Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 17 May 2013
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00205

Measuring attention using the Posner cuing paradigm: the role of across and within trial target probabilities

  • Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada

Numerous studies conducted within the recent decades have utilized the Posner cuing paradigm for eliciting, measuring, and theoretically characterizing attentional orienting. However, the data from recent studies suggest that the Posner cuing task might not provide an unambiguous measure of attention, as reflexive spatial orienting has been found to interact with extraneous processes engaged by the task's typical structure, i.e., the probability of target presence across trials, which affects tonic alertness, and the probability of target presence within trials, which affects voluntary temporal preparation. To understand the contribution of each of these two processes to the measurement of attentional orienting we assessed their individual and combined effects on reflexive attention elicited by a spatially nonpredictive peripheral cue. Our results revealed that the magnitude of spatial orienting was modulated by joint changes in the global probability of target presence across trials and the local probability of target presence within trials, while the time course of spatial orienting was susceptible to changes in the probability of target presence across trials. These data thus raise important questions about the choice of task parameters within the Posner cuing paradigm and their role in both the measurement and theoretical attributions of the observed attentional effects.

Introduction

Reflexive orienting acts to interrupt ongoing behavior by rapidly shifting attention toward an unexpected event. In the laboratory, this behavior is typically elicited and measured using the Posner cuing task, where a cue is used to attract participants' attention to a location in space that may contain a response target (e.g., Posner, 1980; Posner and Cohen, 1984). By manipulating the spatial validity between the location of the cue and the location of the target, the Posner cuing task provides a measure of attention by assessing performance for attended targets occurring at the location indicated by the cue (e.g., cued or valid targets) relative to unattended targets occurring at other locations (e.g., uncued or invalid targets). The data generated by this paradigm have contributed immensely to the current understanding of attentional processes, as over the past several decades this procedure has been used to establish a broad base of knowledge across different populations of participants, such as typically developing adults, infants and children, neuropsychological, and psychiatric patients (e.g., Posner et al., 1988; Rafal et al., 1988; Brodeur et al., 1997; Bartolomeo and Chokron, 2002; Bayliss and Tipper, 2006; Ristic and Kingstone, 2009) and various experimental techniques, such as behavioral tasks, EEG, fMRI, and TMS (e.g., Coull and Nobre, 1998; Milliken et al., 2003; Chambers et al., 2004; Channon and Hopfinger, 2011). Furthermore, the data generated by this task have also served as the basis for the development of one of the major theoretical conceptualizations of attentional processes (Posner, 1978; Rafal et al., 1989; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002), establishing the prevailing division between exogenous or reflexive and endogenous or voluntary attentional control (e.g., Jonides, 1981; Berger et al., 2005).

Recent investigations however cast doubt onto the idea that the popular Posner cuing task provides an unambiguous measure of spatial attention (e.g., Tipper and Kingstone, 2005; Gabay and Henik, 2008, 2010). Specifically, extraneous processes that are engaged by the probability of target presence both across and within trials have been found to modulate the spatial attention effects. However, at present it remains unclear whether each of those factors in isolation or in combination influences attentional orienting. To address this question we examined the contribution of across and within trial target probability to reflexive orienting elicited by the Posner cuing task (e.g., Näätänen, 1972; Gabay and Henik, 2008). Understanding the relationship between the parameters of the Posner cuing task and the resultant attentional effects is important for both the theoretical understanding of the measured effects as well as for recognizing the possible limitations of the Posner cuing paradigm as a means of studying spatial orienting in isolation.

The Parameters of the Posner Cuing Task

Since its conception in the 1970's (e.g., Posner, 1978), the Posner cuing task has served as an experimental backbone for eliciting and measuring attention. While over the years researchers have used different variants of this task, a typical sequence of events remains the same: After the presentation of an attentional cue, a target requiring a response is shown at either the cued location or an uncued location after a time delay usually ranging from 100–1000 ms (i.e., cue-target interval). Any performance benefits (e.g., Response Time or RT facilitation) for cued targets are attributed to the cue's ability to engage attention, with the time course of attentional effects revealed by the change in the orienting profile across the cue-target intervals (Klein, 2000; Milliken et al., 2003).

However, in addition to a potential spatial location of the target, the attentional cue in the Posner cuing task also signals the overall probability of target presence across all trials as well as within each trial. The probability of target occurrence across trials is indicated by the rate of overall target presence. Presenting the target on most trials increases participants' readiness to respond to each target event (e.g., Näätänen, 1972) and keeps their level of continuous internal arousal, known as tonic alertness, at a high level (Näätänen, 1970, 1972; Gabay and Henik, 2008; Weinbach and Henik, 2012). When tonic alertness is high, participants' responses are overall fast. In contrast, presenting the target on only a subset of trials decreases participants' overall readiness to respond, lowers their tonic alertness, and leads to overall slower responses (Näätänen, 1972). Thus, the probability of target occurrence across trials affects participants' tonic alertness, such that when targets are presented on most trials a state of high tonic alertness leads to increased readiness to respond relative to when targets are presented on a subset of trials, where a state of low tonic alertness leads to diminished readiness to respond.

The probability of target occurrence within each trial, on the other hand is affected by the number of targets occurring at each successive cue-target interval relative to the number of total remaining trials (Näätänen, 1970, 1972; Gabay and Henik, 2008). When an equal number of targets are presented at each cue-target interval, which is common practice in the Posner cuing task, the probability that a target will occur within a given trial increases as the cue-target interval lengthens. To illustrate, consider an example in which 30 targets are presented at each of four cue-target intervals, and eight trials contain no target, resulting in 128 total trials. Here, the probability of a target appearing at the shortest cue-target interval is p = 0.23 (30 out of 128 trials) but as the cue-target interval lengthens and the number of remaining trials decreases (from 128 total trials at the shortest cue-target interval to 38 remaining trials at the longest cue-target interval) the probability of a target appearing at the longest cue-target increases to p = 0.79 (30 out of 38 remaining trials; see Figures 2A,C for an illustration). When the sequence of events is presented using this type of distribution in which the probability of within trial target occurrence increases with lengthening of cue-target time (often called an “aging” distribution; Näätänen, 1970; Gabay and Henik, 2008), participants form implicit expectations about when, within each trial a target is most likely to occur (e.g., Coull et al., 2000). In turn, this temporal expectancy leads to participants' voluntary temporal preparation to respond, which is indexed by the foreperiod effect and overall shortening of RTs with lengthening of cue-target time (Bertelson, 1967). One can equate the probability of target presence within each trial by using a distribution of trials in which the number of targets assigned to each successive cue-target interval reflects one half of the total remaining trials (often called a “non-aging” distribution; Näätänen, 1970; Gabay and Henik, 2008). Using the previous example, with 128 total trials, the probability of target presence within a trial remains set at p = 0.5 when the number of targets assigned to each successive cue-target interval is 64, 32, 16, and 8 (i.e., 64/128 total remaining trials at the shortest cue-target interval; 32/64 total remaining trials at the next cue-target interval; 16/32 total remaining trials at the following cue-target interval; and 8/16 total remaining trials at the longest cue-target interval; see Figures 2B,D). Because the likelihood of target presence is equated within a trial, participants form no expectations about when within a trial the target is most likely to occur, their RTs become equated for all cue-target intervals, and the foreperiod effect disappears. Thus, the probability of target occurrence within each trial affects participants' voluntary temporal preparation to respond such that increasing the probability of a target's occurrence within a trial induces implicit temporal expectations and faster responses for targets occurring at longer cue-target intervals (i.e., the foreperiod effect).

It is important to note that while the probabilities of target occurrence across and within trials are integral to the Posner cuing paradigm, the two variables have rarely been manipulated and/or directly measured. Indirect evidence however strongly suggests that the cognitive processes that are engaged by those target probabilities, namely tonic alertness and voluntary temporal preparation interact and depend on similar underlying top-down effects (Callejas et al., 2004; Nobre et al., 2007). This opens up a very real possibility that one or both of these processes aid, influence or interact with the manipulation of spatial validity and the resultant attentional effects, which have typically been attributed to spatial attention alone.

The Influence of the Posner Cuing Task Parameters on Reflexive Attention

Reflexive attention has been extensively studied using the Posner cuing task (Posner et al., 1988; Rafal et al., 1988; Bartolomeo and Chokron, 2002; Milliken et al., 2003; Ristic and Kingstone, 2012). Here, reflexive attention is typically engaged by a peripheral luminance onset, which is spatially uninformative about the upcoming target. Typical results, which have been argued to represent an experimental marker of reflexive attention (Chica et al., 2011) reveal short-lived RT facilitation for cued targets and a lasting inhibition emerging between 300 and 500 ms post-cue (i.e., Inhibition of Return or IOR; Posner and Cohen, 1984; Posner et al., 1985), when target detection responses are measured (Lupiáñez et al., 1997).

Recent studies however indicate that both across and within trial target probability modulate these typical reflexive effects. Tipper and Kingstone (2005) reported that changing the probability of target presence across trials eliminated voluntary temporal preparation and resulted in a decreased magnitude of IOR. IOR magnitude in the baseline condition, in which both the cue and the target appeared on every trial was contrasted with the IOR magnitude in the other four conditions in which either the cue or the target was present in 95% or 75% of trials (e.g., 95% Cue, 75% Cue, 95% Target, 75% Target). A typical foreperiod effect, indexing voluntary temporal preparation emerged in all conditions except when the target was infrequently presented in 75% of trials. Further, it was only in this condition that the magnitude of IOR was attenuated by 11 ms (i.e., by 39%) relative to baseline. Dovetailing with other investigations that have indicated susceptibility of IOR to strategic factors (e.g., Mondor, 1999; Milliken et al., 2003), Tipper and Kingstone concluded that while IOR emerges reflexively, it is modulated by voluntary temporal preparation to respond to the target. In other words, the authors argued that the probability of target presence across trials influences both the foreperiod effect and the magnitude of reflexive spatial orienting.

Gabay and Henik (2008) however argued that Tipper and Kingstone's manipulation of reducing the overall number of targets served to lower participants' tonic alertness and not voluntary temporal preparation, as the slowest overall RTs were observed in the critical 75% Target condition (Näätänen, 1970, 1972; Gabay and Henik, 2008). While keeping the probability of target presence across trials at a high 94%, Gabay and Henik (2008) manipulated the probability of target presence within trials by either using the typical procedure in which the probability of a target within trials increased with lengthening of cue-target interval or by using a distribution in which the probability of a target was equated within trials. They found that while the foreperiod effect was once again eliminated when the probability of target presence was equated within a trial, no change in the magnitude of IOR was observed. This suggests that reflexive spatial orienting is resilient to changes in the within trial target probability when the overall probability of target presence across trials is high. Put simply, the probability of target occurrence within trials did not modulate reflexive orienting, but this effect was only found when the targets occurred frequently overall.

One might notice however that while both Tipper and Kingstone's and Gabay and Henik's manipulations resulted in a decreased magnitude of the foreperiod effect, the experimental manipulations leading to this result were very different. Tipper and Kingstone operationalized voluntary temporal preparation as a decrease in the probability of target presence across trials from 95% to 75%. This manipulation resulted in a reduced foreperiod effect and decreased IOR, but those findings might have been due to infrequent overall presentation of the target (i.e., low tonic alertness; Gabay and Henik, 2008). 2008, Gabay and Henik (2010), on the other hand operationalized voluntary temporal preparation as the change in the probability of target presence within trials while keeping the overall probability of target presence across trials high at 94%. While this manipulation also resulted in a decreased foreperiod effect, in contrast to Tipper and Kingstone, no changes in IOR were observed. However, it is still possible that the resilience of IOR found by Gabay and Henik was due to the overall high probability of target presence across all trials (i.e., high tonic alertness). Thus, it appears that in the Posner cuing task, the probability of target presence across and within trials influences the foreperiod effect but it remains unclear whether either of these two factors in isolation or in combination affect reflexive spatial orienting.

The Present Study

We examined the individual and combined influence of across (i.e., tonic alertness) and within trial (i.e., voluntary temporal preparation) target probability. We manipulated across trials target probability in a same manner as Tipper and Kingstone (2005) by presenting targets (1) frequently on a majority of trials (i.e., 94% Target), leading to high tonic alertness, and (2) infrequently on fewer trials (i.e., 75% Target), leading to low tonic alertness. We manipulated within trial target probability in the same manner as Gabay and Henik (2008, 2010) by using a distribution of trials in which the probability of a target within each trial (1) increased with lengthening of cue-target intervals, leading to voluntary temporal preparation, and (2) remained equal across cue-target intervals, leading to diminished voluntary temporal preparation.

If the probability of target presence across trials influences reflexive spatial orienting, modulations of reflexive attention are expected only when targets appear overall less often on 75% of trials. If, on the other hand, the probability of target presence within trials influences reflexive spatial orienting, modulations of reflexive attention are expected only when targets appear with equal chance at each cue-target interval. If across and within trial target probabilities influence reflexive orienting in a combined manner, modulations of attentional effects are expected across our conditions. Finally, if reflexive orienting is independent from the Posner cuing task parameters, no changes in the orienting effects are expected.

Methods

Participants

Forty-four participants completed the experiment. Twenty-two participants (N = 22) were randomly assigned to the high across trials target probability group (94% Target) while the remaining twenty-two participants (N = 22) were randomly assigned to the low across trials target probability group (75% Target).

Apparatus and Stimuli

Stimuli and an example trial sequence are illustrated in Figure 1. The stimuli were black and white line drawings of a fixation dot, two 2.3° × 2.3° squares presented 5.7° of visual angle to the left and right of fixation, and a capital letter “X” (1°) serving as the target. The stimuli were shown on a 16-in CRT monitor.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Example trial sequence. Trials began with the presentation of a fixation point for 750 ms. Then, one of the placeholders brightened for 80 ms. After the variable cue-target interval (i.e., 106, 372, 638, or 931 ms in the high across trials target probability condition or 106 or 931 ms in the low across trials target probability condition), the target (a capital letter “X”) appeared centered in either the left or right placeholder and remained present on the screen until a response was made or 2000 ms had elapsed. The intertrial interval was 652 ms. Note that the stimuli are not drawn to scale.

Design

Across trials target probability (i.e., tonic alertness) was manipulated between-subjects, while within trial target probability (i.e., voluntary temporal preparation), cue-target interval, and cue validity (cued vs. uncued) were manipulated within-subjects. The peripheral cue was spatially nonpredictive, i.e., the target occurred equally often at either of the two spatial locations regardless of the cue's spatial position.

Figures 2AD illustrate the probability of target occurrence across and within trials, and show the number of trials assigned to each cue-target interval. Please note that across and within trial target probabilities were independent.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Illustration of across and within trials target probabilities. The probability of target presence across and within trials is presented along with the number of targets assigned to each cue-target interval. (A) high across trials target probability (i.e., 94% Target) and increasing within trial target probability; (B) high across trials target probability (i.e., 94% Target) and equal within trial target probability; (C) low across trials target probability (i.e., 75% Target) and increasing within trial target probability; and (D) low across trials target probability (i.e., 75% Target) and equal within trial target probability.

Across trials target probability

The probability of target occurrence across trials was manipulated by changing the number of trials in which a target was presented. High across trials target probability, illustrated in Figures 2A,B was implemented by presenting a target in 94% of all trials. This manipulation is known to increase participants' tonic alertness. In contrast, low across trials target probability, shown in Figures 2C,D was implemented by presenting a target in 75% of all trials. This manipulation is known to decrease participants' tonic alertness (e.g., Gabay and Henik, 2008).

Within trial target probability

The probability of target occurrence within trials was manipulated by changing the number of targets presented at each cue-target interval. As depicted in Figures 2A,C, presenting an equal number of targets at each cue-target interval resulted in an increased probability of a target occurring at longer cue-target intervals (c.f., Gabay and Henik, 2008). Figure 2A shows an increasing within trial target probability in the high across trials probability group. Here 128 targets were assigned to each cue-target interval and 32 trials contained no target, for a total of 544 trials. Thus, the probability of a target within a trial increased from p = 0.23 at the shortest cue-target interval of 103 ms (128/544 trials) to p = 0.8 at the longest cue-target interval of 931 ms (128/160 trials). Corresponding within trial target probabilities for the low across trials probability group are depicted in Figure 2C.

Equating the within trial target probability was implemented differently for the high and low across trials target probability groups. This was necessary because the probability of target presence within trials is mathematically determined both by the number of trials that contain a target and the number of individual cue-target intervals (Näätänen, 1970). Thus, for the high across trials target probability group, when targets occurred frequently in 94% of trials, four cue-target intervals of 106, 372, 638, and 931ms were used, as shown in Figure 2B. Here, the number of targets assigned to each successive cue-target interval reflected half of the total remaining trials, equating the within trial target probability to p = 0.5. Thus, 256 targets were presented at the shortest cue-target interval of 106 ms (out of 512 total trials); 128 targets were presented at the cue-target interval of 372 ms (out of 256 remaining trials); 64 targets were presented at the cue-target interval of 638 ms (out of 128 remaining trials), and 32 targets were presented at the cue-target interval of 931 ms (out of 64 remaining trials). The target was not shown in 32 trials (i.e., 6% no-target trials or 94% Target trials). Corresponding within trial target probability for the low across trials probability group when targets occurred infrequently in 75% of trials is shown in Figure 2D. Here, two cue-target intervals of 106 and 931 ms were used, with 160 trials presented at the short cue-target interval of 106 ms (out of 320 total trials) and 80 trials presented at the long cue-target interval of 931 ms (out of 160 remaining trials), thus once again equating the within trial target probability at p = 0.5. The target was not shown in 80 trials (i.e., 25% no-target trials or 75% Target trials). Note that, controlling for any potential differences in the temporal profile of orienting, the range of cue-target intervals (i.e., from 106 to 931ms) was constant across all experimental manipulations (Cheal and Chastain, 2002).

Participants assigned to the high across trials target probability group completed an average of 528 experimental trials while participants assigned to the low across trials target probability group completed an average of 320 experimental trials. Trials were divided over two testing blocks. Within trial target probability conditions were blocked and randomized across participants with all other variables manipulated in a pseudorandom order within each participant. An increased number of trials in the high across trials target probability group was necessary in order to create the required distribution1. Five practice trials were run at the start of the experiment.

Procedure

The experimental sequence of events was consistent with past studies of reflexive orienting (e.g., Posner and Cohen, 1984; Milliken et al., 2003). Each trial began with the presentation of a central fixation dot, which was flanked by two placeholders for 750 ms. Next one of the two peripheral boxes brightened for 80 ms (accomplished through changing the box line thickness from 1 to 5 points). After the variable cue-target interval, the target appeared centered within either the left or the right placeholder, and participants were instructed to press the spacebar as quickly as possible once they detected the onset of the target (Figure 1). The target remained on the screen until a response was made or until 2000 ms had elapsed. RT was measured from target onset. Participants viewed the stimuli from an approximate distance of 57 cm. They were informed that the cue did not predict the location of the target, were asked to maintain central fixation, and to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible.

Results

Anticipatory responses (RT < 100 ms), timed out responses (RT > 1000 ms), false alarms, and incorrect key presses were classified as errors. Overall response accuracy was high, exceeding 98% in each experimental condition. Error rates did not vary between groups and across conditions, as verified by a mixed effects ANOVA conducted on interparticipant mean accuracy as a function of across trials target probability included as a between-subjects variable, and within trial target probability, cue-target interval, and cue validity, included as within-subjects variables. Only a main effect of cue-target interval [F(1, 42) = 5.29, p < 0.05] was found, indicating more accurate responses at the early relative to the late cue-target interval (all other Fs < 4, ps > 0.05).

Mean correct RTs were analyzed at the common cue-target intervals of 106 and 931 ms. This approach facilitated direct comparisons of spatial orienting as a function of both across trials target probability (i.e., tonic alertness) and within trial target probability (i.e., voluntary temporal preparation)2. An omnibus mixed effects ANOVA was used with across trials target probability (high: 94% Target vs. low: 75% Target) included as a between-subjects variable, and within trial target probability (increasing vs. equal), cue-target interval (106 vs. 931 ms), and cue validity (cued vs. uncued) included as within-subjects variables.

Interparticipant mean RTs as a function of across trials target probability (i.e., tonic alertness) and within trials target probability (i.e., voluntary temporal preparation), cue-target interval, and cue validity are shown in Figures 3AD.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. RT results. Mean correct RTs as a function of cue validity (cued vs. uncued) and cue-target interval are shown for each of the four experimental conditions: (A) high across trials target probability (i.e., 94% Target) and increasing within trial target probability; (B) high across trials target probability (i.e., 94% Target) and equal within trial target probability; (C) low across trials target probability (i.e., 75% Target) and increasing within trial target probability; and (D) low across trials target probability (i.e., 75% Target) and equal within trial target probability. Error bars represent the standard error of the difference between the means.

The Effects of Target Probabilities on Spatial Orienting

The key question of interest was whether changes in target probability across and within trials affected reflexive spatial orienting. Our data revealed that both the time course and the magnitude of spatial orienting were modulated. First, overall we found that the peripheral cue elicited an IOR effect, with faster RTs for uncued relative to cued targets [384 vs. 399 ms; cue validity; F(1, 42) = 27.08, p < 0.001; Posner and Cohen, 1984]. When the probability of target presence across trials was high, inhibition was present at both the short and long cue-target interval, as depicted in Figures 3A,B. However, IOR emerged only at the long cue-target interval when the probability of target presence across trials was low, as shown in Figures 3C,D [across trials target probability x cue-target interval × cue validity; F(1, 42) = 8.08, p < 0.01]. Second, the overall magnitude of spatial orienting was reduced from −18.7 to −8.1 ms for low relative to high across trials target probability group when the trial sequence mirrored Tipper and Kingstone's procedure (2005), i.e., when the probability of target presence also increased within trials. In contrast, no reliable changes in the magnitude of reflexive orienting were found (−18.7 to −12.9 ms) when within trial target probability was equated, and the trial sequence mirrored Gabay and Henik's procedure (2008), i.e., when the target also occurred frequently overall [across trials target probability × within trial target probability × and cue validity; F(1, 42) = 5.77, p < 0.05; all other Fs < 2.5, ps > 0.1].

Thus, the probability of target presence across trials affected spatial orienting as a function of the probability of within trial target presence. To probe this finding, we analyzed responses for the high and low across trials target probability groups separately as a function of within trial target probability, cue-target interval, and cue validity. Reliable inhibition at both the short and long cue-target intervals (100 ms: −8.5 ms; 931 ms: −30.1 ms; both ts > 2, ps < 0.05, based on paired, two-tailed t-tests) with no interactions was found when the targets occurred frequently across trials [F(1, 21) = 2.30, p = 0.14]. However, a typical time course of spatial orienting with IOR emerging at the late cue-target interval of 931 ms [100 ms: −3.65 ms; t(43) = −1.0, p > 0.3; 931 ms: −25.2 ms; t(43) = 9.12, p < 0.001, paired, two-tailed t-tests] was found when trials occurred less frequently overall [cue validity × cue-target interval; F(1, 21) = 58.73, p < 0.001].

Therefore, our findings replicate both Tipper and Kingstone (2005) and Gabay and Henik's (2008) results and additionally reveal that the probability of target presence across trials (i.e., tonic alertness) modulates the time course of spatial orienting, but that the joint effect of across and within trial target probabilities modulates the magnitude of spatial orienting.

The Influence of Target Probabilities on the Foreperiod Effect

Next, we examined the influence of target probabilities on the foreperiod effect. While overall the foreperiod effect was not reliable [F < 1.5, p > 0.2], it was modulated by both across and within trial target probability. Specifically, and replicating previous research, the magnitude of the foreperiod effect was reduced when across trials target probability was low [across trial target probability × cue-target interval, F(1, 42) = 20.60, p < 0.001] as depicted in Figures 3C,D, and when the probability of target presence within trials was equated [within trials target probability × cue-target interval, F(1, 42) = 30.43, p < 0.001] as shown in Figures 3B,D. Furthermore, a typical foreperiod effect was observed only when both the probability of target presence across trials was high and the probability of target presence within trials increased with lengthening of cue-target time, as shown in Figure 3A [across trials target probability × within trial target probability × cue-target interval; F(1, 42) = 6.71, p < 0.05]. These parameters correspond to the task settings typically employed in Posner's cuing procedure (e.g., Buckolz and Rodgers, 1980; Niemi and Näätänen, 1981; Posner and Cohen, 1984). Finally while the omnibus ANOVA returned a marginal main effect of tonic alertness (p = 0.1), a direct comparison between overall RTs using an unpaired two-tailed t-test revealed significantly slower RTs when the target was overall less likely to appear in the low across trials probability group (403 ms) relative to when it was overall more likely to appear in the high across trials probability group [380 ms; t(350) = −22.8, p < 0.001]. Thus, once again replicating the data from both Tipper and Kingstone (2005) and Gabay and Henik (2008) within a single experiment, we found that the magnitude of the foreperiod effect was reduced by decreasing the probability of target occurrence both across and within trials.

Magnitudes of Spatial Orienting and the Foreperiod Effect

Finally, we assessed whether the magnitudes of spatial orienting (uncued RT-cued RT) and the foreperiod effect (the average RT at 106 ms—the average RT at 931 ms) varied systematically with changes in target probabilities.

The analysis of orienting magnitudes, illustrated in Figures 4AD, supported the RT data indicating larger overall inhibition with lengthening of cue-target time [−3.8 vs. −22.7 ms; F(1, 42) = 29.16, p < 0.001, see Figures 4AD] with no difference between the magnitudes of orienting with changes in within trial target probability (−13.4 vs. −13.2 ms; F < 1; Figures 4A,C vs. Figures 4B,D). The early inhibitory effect was present only when the targets occurred frequently overall, i.e., under high tonic alertness, as shown in Figures 4A,B [across trials target probability × cue-target interval; F(1, 42) = 8.08, p < 0.01], indicating once again that the overall high probability of target presence across trials influences the time course of reflexive orienting. Finally, increased overall magnitude of inhibition depended on an interaction between processes engaged by across and within trial target probabilities emerging only when tonic alertness was high and voluntary temporal preparation was induced by the task sequence [across trials target probability × within trial target probability; F(1, 42) = 5.77, p < 0.05; see Figure 4A].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Magnitudes of orienting and the foreperiod effect. Magnitudes of orienting (uncued RT—cued RT) and the foreperiod effect (average RT at the 106 ms cue-target interval—average RT at the 931 ms cue-target interval) are shown for each of the four experimental conditions: (A) high across trials target probability (i.e., 94% Target) and increasing within trial target probability; (B) high across trials target probability (i.e., 94% Target) and equal within trial target probability; (C) low across trials target probability (i.e., 75% Target) and increasing within trial target probability; and (D) low across trials target probability (i.e., 75% Target) and equal within trial target probability. Error bars represent the standard error of the difference between the means.

The magnitude of the foreperiod effect was reduced when targets appeared less often overall [14.5 vs. −14.6 ms; across trials target probability; F(1, 42) = 15.56, p < 0.001; Figures 4A,B vs. Figures 4C,D] and when the probability of target occurrence was equated within trials [16.5 vs. −16.9 ms; within trial target probability; F(1, 42) = 58.53, p < 0.001; Figures 4A,C vs. Figures 4B,D]. The most pronounced decline in the magnitude of the foreperiod effect was found when both target probabilities were reduced, as shown in Figure 4D. This strongly suggests that both the probability of target presence across trials, affecting tonic alertness, and the probability of target presence within trials, affecting voluntary temporal preparation, modulate the foreperiod effect.

Taken together, our results indicate that a typical sequence of events in the Posner cuing task, in which the targets appear frequently overall and the probability of target occurrence within a trial increases with lengthening of cue-target time, leads to interactions between tonic alertness and voluntary temporal preparation. This in turn critically modulates both the time course and the magnitude of the measured attentional effects as well as the magnitude of the foreperiod effect.

Discussion

Motivated by the disparate results showing modulations of reflexive spatial orienting by the processes engaged by across and within trial target probabilities (Tipper and Kingstone, 2005; Gabay and Henik, 2008), we investigated whether systematic changes in those two parameters affected the resultant measure of reflexive attention. We manipulated across trials target probability and within trial target probability in isolation and in conjunction, and measured reflexive spatial orienting elicited by a spatially nonpredictive peripheral onset. Our investigation revealed three key findings, which collectively indicate that tonic alertness and voluntary temporal preparation induced by the parameters of Posner's cuing task critically influence the observed attentional effects, which have typically been attributed to spatial orienting alone.

First, we found that an interaction between processes engaged by across and within trial target probabilities modulates spatial orienting. Overall we found that inhibition was reliable across all experimental conditions. However, the magnitude of inhibition was significantly reduced (i.e., by 10.6 ms) when targets appeared less frequently overall under low tonic alertness. However, this finding held only when at the same time the probability of target occurrence increased within trials. This replicates Tipper and Kingstone (2005), who reported an equal reduction in IOR magnitude (from approximately 30 to 19 ms, cf. Figure 2, Tipper and Kingstone, 2005) using the same manipulation. We also found that overall IOR magnitude was unaffected by the changes in within trial target probability. However, this finding held only when the targets appeared frequently overall, replicating Gabay and Henik's (2008) findings. Extending both of those studies we also found that when the probabilities of target presence across and within trials were reduced in conjunction, the magnitude of spatial orienting was also reduced. Therefore, while in general inhibition appears to be resilient to changes in target probability across and within trials, its magnitude depends on interactions between the processes engaged by those probabilities, i.e., tonic alertness and voluntary temporal preparation.

Second, changes in across trials target probability, or tonic alertness modulated the time course of spatial orienting. Specifically, when targets appeared infrequently in 75% of trials in the low alertness condition, we observed a typical time course of orienting with late emerging IOR. However, an unusual pattern of early and prolonged inhibition was found when targets appeared frequently overall in 94% of trials. This suggests that the inhibitory process might be aided by high tonic alertness, emerging earlier and in a larger magnitude relative to when tonic alertness is reduced. Thus, maintaining high tonic alertness within the Posner cuing task by presenting the response target on the majority of trials appears to fundamentally influence how participants interpret the attentional cue, insofar as the removal of an overall expectancy of a target's presence changes the observed time course of spatial orienting. Conversely, our result showing a typical time course of IOR when targets appeared infrequently in the low alertness case suggests that participants in this condition have either utilized their executive processes to a larger extent because the task required an increased use of response inhibition due to the infrequent presentation of the target, or that they have deployed strategic expectancies in developing inhibitory responses at the long cue-target intervals despite the target's intermittent occurrence. The development of these cognitive expectancies may in turn act to suppress reflexive behaviors like early facilitation, which dovetails with recent results indicating earlier emergence of IOR with increases in tonic alertness (Gabay et al., 2011) and decreases of task difficulty (Van Der Lubbe et al., 2005). It is important to note that unlike IOR, early facilitation is a fragile phenomenon, and has been found to depend on the physical properties of the display (Samuel and Kat, 2003). Thus, the absence of early facilitation in our experiment and perhaps in other studies may reflect its suppression by strategic processes involved in tracking across and within trial target probabilities. Finally, it is also possible that early facilitation and IOR constitute opposing independent processes (e.g., Tassinari et al., 1994; Danziger and Kingstone, 1998) whereby IOR is mediated by strategic factors (e.g., Milliken et al., 2003) and early facilitation is driven by the cue's physical properties (Pratt et al., 2001; Samuel and Kat, 2003), which might come to the fore when strategic processes are reduced, as it is the case when across trials target probability, i.e., tonic alertness is low. All of these possibilities however suggest a tight coupling between tonic alertness and reflexive spatial orienting, and as such the contributions of each factor within the Posner cuing task should be explicitly examined and dissociated in future studies. In other words, the time course of reflexive attention as measured by the Posner cuing task is directly influenced by the choice of across trials target probability irrespectively of the cue's spatial validity.

Third, we found that the magnitude of the foreperiod effect was modulated by both across and within trial target probability. The foreperiod effect is generally thought to reflect a decreased uncertainty in responding when an advanced warning signal is given to participants (Bertelson, 1967; Niemi and Näätänen, 1981). However, we also found that an overall decrease in target probability across trials affected the foreperiod effect. This finding is perhaps due to the fact that in the Posner cuing task the same cue event serves as an indicator of both across and within trial target probability (Weinbach and Henik, 2012). That is, it appears that the controlled processes, which are driven by those target probabilities, i.e., tonic alertness and voluntary temporal preparation also mediate the foreperiod effect (Callejas et al., 2005). This is supported by our result indicating a typical foreperiod effect only when targets appeared overall more frequently leading to high tonic alertness and when the target had an increased probability of occurring with lengthening of cue-target time, while the most pronounced reduction in its magnitude was observed when both of these variables were experimentally reduced.

Thus, the choice of task parameters within the Posner cuing paradigm, specifically those that determine across and within trial target probabilities, fundamentally influences the observed attentional effects. That is, we were able to modulate both the magnitude and the time course of spatial attention by changing the probabilities of target occurrence while keeping the spatial validity of the cue constant. In particular, we found that the magnitude of spatial orienting was affected by an interaction between tonic alertness engaged by across trials target probability and voluntary temporal preparation engaged by within trial target probability while the time course of orienting was susceptible to changes in tonic alertness only. Furthermore, we found that the typical foreperiod effect also depended on both tonic alertness and voluntary temporal preparation, suggesting that these two processes share underlying cognitive and executive processes (e.g., Callejas et al., 2004) rather than constituting independent effects (e.g., Fan et al., 2002). These two key results pose interpretational issues for understanding the attentional effects measured by the Posner cuing task and question the attributions of those effects to reflexive or voluntary attentional control (e.g., Posner and Cohen, 1984; Müller and Rabbitt, 1989). That is, it still remains unclear to what extent the magnitude and the time course of reflexive spatial orienting elicited by the Posner cuing task can be attributed to attentional orienting versus processes engaged by the task parameters. One possible reason for the mutual process interference is the fact that in the Posner's cuing task an attentional cue serves multiple purposes—acting as a spatial cue, an alerting signal, and a temporal warning signal. An important aim for future investigations is to dissociate the effects of spatial orienting, tonic alertness, and voluntary temporal preparation within the Posner cuing task, rather than using a set of different tasks (e.g., Fan et al., 2002) by employing distinct events to convey each type of information (see Lawrence and Klein, 2012). The outstanding question is whether and how dissociating the individual contributions of each of these processes will affect the measurement of spatial attention and the resultant theoretical conceptualizations of the attentional effects and their associated control demands (i.e., reflexive and voluntary processes).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Marilena Côté-Lecaldare for her help with data collection. This research was supported by a graduate fellowship awarded to Dana A. Hayward from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), and by grant support to Jelena Ristic from NSERC, G. W., Stairs, and William Dawson funds.

Footnotes

  1. ^The magnitude of spatial orienting has been found to vary with increased trial sampling (e.g., Weaver et al., 1998; Pratt and McAuliffe, 1999; Lupiáñez et al., 2001). To examine whether this was the case in our data, we analyzed RTs as a function of the testing block (1st vs. 2nd), with across trials target probability included as a between-subjects variable, and within trial target probability, cue-target interval, and cue validity included as within-subjects variables. Two effects involving testing block were reliable. First, we found a larger reduction in the foreperiod effect in the second block [testing block × cue-target interval; F(1, 42) = 6.1, p < 0.05]. Second, we found an increased magnitude of IOR in the second block, but only when targets occurred infrequently in 75% of trials [testing block × across trials target probability × and cue validity; F(1, 42) = 8, p < 0.01]. Importantly, a similar finding was not observed when the targets occurred frequently in 94% of trials, which is the condition with more experimental trials (all other effects involving testing block Fs < 1.4, ps > 0.2). Thus, any differences in spatial orienting as a function of across trials target probability are not due to the increased number of trials in the high across trials probability case but likely reflect the development of executive strategies over time when targets occur infrequently.
  2. ^The same data pattern held when all cue-target intervals were included in the analyses conducted separately for high vs. low across trials target probability groups. When targets occurred frequently in 94% of trials, a foreperiod effect was present [F (3, 63) = 14.87, p < 0.001], participants detected uncued targets faster than cued targets [F(1, 21) = 30.44, p < 0.001], and responded overall faster when the probability of a target increased within trials [F(1, 21) = 4.67, p < 0.05]. The magnitude of the foreperiod effect decreased when target probability was equated within trials [within trial target probability × cue-target interval; F(3, 63) = 18.78, p < 0.001] while the IOR effect emerged early and grew in magnitude with cue-target time [cue validity × cue-target interval; F(3, 63) = 6, p < 0.01; All other Fs < 1.8, ps > 0.15]. When targets occurred infrequently in 75% of trials, the foreperiod effect was reduced [F(1, 21) = 16.50, p < 0.001], while spatial orienting [F(1, 21) = 13.31, p < 0.01] did not vary as a function of within trial target probability (p > 0.2). Once again, equating the probability of target presence within trials decreased the magnitude of the foreperiod effect [within trial target probability × cue-target interval; F(1, 21) = 6.13, p < 0.05] while the IOR effect emerged at the late cue-target interval only [cue validity × cue-target interval; F(1, 21) = 58.73, p < 0.001; All other Fs < 3, ps > 0.1].

References

Bartolomeo, P., and Chokron, S. (2002). Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 217–234.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bayliss, A. P., and Tipper, S. P. (2006). Predictive gaze cues and personality judgments: should eye trust you? Psychol. Sci. 17, 514–520.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berger, A., Henik, A., and Rafal, R. D. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 134, 207–221.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bertelson, P. (1967). Time course of preparation. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 19, 272–279.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brodeur, D., Trick, L., and Enns, J. T. (1997). Selective Attention Over the Lifespan. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Buckolz, E., and Rodgers, R. (1980). The influence of catch trial frequency on simple reaction time. Acta Psychol. 44, 191–200.

Callejas, A., Lupiáñez, J., Funes, M. J., and Tudela, P. (2005). Modulations among the alerting, orienting and executive control networks. Exp. Brain Res. 167, 27–37.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Callejas, A., Lupiáñez, J., and Tudela, P. (2004). The three attentional networks: on their independence and interactions. Brain Cogn. 54, 225–227.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chambers, C. D., Payne, J. M., Stokes, M. G., and Mattingley, J. B. (2004). Fast and slow parietal pathways mediate spatial attention. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 217–218.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Channon, V. W., and Hopfinger, J. B. (2011). ERPs reveal similar effects of social gaze orienting and voluntary attention, and distinguish each from reflexive attention. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 2502–2513.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cheal, M., and Chastain, G. (2002). Timing of facilitatory and inhibitory effects of visual attention. Vis. Cogn. 9, 969–1002.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Chica, A. B., Bartolomeo, P., and Valero-Cabre, A. (2011). Dorsal and ventral parietal contributions to spatial orienting in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 31, 8143–8149.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Buchel, C., and Nobre, A. C. (2000). Orienting attention in time: behaviour and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsychologia 38, 808–819.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coull, J. T., and Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. J. Neurosci. 18, 7426–7435.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Danziger, S., and Kingstone, A. (1998). Unmasking the inhibition of return phenomenon. Percept. Psychophys. 61, 1024–1037.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340–347.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gabay, S., and Henik, A. (2008). The effects of expectancy on inhibition of return. Cognition 106, 1478–1486.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gabay, S., and Henik, A. (2010). Temporal expectancy modulates inhibition of return in a discrimination task. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 17, 47–51.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gabay, S., Pertzov, Y., and Henik, A. (2011). Orienting of attention, pupil size, and the norepinephrine system. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73, 123–129.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jonides, J. (1981). “Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement,” in Attention and Performance, eds J. B. Long and A. D. Baddeley (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 187–203.

Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 138–147.

Lawrence, M. A., and Klein, R. M. (2012). Isolating exogenous and endogenous modes of temporal attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 560–572.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lupiáñez, J., Milán, E. G., Tornay, F. J., Madrid, E., and Tudela, P. (1997). Does IOR occur in discrimination tasks? Yes it does, but later. Percept. Psychophys. 59, 1241–1254.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Lupiáñez, J., Weaver, B., Tipper, S. P., and Madrid, C. (2001). The effects of practice on cueing in detection and discrimination tasks. Psicológica 22, 1–23.

Milliken, B., Lupiáñez, J., Roberts, M., and Stevanovski, B. (2003). Orienting in space and time: joint contributions to exogenous spatial cuing effects. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 10, 877–883.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mondor, T. A. (1999). Predictability of the cue-target relation and the time-course of auditory inhibition of return. Percept. Psychophys. 61, 1501–1509.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Müller, H. J., and Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1989). Spatial cueing and the relation between the accuracy of “where” and “what” decisions in visual search. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 41, 747–773.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Näätänen, R. (1970). The diminishing time-uncertainty with the lapse of time after the warning signal in reaction-time experiments with varying fore-periods. Acta Psychol. 34, 399–419.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Näätänen, R. (1972). Time uncertainty and occurrence uncertainty of the stimulus in a simple reaction time task. Acta Psychol. 36, 492–503.

Niemi, P., and Näätänen, R. (1981). Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychol. Bull. 89, 133–162.

Nobre, A. C., Correa, A., and Coull, J. T. (2007). The hazards of time. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 465–470.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric Explorations of Mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32, 3–25.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Posner, M. I., and Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of Visual Orienting. Nillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Posner, M. I., Early, T. S., Reiman, E., Pardo, P. J., and Dhawan, M. (1988). Asymmetries in hemispheric control of attention in Schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 45, 814–821.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Posner, M. I., Rafal, R. D., Choate, L. S., and Vaughn, J. (1985). Inhibition of return: neural basis and function. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2, 211–228.

Pratt, J., Hillis, J., and Gold, J. M. (2001). The effect of the physical characteristics of cues and targets on facilitation and inhibition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 8, 489–495.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Pratt, J., and McAuliffe, J. (1999). Examining the effect of practice on inhibition of return in static displays. Percept. Psychophys. 61, 756–765.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rafal, R. D., Calabresi, P. A., Brennan, C. W., and Sciolto, T. K. (1989). Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 15, 673–685.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rafal, R. D., Posner, M. I., Friedman, J. H., Inhoff, A. W., and Bernstein, E. (1988). Orienting of visual attention in progressive supranuclear palsy. Brain 111, 267–280.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ristic, J., and Kingstone, A. (2009). Rethinking attentional development: reflexive and volitional orienting in children and adults. Dev. Sci. 12, 289–296.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ristic, J., and Kingstone, A. (2012). A new form of human spatial attention: automated symbolic orienting. Vis. Cogn. 20, 244–264.

Samuel, A. G., and Kat, D. (2003). Inhibition of return: a graphical meta-analysis of its time course and an empirical test of its temporal and spatial properties. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 10, 897–906.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tassinari, G., Aglioti, S., Chelazzi, L., Peru, A., and Berlucchi, G. (1994). Do peripheral non-informative cues induce early facilitation of target detection? Vision Res. 34, 179–189.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tipper, C., and Kingstone, A. (2005). Is inhibition of return a reflexive effect? Cognition 97, B55–B62.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Der Lubbe, R. H. J., Vogel, R. O., and Postma, A. (2005). Different effects of exogeneous cues in a visual detection and discrimination task: delayed attention withdrawal and/or speeded motor inhibition? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1829–1840.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weaver, B., Lupiáñez, J., and Watson, F. L. (1998). The effects of practice on object-based, location-based and static display inhibition of return. Percept. Psychophys. 60, 993–1003.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Weinbach, N., and Henik, A. (2012). Temporal orienting and alerting—the same or different? Front. Psychol. 3:236. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00236

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: Posner cuing task, spatial orienting, tonic alertness, voluntary temporal preparation, reflexive attention

Citation: Hayward DA and Ristic J (2013) Measuring attention using the Posner cuing paradigm: the role of across and within trial target probabilities. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:205. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00205

Received: 12 February 2013; Accepted: 02 May 2013;
Published online: 17 May 2013.

Edited by:

John J. Foxe, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA

Reviewed by:

Redmond O'Connell, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
John I. Broussard, Baylor College of Medicine, USA

Copyright © 2013 Hayward and Ristic. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Dana A. Hayward and Jelena Ristic, Department of Psychology, McGill University, 1205 Dr Penfield Ave., Montreal, QC H3A1B1, Canada. e-mail: dana.hayward@mail.mcgill.ca; jelena.ristic@mcgill.ca

Download