting style may result in increased openness to experiences and carefulness, together with increase in gray matter in the posterior insula, which is involved in empathy modulation due to the connections with amygdala (Matsudaira et al., 2016). By using fMRI-based neurofeedback, it was possible to demonstrate that both children and adolescents can learn to upregulate amygdala function, suggesting a possible tool to act on regulation of emotional reactivity (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2016).

5. Conclusion

The possibility of non-invasive exploration of the living brain also in children, emerged in the last years, led to a dramatic increase in our understanding of development of the brain and its functions, in the context of the growing body. The development of cognitive, emotional, and social behaviors appears more along a continuum than limited to narrow developmental windows, while certain attainments may be specific to some developmental periods (Guyer et al., 2018). Out of the laboratory, the increasing knowledge of the developmental processes led to a paradigm shift in the approach to children, in the context of parental relationships and pedagogical approaches, up to inform the political processes. Actually, sharing knowledge accumulating through dedicated studies on brain development and the increasing evidence about the long-lasting functional outcome of environmental modifications, may serve to raise consciousness about the actions to undertake to provide support and care to fragile children. While awareness of environmental risks for development and overall health is increasing (Chesney and Duderstadt, 2022), a widespread knowledge of the risks and possibilities for external actions to drive children development is still on the way. The possibility of early detection of child needs even with primary pediatric care and support to the family increases the chances of steering cognitive, emotional and social development towards positive outcomes (Williams and Lerner, 2019). Addressing adverse childhood experiences requires fostering health and educational services to promote the foundation of lifelong health, with the necessary inclusion of family (Bethell et al., 2017). Assistance for families, starting from maternal health, and for communities will provide supporting relationships to lay the foundation of resilience throughout life (Traub and Boynton-Jarrett, 2017). This should be declined across different cultures and is particularly relevant for children with additional requirements, like neurodevelopmental disorders (Bannink Mbazzi and Kawesa, 2022). Including constructs like ‘neuroplasticity’ in the educational trajectories led to an empowerment of the main actors, children, parents and teachers by fostering executive functions (Choudhury and Wannyn, 2022). The contribution of widespread schooling on the social construction of cognition and neurocognitive development has long been appreciated (Baker et al., 2012). In these last years, programs have been designed to support selective attention in children from low socioeconomic status with some genotypes which may represent a risk factor (Isbell et al., 2017). However, cognitive development is only one side of the coin: the role of social regulation of development, starting from parents to the group of peers needs to be recognized and actively included in political long-sighted plans. More can be done on the bases of the recent data on the involvement of social processes in the development of self-regulatory processes, to improve both personal development and society.

Author contributions

CM-C: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing – original draft. GS: Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by a grant from the University of Padova (DOR2021) to CM-C.

Acknowledgments

We thank Luisa Canella for pushing us over the years to maintain our experimental (CM-C) and clinical (GS) focus on child development, and Antonio Caretta for fostering CM-C interest in brain development.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aas, M., Haukvik, U. K., Djurovic, S., Bergmann, Ø., Athanasiu, L., Tesli, M. S., et al. (2013). BDNF val66met modulates the association between childhood trauma, cognitive and brain abnormalities in psychoses. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 46, 181–188. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abraham, E., Hendler, T., Zagoory-Sharon, O., and Feldman, R. (2016). Network integrity of the parental brain in infancy supports the development of children’s social competencies. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1707–1718. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aghajani, M., Veer, I. M., van Hoof, M. J., Rombouts, S. A., van der Wee, N. J., and Vermeiren, R. R. (2016). Abnormal functional architecture of amygdala-centered networks in adolescent posttraumatic stress disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1120–1135. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

An, K. M., Hasegawa, C., Hirosawa, T., Tanaka, S., Saito, D. N., Kumazaki, H., et al. (2020). Brain responses to human-voice processing predict child development and intelligence. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 2292–2301. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, D. P., Salinas, D., and Eslinger, P. J. (2012). An envisioned bridge: schooling as a neurocognitive developmental institution. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, S6–S17. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannink Mbazzi, F., and Kawesa, E. S. (2022). Impairments of the brain: global south perspectives on childhood neurodevelopmental disability. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 64, 1193–1201. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.15253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baum, G. L., Flournoy, J. C., Glasser, M. F., Harms, M. P., Mair, P., Sanders, A. F. P., et al. (2022). Graded variation in T1w/T2w ratio during adolescence: measurement, caveats, and implications for development of cortical myelin. J. Neurosci. 42, 5681–5694. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2380-21.2022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bengtsson, S. L., Nagy, Z., Skare, S., Forsman, L., Forssberg, H., and Ullén, F. (2005). Extensive piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter development. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1148–1150. doi: 10.1038/nn1516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bethell, C. D., Solloway, M. R., Guinosso, S., Hassink, S., Srivastav, A., Ford, D., et al. (2017). Prioritizing possibilities for child and family health: an agenda to address adverse childhood experiences and Foster the social and emotional roots of well-being in pediatrics. Acad. Pediatr. 17, S36–S50. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2017.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biagi, L., Cioni, G., Fogassi, L., Guzzetta, A., Sgandurra, G., and Tosetti, M. (2016). Action observation network in childhood: a comparative fMRI study with adults. Dev. Sci. 19, 1075–1086. doi: 10.1111/desc.12353

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolhuis, K., Mulder, R. H., de Mol, C. L., Defina, S., Warrier, V., White, T., et al. (2022). Mapping gene by early life stress interactions on child subcortical brain structures: a genome-wide prospective study. JCPP Adv. 2:12113. doi: 10.1002/jcv2.12113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyce, W. T., Sokolowski, M. B., and Robinson, G. E. (2020). Genes and environments, development and time. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 117, 23235–23241. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2016710117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandwein, A. B., Foxe, J. J., Russo, N. N., Altschuler, T. S., Gomes, H., and Molholm, S. (2011). The development of audiovisual multisensory integration across childhood and early adolescence: a high-density electrical mapping study. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1042–1055. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brant, A. M., Munakata, Y., Boomsma, D. I., Defries, J. C., Haworth, C. M., Keller, M. C., et al. (2013). The nature and nurture of high IQ: an extended sensitive period for intellectual development. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1487–1495. doi: 10.1177/0956797612473119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brehmer, Y., Li, S. C., Müller, V., von Oertzen, T., and Lindenberger, U. (2007). Memory plasticity across the life span: uncovering children’s latent potential. Dev. Psychol. 43, 465–478. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bresin, K., Sima Finy, M., and Verona, E. (2013). Childhood emotional environment and self-injurious behaviors: the moderating role of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism. J. Affect. Disord. 150, 594–600. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.01.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgaleta, M., Johnson, W., Waber, D. P., Colom, R., and Karama, S. (2014). Cognitive ability changes and dynamics of cortical thickness development in healthy children and adolescents. NeuroImage 84, 810–819. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantlon, J. F., Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., and Pelphrey, K. A. (2011). Cortical representations of symbols, objects, and faces are pruned back during early childhood. Cereb. Cortex 21, 191–199. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock-Heyman, L., Weng, T. B., Kienzler, C., Weisshappel, R., Drollette, E. S., Raine, L. B., et al. (2020). Brain network modularity predicts improvements in cognitive and scholastic performance in children involved in a physical activity intervention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:346. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheatham, C. L., Larkina, M., Bauer, P. J., Toth, S. L., and Cicchetti, D. (2010). Declarative memory in abused and neglected infants. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 38, 161–182. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-374471-5.00007-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chesney, M. L., and Duderstadt, K. (2022). Children’s rights, environmental justice, and environmental health policy in the United States. J. Pediatr. Health Care 36, 3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.pedhc.2021.08.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choudhury, S., and Wannyn, W. (2022). Politics of plasticity: implications of the new science of the teen brain for education. Cult. Med. Psychiatry 46, 31–58. doi: 10.1007/s11013-021-09731-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Churchwell, J. C., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2013). Age-related changes in insula cortical thickness and impulsivity: significance for emotional development and decision-making. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 80–86. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cicchetti, D. (2010). Resilience under conditions of extreme stress: a multilevel perspective. World Psychiatry 9, 145–154. doi: 10.1002/j.2051-5545.2010.tb00297.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen Kadosh, K., Krause, B., King, A. J., Near, J., and Cohen, K. R. (2015). Linking GABA and glutamate levels to cognitive skill acquisition during development. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 4334–4345. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen Kadosh, K., Luo, Q., de Burca, C., Sokunbi, M. O., Feng, J., Linden, D. E. J., et al. (2016). Using real-time fMRI to influence effective connectivity in the developing emotion regulation network. NeuroImage 125, 616–626. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comasco, E., Åslund, C., Oreland, L., and Nilsson, K. W. (2013). Three-way interaction effect of 5-HTTLPR, BDNF Val66Met, and childhood adversity on depression: a replication study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 23, 1300–1306. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corpuz, R. (2021). The role of maternal environment on calibrating mini puberty in early infant development. Dev. Psychobiol. 63, 800–807. doi: 10.1002/dev.22033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Court, J. A., Perry, E. K., Johnson, M., Piggott, M. A., Kerwin, J. A., Perry, R. H., et al. (1993). Regional patterns of cholinergic and glutamate activity in the developing and aging human brain. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 74, 73–82. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(93)90085-o

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, E. T., Brennaman, L. H., Gable, K. L., Hamer, R. M., Glantz, L. A., Lamantia, A. S., et al. (2009). Developmental regulation of neural cell adhesion molecule in human prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 162, 96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.04.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craighero, L., Ghirardi, V., Lunghi, M., Panin, F., and Simion, F. (2020). Two-day-old newborns learn to discriminate accelerated-decelerated biological kinematics from constant velocity motion. Cognition 195:104126. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damaraju, E., Caprihan, A., Lowe, J. R., Allen, E. A., Calhoun, V. D., and Phillips, J. P. (2014). Functional connectivity in the developing brain: a longitudinal study from 4 to 9months of age. NeuroImage 84, 169–180. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darki, F., Sauce, B., and Klingberg, T. (2020). Pediatric imaging, neurocognition, and genetics study inter-individual differences in striatal connectivity is related to executive function through fronto-parietal connectivity. Cereb. Cortex 30, 672–681. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daum, M. M., Wronski, C., Harms, A., and Gredebäck, G. (2016). Action perception in infancy: the plasticity of 7-month-olds’ attention to grasping actions. Exp. Brain Res. 234, 2465–2478. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4651-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Mendonça Filho, E. J., Barth, B., Bandeira, D. R., de Lima, R. M. S., Arcego, D. M., Dalmaz, C., et al. (2021). Cognitive development and brain gray matter susceptibility to prenatal adversities: moderation by the prefrontal cortex brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene co-expression network. Front. Neurosci. 15:744743. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.744743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delalande, L., Moyon, M., Tissier, C., Dorriere, V., Guillois, B., Mevell, K., et al. (2020). Complex and subtle structural changes in prefrontal cortex induced by inhibitory control training from childhood to adolescence. Dev. Sci. 23:e12898. doi: 10.1111/desc.12898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deoni, S. C., O’Muircheartaigh, J., Elison, J. T., Walker, L., Doernberg, E., Waskiewicz, N., et al. (2016). White matter maturation profiles through early childhood predict general cognitive ability. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 1189–1203. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0947-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domenici, N., Tonelli, A., and Gori, M. (2022). The development of adaptation aftereffects in the vibrotactile domain. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 151, 3134–3143. doi: 10.1037/xge0001252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernst, M. O. (2008). Multisensory integration: a late bloomer. Curr. Biol. 18, R519–R521. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Estrada, E., Ferrer, E., Román, F. J., Karama, S., and Colom, R. (2019). Time-lagged associations between cognitive and cortical development from childhood to early adulthood. Dev. Psychol. 55, 1338–1352. doi: 10.1037/dev0000716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eyre, J. A., Miller, S., Clowry, G. J., Conway, E. A., and Watts, C. (2000). Functional corticospinal projections are established prenatally in the human foetus permitting involvement in the development of spinal motor centres. Brain 123, 51–64. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.1.51

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, I., de Bie, E., Davis, N. M., and Campbell, I. G. (2011). Topographic differences in the adolescent maturation of the slow wave EEG during NREM sleep. Sleep 34, 325–333. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.3.325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrara, K., Seydell-Greenwald, A., Chambers, C. E., Newport, E. L., and Landau, B. (2021). Development of bilateral parietal activation for complex visual-spatial function: evidence from a visual-spatial construction task. Dev. Sci. 24:e13067. doi: 10.1111/desc.13067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

François, C., Chobert, J., Besson, M., and Schön, D. (2013). Music training for the development of speech segmentation. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2038–2043. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedrich, M., Wilhelm, I., Born, J., and Friederici, A. D. (2015). Generalization of word meanings during infant sleep. Nat. Commun. 6:6004. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuhrmann, D., Knoll, L. J., and Blakemore, S. J. (2015). Adolescence as a sensitive period of brain development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 558–566. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gautam, P., Nuñez, S. C., Narr, K. L., Kan, E. C., and Sowell, E. R. (2014). Effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on the development of white matter volume and change in executive function. Neuroimage Clin. 5, 19–27. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girault, J. B., Cornea, E., Goldman, B. D., Jha, S. C., Murphy, V. A., Li, G., et al. (2020). Cortical structure and cognition in infants and toddlers. Cereb. Cortex 30, 786–800. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girault, J. B., Cornea, E., Goldman, B. D., Knickmeyer, R. C., Styner, M., and Gilmore, J. H. (2019). White matter microstructural development and cognitive ability in the first 2 years of life. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 1195–1210. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24439

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guittard, C., Novo, A., Eutrope, J., Gower, C., Barbe, C., Bednarek, N., et al. (2023). Protocol for a prospective multicenter longitudinal randomized controlled trial (CALIN) of sensory-tonic stimulation to foster parent child interactions and social cognition in very premature infants. Front. Pediatr. 10:913396. doi: 10.3389/fped.2022.913396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guyer, A. E., Pérez-Edgar, K., and Crone, E. A. (2018). Opportunities for neurodevelopmental plasticity from infancy through early adulthood. Child Dev. 89, 687–697. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Habibi, A., Cahn, B. R., Damasio, A., and Damasio, H. (2016). Neural correlates of accelerated auditory processing in children engaged in music training. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, J. L., Nacewicz, B. M., Sutterer, M. J., Cayo, A. A., Schaefer, S. M., Rudolph, K. D., et al. (2015). Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 314–323. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardi, F. A., Goetschius, L. G., Peckins, M. K., Brooks-Gunn, J., McLanahan, S. S., McLoyd, V., et al. (2022). Differential developmental associations of material hardship exposure and adolescent amygdala-prefrontal cortex White matter connectivity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 34, 1866–1891. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huttenlocher, P. R., and de Courten, C. (1987). The development of synapses in striate cortex of man. Hum. Neurobiol. 6, 1–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C., et al. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 29, 3019–3025. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ireland, K., Iyer, T. A., and Penhune, V. B. (2019). Contributions of age of start, cognitive abilities and practice to musical task performance in childhood. PLoS One 14:e0216119. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0216119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isbell, E., Stevens, C., Pakulak, E., Hampton Wray, A., Bell, T. A., and Neville, H. J. (2017). Neuroplasticity of selective attention: research foundations and preliminary evidence for a gene by intervention interaction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 114, 9247–9254. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1707241114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaramillo, V., Volk, C., Maric, A., Furrer, M., Fattinger, S., Kurth, S., et al. (2020). Characterization of overnight slow-wave slope changes across development in an age-, amplitude-, and region-dependent manner. Sleep 43:38. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeong, H. J., Durham, E. L., Moore, T. M., Dupont, R. M., McDowell, M., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., et al. (2021). The association between latent trauma and brain structure in children. Transl. Psychiatry 11:240. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01357-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jernigan, T. L., Trauner, D. A., Hesselink, J. R., and Tallal, P. A. (1991). Maturation of human cerebrum observed in vivo during adolescence. Brain 114, 2037–2049. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.5.2037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolles, D., Supekar, K., Richardson, J., Tenison, C., Ashkenazi, S., Rosenberg-Lee, M., et al. (2016b). Reconfiguration of parietal circuits with cognitive tutoring in elementary school children. Cortex 83, 231–245. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolles, D., Wassermann, D., Chokhani, R., Richardson, J., Tenison, C., Bammer, R., et al. (2016a). Plasticity of left perisylvian white-matter tracts is associated with individual differences in math learning. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 1337–1351. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0975-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanemaru, N., Watanabe, H., and Taga, G. (2012). Increasing selectivity of interlimb coordination during spontaneous movements in 2- to 4-month-old infants. Exp. Brain Res. 218, 49–61. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3001-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khundrakpam, B. S., Reid, A., Brauer, J., Carbonell, F., Lewis, J., Ameis, S., et al. (2013). Brain development cooperative group. Developmental changes in organization of structural brain networks. Cereb. Cortex 23:2072. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhl, U., Friederici, A. D., LEGASCREEN consortium, and Skeide, M. A. (2020). Early cortical surface plasticity relates to basic mathematical learning. NeuroImage 204:116235. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., and Brent, H. P. (2003). Expert face processing requires visual input to the right hemisphere during infancy. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 1108–1112. doi: 10.1038/nn1121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C., Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Jones, K. L., Adnams, C. M., May, P. A., et al. (2012). A longitudinal study of the long-term consequences of drinking during pregnancy: heavy in utero alcohol exposure disrupts the normal processes of brain development. J. Neurosci. 32, 15243–15251. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1161-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehto, K., Mäestu, J., Kiive, E., Veidebaum, T., and Harro, J. (2016). BDNF Val66Met genotype and neuroticism predict life stress: a longitudinal study from childhood to adulthood. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26, 562–569. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.12.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackes, N. K., Mehta, M. A., Beyh, A., Nkrumah, R. O., Golm, D., Sarkar, S., et al. (2022). A prospective study of the impact of severe childhood deprivation on brain White matter in adult adoptees: widespread localized reductions in volume but unaffected microstructural organization. eNeuro 9, ENEURO.0188–ENEU22.2022. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0188-22.2022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mareckova, K., Miles, A., Liao, Z., Andryskova, L., Brazdil, M., Paus, T., et al. (2022). Prenatal stress and its association with amygdala-related structural covariance patterns in youth. Neuroimage Clin. 34:102976. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2022.102976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marin, M. M. (2009). Effects of early musical training on musical and linguistic syntactic abilities. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 187–190. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04777.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martel, M., Finos, L., Koun, E., Farnè, A., and Roy, A. C. (2021). The long developmental trajectory of body representation plasticity following tool use. Sci. Rep. 11:559. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-79476-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matejko, A. A., Price, G. R., Mazzocco, M. M., and Ansari, D. (2013). Individual differences in left parietal white matter predict math scores on the preliminary scholastic aptitude test. NeuroImage 66, 604–610. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsudaira, I., Yokota, S., Hashimoto, T., Takeuchi, H., Asano, K., Asano, M., et al. (2016). Parental praise correlates with posterior insular cortex gray matter volume in children and adolescents. PLoS One 11:e0154220. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, S. R., McEwen, L. M., Quirt, J., Morin, A., Mah, S. M., Barr, R. G., et al. (2017). Epigenetic correlates of neonatal contact in humans. Dev. Psychopathol. 29, 1517–1538. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417001213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., and Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1425–1433. doi: 10.1177/0956797611416999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, S., Lee, Y., Janus, M., and Bialystok, E. (2015). Short-term second language and music training induces lasting functional brain changes in early childhood. Child Dev. 86, 394–406. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, S., Marques, C., Santos, A., Santos, M., Castro, S. L., and Besson, M. (2009). Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: more evidence for brain plasticity. Cereb. Cortex 19, 712–723. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, K. M., Beston, B. R., Boley, P. M., and Jones, D. G. (2005). Development of human visual cortex: a balance between excitatory and inhibitory plasticity mechanisms. Dev. Psychobiol. 46, 209–221. doi: 10.1002/dev.20053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, T. V., Gower, P., Albaugh, M. D., Botteron, K. N., Hudziak, J. J., Fonov, V. S., et al. (2016). The developmental relationship between DHEA and visual attention is mediated by structural plasticity of cortico-amygdalar networks. Psychoneuroendocrinology 70, 122–133. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, T. V., Wu, M., Lew, J., Albaugh, M. D., Botteron, K. N., Hudziak, J. J., et al. (2017). Dehydroepiandrosterone impacts working memory by shaping cortico-hippocampal structural covariance during development. Psychoneuroendocrinology 86, 110–121. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Kan, E., and Sowell, E. R. (2012). Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Dev. Sci. 15, 516–527. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01147.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouss, L., Le Normand, M. T., Bailly, K., Leitgel Gille, M., Gosme, C., Simas, R., et al. (2018). Developmental trajectories of hand movements in typical infants and those at risk of developmental disorders: an observational study of kinematics during the first year of life. Front. Psychol. 9:83. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palomar-García, M. Á., Hernández, M., Olcina, G., Adrián-Ventura, J., Costumero, V., Miró-Padilla, A., et al. (2020). Auditory and frontal anatomic correlates of pitch discrimination in musicians, non-musicians, and children without musical training. Brain Struct. Funct. 225, 2735–2744. doi: 10.1007/s00429-020-02151-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paredes, M. F., James, D., Gil-Perotin, S., Kim, H., Cotter, J. A., Ng, C., et al. (2016). Extensive migration of young neurons into the infant human frontal lobe. Science 354:AAF7073. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf7073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, Y., Parker, N., Shin, J., Howard, D., et al. (2021). Virtual histology of cortical thickness and shared neurobiology in 6 psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 78, 47–63. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2694

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penfield, W., and Boldrey, E. (1937). Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain 60, 389–443. doi: 10.1093/brain/60.4.389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penfield, W, and Jasper, H. (1954) Epilepsy and the functional anatomy of the human brain. Oxford, England: Little, Brown & Co, 47, 704

Google Scholar

Penhune, V., Watanabe, D., and Savion-Lemieux, T. (2005). The effect of early musical training on adult motor performance: evidence for a sensitive period in motor learning. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1060, 265–268. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piccolo, L. R., and Noble, K. G., Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics Study (2018). Perceived stress is associated with smaller hippocampal volume in adolescence. Psychophysiology 55:e13025. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pisch, M., Wiesemann, F., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2019). Infant wake after sleep onset serves as a marker for different trajectories in cognitive development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60, 189–198. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, A. J., Collado-Torres, L., Ivanov, N. A., Xia, W., Burke, E. E., Shin, J. H., et al. (2019). Divergent neuronal DNA methylation patterns across human cortical development reveal critical periods and a unique role of CpH methylation. Genome Biol. 20:196. doi: 10.1186/s13059-019-1805-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provenzi, L., Grumi, S., Altieri, L., Bensi, G., Bertazzoli, E., Biasucci, G., et al. (2021). Prenatal maternal stress during the COVID-19 pandemic and infant regulatory capacity at 3 months: a longitudinal study. Dev. Psychopathol. 35, 35–43. doi: 10.1017/S0954579421000766

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pugin, F., Metz, A. J., Wolf, M., Achermann, P., Jenni, O. G., and Huber, R. (2015). Local increase of sleep slow wave activity after three weeks of working memory training in children and adolescents. Sleep 38, 607–614. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puhlmann, L. M., Derome, M., Morosan, L., Kilicel, D., Vrtička, P., and Debbané, M. (2023). Longitudinal associations between self-reported attachment dimensions and neurostructural development from adolescence to early adulthood. Attach Hum. Dev. 25, 162–180. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2021.1993628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, A., Zhang, H., Wang, C., Chong, Y. S., Shek, L. P., Gluckman, P. D., et al. (2021). Canonical TGF-β signaling regulates the relationship between prenatal maternal depression and amygdala development in early life. Transl. Psychiatr. 11:170. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01292-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Remer, J., Croteau-Chonka, E., Dean, D. C., D’Arpino, S., Dirks, H., Whiley, D., et al. (2017). Quantifying cortical development in typically developing toddlers and young children, 1–6 years of age. NeuroImage 153, 246–261. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., and Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Dev. Neuropsychol. 28, 573–594. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rusterholz, T., Hamann, C., Markovic, A., Schmidt, S. J., Achermann, P., and Tarokh, L. (2018). Nature and nurture: brain region-specific inheritance of sleep neurophysiology in adolescence. J. Neurosci. 38, 9275–9285. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0945-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sa de Almeida, J., Lordier, L., Zollinger, B., Kunz, N., Bastiani, M., Gui, L., et al. (2020). Music enhances structural maturation of emotional processing neural pathways in very preterm infants. NeuroImage 207:116391. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saarikivi, K., Putkinen, V., Tervaniemi, M., and Huotilainen, M. (2016). Cognitive flexibility modulates maturation and music-training-related changes in neural sound discrimination. Eur. J. Neurosci. 44, 1815–1825. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savion-Lemieux, T., Bailey, J. A., and Penhune, V. B. (2009). Developmental contributions to motor sequence learning. Exp. Brain Res. 195, 293–306. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1786-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Overy, K., and Winner, E. (2005). Effects of music training on the child's brain and cognitive development. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1060, 219–230. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnack, H. G., van Haren, N. E., Brouwer, R. M., Evans, A., Durston, S., Boomsma, D. I., et al. (2015). Changes in thickness and surface area of the human cortex and their relationship with intelligence. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1608–1617. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoch, S. F., Riedner, B. A., Deoni, S. C., Huber, R., LeBourgeois, M. K., and Kurth, S. (2018). Across-night dynamics in traveling sleep slow waves throughout childhood. Sleep 41:165. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senju, A., Vernetti, A., Ganea, N., Hudry, K., Tucker, L., Charman, T., et al. (2015). Early social experience affects the development of eye gaze processing. Curr. Biol. 25, 3086–3091. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, J., French, L., Xu, T., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Pike, G. B., et al. (2018). Cell-specific gene-expression profiles and cortical thickness in the human brain. Cereb. Cortex 28, 3267–3277. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorrells, S. F., Paredes, M. F., Velmeshev, D., Herranz-Pérez, V., Sandoval, K., Mayer, S., et al. (2019). Immature excitatory neurons develop during adolescence in the human amygdala. Nat. Commun. 10:2748. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-10765-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, C. J., Bailey, J. A., Zatorre, R. J., and Penhune, V. B. (2013). Early musical training and white-matter plasticity in the corpus callosum: evidence for a sensitive period. J. Neurosci. 33, 1282–1290. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3578-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterner, K. N., Weckle, A., Chugani, H. T., Tarca, A. L., Sherwood, C. C., Hof, P. R., et al. (2012). Dynamic gene expression in the human cerebral cortex distinguishes children from adults. PLoS One 7:e37714. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanti, A., Belliveau, C., Nagy, C., Maitra, M., Denux, F., Perlman, K., et al. (2022). Child abuse associates with increased recruitment of perineuronal nets in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex: a possible implication of oligodendrocyte progenitor cells. Mol. Psychiatry 27, 1552–1561. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01372-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Traub, F., and Boynton-Jarrett, R. (2017). Modifiable resilience factors to childhood adversity for clinical pediatric practice. Pediatrics 139:e20162569. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tröndle, M., Popov, T., Dziemian, S., and Langer, N. (2022). Decomposing the role of alpha oscillations during brain maturation. elife 11:e77571. doi: 10.7554/eLife.77571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urbain, C., De Tiège, X., Op De Beeck, M., Bourguignon, M., Wens, V., Verheulpen, D., et al. (2016). Sleep in children triggers rapid reorganization of memory-related brain processes. NeuroImage 134, 213–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandewouw, M. M., Hunt, B. A. E., Ziolkowski, J., and Taylor, M. J. (2021). The developing relations between networks of cortical myelin and neurophysiological connectivity. NeuroImage 237:118142. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaquero, L., Rousseau, P. N., Vozian, D., Klein, D., and Penhune, V. (2020). What you learn & when you learn it: impact of early bilingual & music experience on the structural characteristics of auditory-motor pathways. NeuroImage 213:116689. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116689

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volk, C., Jaramillo, V., Studler, M., Furrer, M., O’Gorman Tuura, R. L., and Huber, R. (2019). Diurnal changes in human brain glutamate + glutamine levels in the course of development and their relationship to sleep. NeuroImage 196, 269–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walther, M., Berweck, S., Schessl, J., Linder-Lucht, M., Fietzek, U. M., Glocker, F. X., et al. (2009). Maturation of inhibitory and excitatory motor cortex pathways in children. Brain Dev 31, 562–567. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2009.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., Weng, J., Yao, Y., Dong, S., Liu, Y., and Chen, F. (2017). Effect of abacus training on executive function development and underlying neural correlates in Chinese children. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 5234–5249. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weder, N., Zhang, H., Jensen, K., Yang, B. Z., Simen, A., Jackowski, A., et al. (2014). Child abuse, depression, and methylation in genes involved with stress, neural plasticity, and brain circuitry. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53:417. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westerhausen, R., Luders, E., Specht, K., Ofte, S. H., Toga, A. W., Thompson, P. M., et al. (2011). Structural and functional reorganization of the corpus callosum between the age of 6 and 8 years. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1012–1017. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whittle, S., Simmons, J. G., Hendriksma, S., Vijayakumar, N., Byrne, M. L., Dennison, M., et al. (2016). Childhood maltreatment, psychopathology, and the development of hippocampal subregions during adolescence. Brain Behav. 7:e00607. doi: 10.1002/brb3.607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wijeakumar, S., Kumar, A., Delgado Reyes, L. M., Tiwari, M., and Spencer, J. P. (2019). Early adversity in rural India impacts the brain networks underlying visual working memory. Dev. Sci. 22:e12822. doi: 10.1111/desc.12822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilhelm, I., Kurth, S., Ringli, M., Mouthon, A. L., Buchmann, A., Geiger, A., et al. (2014). Sleep slow-wave activity reveals developmental changes in experience-dependent plasticity. J. Neurosci. 34, 12568–12575. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0962-14.2014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, P. G., and Lerner, M. A. (2019). COUNCIL ON EARLY CHILDHOOD; COUNCIL ON SCHOOL HEALTH. School readiness. Pediatrics 144:e20191766. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-1766

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wrigglesworth, J., Ryan, J., Vijayakumar, N., and Whittle, S. (2019). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor DNA methylation mediates the association between neighborhood disadvantage and adolescent brain structure. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 285, 51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, M. S., Zdunek, S., Bergmann, O., Bernard, S., Salehpour, M., Alkass, K., et al. (2014). Dynamics of oligodendrocyte generation and myelination in the human brain. Cells 159, 766–774. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zacharopoulos, G., Sella, F., Cohen Kadosh, K., Hartwright, C., Emir, U., and Cohen, K. R. (2021). Predicting learning and achievement using GABA and glutamate concentrations in human development. PLoS Biol. 19:e3001325. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Widaman, K., and Belsky, J. (2021). Beyond orchids and dandelions: susceptibility to environmental influences is not bimodal. Dev. Psychopathol. 35, 191–203. doi: 10.1017/S0954579421000821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, H., Yao, Y., Geng, F., Chen, F., and Hu, Y. (2022). Right fusiform gray matter volume in children with long-term abacus training positively correlates with arithmetic ability. Neuroscience 507, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2022.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuk, J., Vanderauwera, J., Turesky, T., Yu, X., and Gaab, N. (2022). Neurobiological predispositions for musicality: white matter in infancy predicts school-age music aptitude. Dev. Sci. 26:e13365. doi: 10.1111/desc.13365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: development, brain, behavior, childhood, environment, music, stress

Citation: Mucignat-Caretta C and Soravia G (2023) Positive or negative environmental modulations on human brain development: the morpho-functional outcomes of music training or stress. Front. Neurosci. 17:1266766. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1266766

Received: 27 July 2023; Accepted: 18 October 2023;
Published: 03 November 2023.

Edited by:

Raed Mualem, Oranim Academic College, Israel

Reviewed by:

Xueyun Shao, Shenzhen University, China
Ciro De Luca, University of Campania Luigi Vanvitelli, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Mucignat-Caretta and Soravia. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carla Mucignat-Caretta, carla.mucignat@unipd.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.