Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 26 November 2019
Sec. Motivation and Reward
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00257

The Sexual Motivation of Male Rats as a Tool in Animal Models of Human Health Disorders

  • 1Department of Experimental and Clinical Physiology, Laboratory of Centre for Preclinical Research, Medical University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
  • 2Department of Biophysics and Human Physiology, Medical University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland

Normal or dysfunctional sexual behavior seems to be an important indicator of health or disease. Many health disorders in male patients affect sexual activity by directly causing erectile dysfunction, affecting sexual motivation, or both. Clinical evidence indicates that many diseases strongly disrupt sexual motivation and sexual performance in patients with depression, addiction, diabetes mellitus and other metabolic disturbances with obesity and diet-related factors, kidney and liver failure, circadian rhythm disorders, sleep disturbances including obstructive sleep apnea syndrome, developmental and hormonal disorders, brain damages, cardiovascular diseases, and peripheral neuropathies. Preclinical studies of these conditions often require appropriate experimental paradigms, including animal models. Male sexual behavior and motivation have been intensively investigated over the last 80 years in animal rat model. Sexual motivation can be examined using such parameters as: anticipatory behavior and 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations reflecting the emotional state of rats, initiation of copulation, efficiency of copulation, or techniques of classical (pavlovian) and instrumental conditioning. In this review article, we analyze the behavioral parameters that describe the sexual motivation and sexual performance of male rats in the context of animal experimental models of human health disorders. Based on analysis of the parameters describing the heterogeneous and complex structure of sexual behavior in laboratory rodents, we propose an approach that is useful for delineating distinct mechanisms affecting sexual motivation and sexual performance in selected disease states and the efficacy of therapy in preclinical investigations.

Introduction

Sexual interaction has been one of the most intensively studied appetitive behaviors over the last 80 years. Copulation differs between species, but detailed investigation of the mechanisms regulating the behavior of one species seems to be important from the perspective of comparative physiological research. Furthermore, effective sexual interactions involve activation of a sequence of behavioral patterns that depend on distinct brain structures, neural networks, and neurotransmitters. The amygdala (A), bed nucleus of stria terminalis (BNST), medial preoptic area (MPOA), and central tegmental field/subparafascicular nucleus of the thalamus constitute the core central structures. They connect with the dopaminergic mesolimbical, mesocortical, and nigrostriatal tracts, lateral and ventromedial hypothalamus, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus, ventral premammilary nucleus, midbrain periaqueductal gray, nucleus paragigantocellularis of the medulla, and autonomic regions of the spinal cord and regulate sexual motivation, arousal, and copulatory performance. Detailed analysis of neural networks and neurotransmitters in the context of sexual behavior is outlined in several recent reviews (Hull and Rodríguez-Manzo, 2017; Hill and Elias, 2018; Seizert, 2018; Le Moëne and Ågmo, 2019).

With this background, analysis of the sexual activity of laboratory rodents provides a powerful experimental tool for studying the inheritable traits, endocrine factors, neurotransmitter systems, and neural networks involved in evolutionarily preserved as well as experience-dependent aspects of behavior.

In this review article, we analyze the behavioral parameters describing the sexual motivation and sexual performance of male rats in the context of health disorders in humans. Based on analysis of parameters describing the heterogeneous and complex structure of sexual behavior in laboratory rodents, we propose an approach that is useful for delineating distinct mechanisms affecting sexual motivation and sexual performance in disease states and the efficacy of therapy in preclinical investigations. Furthermore, we argue that this approach could be applied for more precise determination of specific mechanisms involved in abnormal or disturbed sexual behavior in rats that are translationally related to human health disorders. In particular, translational research in rodent models of sexual behavior has provided important insights into the pathomechanisms and pharmacotherapy of clinical conditions that are described in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th edition (DSM 5) and in the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th revision (ICD-10), including premature ejaculation, paraphilias, mood and anxiety disorders as well as neurological and metabolic diseases (McVary et al., 1997; Grønli et al., 2005; Giuliano and Clément, 2006; Hawley et al., 2013; Pfaus et al., 2013; Kang et al., 2014; Olayo-Lortia et al., 2014; Sanna et al., 2014; Faulkner et al., 2015; Babaei-Balderlou and Khazali, 2016; Oosting et al., 2016; Ramírez-Rodríguez et al., 2017; Hernández and Fernández-Guasti, 2018; Novati et al., 2018). In this light, we propose that in various rodent models of human disease states, sexual motivation and performance may be differently affected, which is reflected in distinct changes of specific components of male rat sexual behavior. However, this translational potential of animal models of sexual behavior for investigating human disorders should be exploited cautiously, as not all aspects of sexual behavior and health disorders are identical in rats and humans (Le Moëne and Ågmo, 2019). Here, we present an outline of male rat sexual behavior in the context of rodent models of human diseases, which should be helpful in finding appropriate experimental models for evaluation of pathomechanisms, therapeutic interventions, and alternatives to the current therapies in preclinical studies. Owing to the specificities and differences of male and female sexual motivation and behavior under physiological conditions and in health disorders (Pfaff, 2017; Hill and Elias, 2018), we did not analyze female sexual behavior in the review.

Sexual Behavior as an Experimental Model

The sexual behavior of male rats consists of the anticipatory stage during which a male searches for a receptive female, followed by an initiation stage during which a male and a female show mutual investigation. At the end of the initiatory stage, rats begin to copulate. Female behaviors, including sex-soliciting behavior, receptivity, and occurrence of the lordosis reflex (measured as % displaying lordosis), influence the initiation stage and copulatory performance, as lordosis allows for intromission. Copulation comprises specific highly stereotypical motor patterns, including mounting, intromission, and ejaculation. Mounting is a pattern when a male lifts his forebody over the female hindquarters, clasping her flanks with his forepaws, and begins a series of rapid shallow movements of the pelvis. When the glans penis detects the vagina, a male can perform rapid erection, with a deeper intravaginal thrust, which is followed by immediate dismounting. This mounting-intromission-dismounting pattern is repeated until ejaculation is achieved. Ejaculation is marked by a long-lasting intromission (about 1–2 s), which a male rat usually achieves after a few to a dozen intromissions. Rats usually copulate for up to eight ejaculations until copulatory satiation (Larsson, 1956; Sachs and Barfield, 1976). During all these stages, rats produce a complex series of ultrasound vocalizations of various frequencies and temporal patterns (Barfield et al., 1979). Furthermore, fully expressed sexual behavior requires both sexual motivation and sexual arousal, which should be treated as two distinct phenomena (Sachs, 2000). Sexual arousal depends on the activation of brain networks within the brainstem that simultaneously control behavioral and autonomic nervous system responses during sexual interaction and is mainly manifested by penile erection, whereas sexual motivation drives and maintains subsequent stages of sexual behavior (Schober et al., 2011; Ågmo, 2011). Differences between sexual motivation and sexual arousal, or more precisely the fact that these processes are not interchangeable or equivalent, can be explained by the analysis of non-copulating male rats or asexual orientation in humans. In healthy non-copulating rats, noncontact erections are present during exposure to the receptive female in spite of the absence of an attempt to copulate, which is especially visible after medial preoptic lesions (Stefanick and Davidson, 1987; Liu et al., 1997; Portillo and Paredes, 2019). Similar dissociation between sexual arousal and sexual motivation can be seen in asexually oriented men. The level of masturbation in healthy asexual men is similar to that in heterosexual counterparts, but with no motivation for either hetero or homosexual interactions (Brotto et al., 2010; Portillo and Paredes, 2019).

The sexual behavior of a male rat contains both inheritable arousal-activated neuronal networks and networks that are experience-dependent and modified by classical and instrumental conditioning (Pfaus et al., 2001; Hull and Rodríguez-Manzo, 2017). Based on statistical factor analysis of sexual behavior in copulatory experienced rats, five independent components of sexual behavior have been distinguished: anticipatory, initiation, rate of copulation, number of intromissions, and intromission ratio (IR; Sachs, 1978; Pfaus et al., 1990). All five components of sexual interactions are summarized in Figure 1 and discussed below from the perspectives of sexual motivation, general arousal, and sexual arousal.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Components—factors of male sexual behavior in relation to sexual motivation, general arousal, and sexual arousal. Anticipatory behavior is related to sexual motivation and general arousal. The initiation phase is powered by sexual motivation and leads to the enhancement of sexual arousal necessary to evoke penile erection. Anxiety inhibits this phase. Copulation rate is related to the reciprocally augmented level of sexual motivation and sexual arousal as well as to general arousal. NI, number of intromissions, is reciprocally related to sexual arousal accumulation and affects the copulatory rate. IR, intromission ratio, is dependent on erectile function/dysfunction and strongly affects copulatory efficiency.

Anticipatory Behavior

In standard laboratory procedure, a male rat is usually placed in the experimental chamber for 5 min before introduction of a receptive female (Larsson, 1956). During this time, anticipatory behavior is measured by intensiveness of chamber exploration, number of rearings, or changes of levels in special bi-level apparatus (Mendelson and Gorzalka, 1987; Mendelson and Pfaus, 1989). In sexually experienced males, intensiveness of exploration with looking for cues from a female co-occurs with intensive ultrasonic vocalizations in the 50-kHz band, termed precontact vocalizations (PVs; Bialy et al., 2000). Ultrasounds emitted by rodents, in addition to olfactory cues, are a signal for identification of individuals (Holy and Guo, 2005; Asaba et al., 2014). Ultrasonic vocalizations in the 50-kHz band reflect the emotional state of rodents and are related to the activation of the nucleus accumbens (Hamed et al., 2016; Mulvihill and Brudzynski, 2018). Ultrasounds also cause rats to react by approaching a sound source (Wöhr and Schwarting, 2007; Pultorak et al., 2016). The number of PVs during the acquisition of sexual experience is related to the level of sexual experience and conditioning to odor cues and conditioning stimuli (CS) from the experimental chamber, and it depends on the rewarding value of sexual contacts (Bialy et al., 2000).

PVs and other elements of anticipatory behavior can be completely suppressed by repeated dopamine D1 receptor activation in the nucleus accumbens without significant effects on subsequent copulatory behavior (Bialy et al., 2010). Furthermore, increase in the number of PVs during the acquisition of sexual experience is inhibited by NMDA antagonists but is not related to the initiation of copulation measured by mount latency (ML; see below; Bialy et al., 2000). These observations indicate that anticipatory behavior depends on different neural networks than initiation and copulatory behaviors.

In the sexual context, penile erection is treated as an indicator of an elevated level of sexual arousal (Sachs, 2000). However, our studies indicate that penile erection is not observed during anticipatory behavior (Bialy et al., 2000, 2010). This implies that the anticipatory behavior depends on stimulation of the general arousal system and motivation to look for cues related to sexual activity rather than on the sexual arousal itself.

Initiation Behavior

The time between exposure of a male rat to a receptive female and the first mounting determines the length of the initiation stage and is described as ML. When a female is introduced, both male and female show mutual investigation and mutually emit ultrasonic vocalizations. Odor, visual, auditory, and tactile cues enhance both the level of sexual motivation, leading to copulatory behavior, and the level of sexual arousal, making it sufficient to evoke erection and effective intromission (Hull and Rodríguez-Manzo, 2017). ML depends on sexual motivation enhanced by mutual male-female investigation. In addition to the motivational aspect measured by ML, the latency between the introduction of a female to the first intromission, termed intromission latency (IL), indicates the time required to reach a sufficiently high level of sexual arousal to induce erection (Hull and Rodríguez-Manzo, 2017). ML is prolonged in sexually naïve males and is significantly shorter in sexually experienced rats. Sexual experience and conditioning to cues from a female or experimental chamber significantly reduce both ML and IL (Larsson, 1959; Dewsbury, 1969; Bialy et al., 2000).

The initiation of copulation is strongly related to sexual motivation, and it is inhibited by an enhanced level of anxiety (Pfaus and Wilkins, 1995; Miwa et al., 2011). Activation of cAMP-response element-binding protein (CREB) in the nucleus accumbens reduces anxiety level and ML, but it has no effect on copulatory efficiency as measured by ejaculation latency (EL; Barrot et al., 2005). Similarly, acute administration of D1 receptor agonist into the nucleus accumbens significantly increases the percentage of sexually naïve males that display mounting, but without an increase in PVs or shortening of EL (Bialy et al., 2010). ML is also dramatically prolonged by lesion of the anterior cingulate cortex (Ågmo et al., 1995), suggestive of a critical role for the nucleus accumbens–anterior cingulate cortex/medial prefrontal cortex network in sexual motivation and initiation of a new behavior (Bialy et al., 2010; Sanna et al., 2017). Sexual motivation can also be described by approach behavior in the sexual incentive motivation test arena (Le Moëne and Ågmo, 2019).

Copulatory Efficiency (Copulatory Rate Factor)

Sexual motivation during copulation controls the behavior directed towards the satisfaction of sexual drive by achieving intromissions and ejaculations. Copulatory efficiency describes the ability to satisfy the sexual drive and depends on sexual experience (Larsson, 1959; Dewsbury, 1969; Bialy et al., 2000). Simultaneously with sexual motivation, adequate sexual arousal has to be achieved and accumulated to elicit penile erection and ejaculation, which are mediated by activation of the autonomic nervous system (Giuliano and Rampin, 2004).

The most important measure of copulatory efficiency is EL, which describes the time from first intromission to ejaculation. Additionally, copulatory efficiency can be measured by inter-intromission interval (III), which provides the mean time between intromissions before each ejaculation (Sachs and Barfield, 1976; Sachs, 1978).

Acquisition of sexual experience has been shown to involve different neuronal networks in copulatory efficiency and in the initiation of copulation (Bialy et al., 2000, 2010). The key neural network that regulates copulatory rate involves connections between the amygdala, BNST, central tegmental field, and MPOA (Hull and Rodríguez-Manzo, 2017). MPOA is one of the brain structures in which acquisition of sexual experience leads to an increase in Fos expression, and higher levels of neurotransmitters, receptors, or enzymes and hormones important for regulation of sexual activity (Hull and Rodríguez-Manzo, 2017). Specifically, it was shown that acquisition of sexual experience is associated with an increase in nitric oxide with higher levels of nitric oxide synthase (Dominguez et al., 2006), glutamate and dopamine (Will et al., 2014), D1 receptor signaling (McHenry et al., 2012) and D2 receptor signaling (Nutsch et al., 2016), and oxytocin receptors (Gil et al., 2013), and an increased number of neurons containing androgen receptors (Swaney et al., 2012). Moreover, acquisition of sexual experience and improvement in copulatory efficiency was shown to involve neuronal plasticity as measured by c-fos expression in the parieto-occipital cortex (Bialy et al., 1992; Bialy and Kaczmarek, 1996). Additionally, dopamine and noradrenaline levels in the medial prefrontal cortex correlate with sexual experience (Sanna et al., 2017).

Number of Intromissions

The number of intromissions indicates the level of genital stimulation required to induce ejaculation and describes the accumulation of sexual arousal. The cortico-medial part of the amygdala accumulates arousal in rats (de Jonge et al., 1992), as lesions in this region lead to a dramatic increase in number of intromissions (Harris and Sachs, 1975). Similar effects were observed after BNST lesion (Valcourt and Sachs, 1979). Strong reductions in intromission number were observed after serotonin 5HT1A receptor agonist (Snoeren et al., 2014) and D2 agonist and, less effectively, D1 agonist, but not D4 agonists (Cagiano et al., 1989; Beck et al., 2002; Sanna et al., 2015).

Intromission Ratio

Males display intromissions and/or mounts without intromission from the initiation of copulation to ejaculation. The IR describes the proportion of intromissions to the sum of mounts and intromissions. A low value of this parameter is strongly related to erectile dysfunction, which may be due to a low level of NO synthesis (neuronal and epithelial source), peripheral neuropathy, or vascular pathology (Hull et al., 1994; Bialy et al., 1996).

Evaluation of copulatory efficiency is critically important in rat models of premature ejaculation. Short ejaculation latencies with a very low number of intromissions (1 or 2 in a copulatory series) were observed in rats treated with 5HT-1A agonist, which can be considered a model of premature ejaculation (Coolen et al., 1997). Another model of premature ejaculation is based on the fact that in sexually experienced rats, there are two extreme endophenotypes. One represents premature ejaculation, with male rats achieving rapid and frequent ejaculations, up to five ejaculations during a short 30-min session of sexual interactions. The other phenotype represents the animal model of retarded ejaculation, with sexually experienced rats achieving only intromissions without any ejaculations during such a session (Pattij et al., 2005a; Waldinger and Olivier, 2005). Such model can be useful for understanding the mechanisms and pharmacological background of premature ejaculation and the role of serotonin receptors, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI), and oxytocin receptors (Giuliano and Clément, 2006; Clément et al., 2007, 2012, 2013; Kang et al., 2013, 2014; Oosting et al., 2016) but only in the case when less genital stimulation is required to achieve ejaculation (fewer intromissions). However, findings from our laboratory indicated that the majority of normal male rats were capable of achieving extravaginal ejaculations when mounting a female with a closed vaginal orifice, provided the male rats received sufficient genital stimulation during at least two intromissions preceding the extravaginal ejaculation. Furthermore, this phenomenon was independent of the number of mountings and was present without any pharmacological intervention (Beck and Bialy, 2000).

In addition to the retarded ejaculation model described above, copulatory efficiency and sexual motivation are strongly affected by metabolic disorders, especially type 2 diabetes mellitus (McVary et al., 1997; Faulkner et al., 2015), depressive-like/anhedonic states (Pfaus and Phillips, 1991; Van Furth et al., 1994; Bialy et al., 2014), or high anxiety levels (Hawley et al., 2013; Sanna et al., 2014).

Postejaculatory Behavior

The mechanisms behind the postejaculatory period are relatively poorly understood and involve numerous spinal and supraspinal structures of the central nervous system (Seizert, 2018; Le Moëne and Ågmo, 2019). In the postejaculatory period, all three processes: general arousal, sexual motivation, and sexual arousal, which control the male’s behavior are reflected in different parameters. After ejaculation, a male usually moves to one of the corners of a chamber and starts to emit a vocalization in the 22-kHz band. Most of the time, a male does not move when vocalizing. About 2 min after the first ejaculation (and significantly later after the second one), a male starts to explore the experimental chamber, even before termination of the postejaculatory vocalizations (Sachs and Bialy, 2000). This exploratory behavior reflects increasing general arousal but not sexual motivation or sexual arousal. Operant behavior shows that, at this time, male rats very often perform instrumental reactions—bar-pressing or run in a runway—but that after arriving in the compartment with a female, they evidently escape any socio-sexual contact and show a departure reaction (Beck, 1986; Beck et al., 2002). On the other hand, sexual arousal measured by penile erection occurs later than first exploratory behavior, at least during the first postejaculatory period. Such erection is visible even before the termination of postejaculatory 22-kHz vocalizations, suggesting that sexual arousal increases before a male starts to show interest in a female due to enhanced sexual motivation (Sachs and Bialy, 2000). Sexual motivation and interest in a receptive female appear after the termination of vocalizations. Furthermore, weak painful stimuli that increase sexual motivation in a non-specific way enhance a male’s interest in a female and mating, but this is present only after the end of postejaculatory vocalizations (Sachs and Barfield, 1974, 1976). These findings suggest that sexual motivation during postejaculatory ultrasonic vocalizations remains at a very low level. Therefore, after ejaculation, three parameters, latency to the first exploration, latency to the first noncontact erection, and latency to approaching a female, can be treated as measures of general arousal, sexual arousal, and sexual motivation, respectively. The initiation of enhanced sexual motivation later than of enhanced sexual arousal indicates that the postejaculatory interval is not simply the mirror state of the anticipatory and initiation phases of sexual behavior. Ultrasonic postejaculatory vocalizations, on the other hand, reflect, in our opinion, a relaxation state after ejaculation (Bialy et al., 2016). An enhanced level of general arousal and sexual arousal before the termination of postejaculatory vocalizations can be distinguished by spectral analysis of postejaculatory calls. Before the termination of vocalizations, at the time as exploration or noncontact erection take place, some frequency modulations or a shift from about the 45-kHz to the 28–23-kHz band are more often detected, and these differ from the very flat 22-kHz frequency ultrasonic vocalizations at the beginning and middle of the postejaculatory period (Bialy et al., 2019).

Rewarding Value of Sexual Interactions

The rewarding properties of mountings, intromissions, and ejaculations can be evaluated by conditioning procedures. In fact, the process of conditioning in appetitive behavior usually requires several sessions before there are visible effects. Ultrasound vocalizations in the 50-kHz band seem to be the most robust parameter reflecting positive emotional states (Brudzynski, 2007). In this line, high numbers of PVs convey the rewarding value of previous sexual contacts (Bialy et al., 2000). Additionally, conditioning during a second-order procedure (Everitt et al., 1989), conditioned place preference procedure (Camacho et al., 2009; Tenk et al., 2009), and instrumental conditioned reflexes during copulation (Beck, 1971; Beck et al., 2002) are useful in the evaluation of the rewarding value of subsequent events during sexual interactions. In addition, postejaculatory vocalizations—the most of time, extremely flat long-lasting vocalizations in the 22-kHz frequency band—probably reflect abrupt decreases in sexual arousal and a relaxation state following ejaculation. Thus, in this sense, these 22-kHz vocalizations can be used as an additional measure of reduction in sexual arousal and motivation related to the preceding ejaculation (Bialy et al., 2016). Postejaculatory vocalizations usually co-occur with a male’s inactivity or grooming. In addition, we found that males vocalize for significantly longer when a female is present in the copulatory chamber after ejaculation (Sachs and Bialy, 2000). Furthermore, such vocalizations are present only in a familiar environment, and cues that increase anxiety level (odor cues from unfamiliar males) significantly reduce such postejaculatory vocalization (Bialy et al., 2016). Moreover, the postejaculatory vocalizations are distinct from shorter low-frequency vocalizations that are produced by a male rat expressing a sexually related frustration state (Bialy et al., 2019).

Rat Sexual Behavior and Human Diseases Associated With Sexual Dysfunction

Table 1 summarizes the key components of rat sexual behavior with relevant parameters that describe specific aspects of the behavior (first column). These parameters may be used to quantify disturbances of sexual motivation and performance (second column) that are observed in various rodent models of human diseases (third column). Moreover, additional parameters and tests can be used to further delineate and confirm the underlying causes of abnormal sexual behavior (fourth column), for example, level of anxiety or anhedonia. Furthermore, analysis of these parameters may be useful for evaluation of the efficacy of therapeutic interventions in preclinical investigations.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Particular components of male sexual behavior, parameters that describe them and their relation to abnormal sexual motivation, arousal and performance due to psychiatric, cardiovascular, neurologic, endocrine and metabolic health disorder, and additional tests to confirm causes of changes in parameters in the rat models.

The translational application of animal models should be exploited cautiously, as not all aspects of sexual behavior and health disorders are identical in rats and humans (Le Moëne and Ågmo, 2019). Additionally, rodent models usually comprise only selected aspects of the complex pathogenesis of neurological, cardiovascular, and metabolic diseases in humans (Zaragoza et al., 2011; Dawson et al., 2018; Lutz, 2018). Even though the sexual behavior of a male rat is not identical to that seen in humans, neurotransmitters, brain structures, and neuronal networks and the motivational, and consummatory aspects of the sexual behavior seem to be fundamentally similar (Larsson and Ahlenius, 1999; Pattij et al., 2005b; Chan et al., 2008; Georgiadis et al., 2012). Since the sexual behavior of a male rat is well defined in terms behavioral, anatomical, and neurochemical characteristics, investigation of sexual behavior in various rodent models of human diseases provides a translational framework for better recognition of the underlying mechanisms of the sexual dysfunction seen in numerous human health disorders and their potential treatment.

Author Contributions

MB conceived the study, analyzed the literature, prepared the figure, wrote and revised the manuscript, and secured funding. WB-R analyzed the literature, prepared the table, and wrote the manuscript. JP analyzed the literature, reviewed the manuscript, and secured funding. TZ analyzed the literature, prepared the table, and wrote and revised the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Medical University of Warsaw. The study was carried out with the use of the CePT infrastructure financed by the European Union—the European Regional Development Fund within the Operational Programme “Innovative Economy” for 2007–2013.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial srelationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Marcin Kumosa for technical assistance with preparing Figure 1. The first author personally thanks Dr. Józef Beck for introducing him to the subject of the sexual behavior of rats and Prof. Benjamin Sachs for a beautiful time during author’s postdoctoral fellowship and many discussions on male rat sexual behavior.

References

Ågmo, A. (2011). On the intricate relationship between sexual motivation and arousal. Horm. Behav. 59, 681–688. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ågmo, A., Villalpando, A., Picker, Z., and Fernandez, H. (1995). Lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex and sexual behavior in the male rat. Brain Res. 696, 177–186. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00852-h

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asaba, A., Hattori, T., Mogi, K., and Kikusui, T. (2014). Sexual attractiveness of male chemicals and vocalizations in mice. Front. Neurosci. 8:231. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babaei-Balderlou, F., and Khazali, H. (2016). Effects of Ghrelin on sexual behavior and luteinizing hormone beta-subunit gene expression in male rats. J. Reprod. Infertil. 17, 88–96.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Barfield, R. J., Auerbach, P., Geyer, L. A., and McIntosh, T. K. (1979). Ultrasonic vocalizations in rat sexual behavior. Integr. Comp. Biol. 19, 469–480. doi: 10.1093/icb/19.2.469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, A. M., Fiorino, D. F., and Phillips, A. G. (1999). Effects of withdrawal from an escalating dose schedule of d-amphetamine on sexual behavior in the male rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 64, 597–604. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(99)00156-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrot, M., Wallace, D. L., Bolanos, C. A., Graham, D. L., Perrotti, L. I., Neve, R. L., et al. (2005). Regulation of anxiety and initiation of sexual behavior by CREB in the nucleus accumbens. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 102, 8357–8362. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500587102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, J. (1971). Instrumental conditioned reflexes with sexual reinforcement in rats. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 31, 251–252.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Beck, J. (1986). Effect of visual cues from female on the postejaculatory operant behavior of male rat. Acta Physiol. Pol. 37, 207–212.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Beck, J., and Bialy, M. (2000). The role of mounts and intromissions in triggering ejaculation in rats. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 60, 29–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Beck, J., Bialy, M., and Kostowski, W. (2002). Effects of D(1) receptor agonist SKF 38393 on male rat sexual behavior and postcopulatory departure in the goal compartment-runway paradigm. Physiol. Behav. 76, 91–97. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00678-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Beck, J., Abramczyk, P., Trzebski, A., and Przybylski, J. (1996). Sexual behavior in male rats after nitric oxide synthesis inhibition. Physiol. Behav. 60, 139–143. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02272-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Bogacki-Rychlik, W., Kasarello, K., Nikolaev, E., and Sajdel-Sulkowska, E. M. (2016). Modulation of 22-khz postejaculatory vocalizations by conditioning to new place: evidence for expression of a positive emotional state. Behav. Neurosci. 130, 415–421. doi: 10.1037/bne0000153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., and Kaczmarek, L. (1996). c-Fos expression as a tool to search for the neurobiological base of the sexual behaviour of males. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 56, 567–577.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Kalata, U., Nikolaev-Diak, A., and Nikolaev, E. (2010). D1 receptors involved in the acquisition of sexual experience in male rats. Behav. Brain Res. 206, 166–176. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Nikolaev, E., Beck, J., and Kaczmarek, L. (1992). Delayed c-fos expression in sensory cortex following sexual learning in male rats. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 14, 352–356. doi: 10.1016/0169-328x(92)90103-i

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Podobinska, M., Barski, J., Bogacki-Rychlik, W., and Sajdel-Sulkowska, E. M. (2019). Distinct classes of low frequency ultrasonic vocalizations in rats during sexual interactions relate to different emotional states. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 79, 1–12. doi: 10.21307/ane-2019-001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Rydz, M., and Kaczmarek, L. (2000). Precontact 50-kHz vocalizations in male rats during acquisition of sexual experience. Behav. Neurosci. 114, 983–990. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.5.983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialy, M., Strefnel, M., Nikolaev-Diak, A., Socha, A., Nikolaev, E., and Boguszewski, P. M. (2014). Sexual performance and precontact 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in WAG/Rij rats: effects of opioid receptor treatment. Epilepsy Behav. 39, 66–72. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotto, L. A., Knudson, G., Inskip, J., Rhodes, K., and Erskine, Y. (2010). Asexuality: a mixed-methods approach. Arch. Sex. Behav. 39, 599–618. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9434-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brudzynski, S. M. (2007). Ultrasonic calls of rats as indicator variables of negative or positive states: acetylcholine-dopamine interaction and acoustic coding. Behav. Brain Res. 182, 261–273. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cagiano, R., Barfield, R. J., White, N. R., Pleim, E. T., and Cuomo, V. (1989). Mediation of rat postejaculatory 22 kHz ultrasonic vocalization by dopamine D2 receptors. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 34, 53–58. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90352-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camacho, F. J., Portillo, W., Quintero-Enriquez, O., and Paredes, R. G. (2009). Reward value of intromissions and morphine in male rats evaluated by conditioned place preference. Physiol. Behav. 98, 602–607. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, J. S. W., Olivier, B., de Jong, T. R., Snoeren, E. M. S., Kooijman, E., van Hasselt, F. N., et al. (2008). Translational research into sexual disorders: pharmacology and genomics. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 585, 426–435. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2008.02.098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clément, P., Bernabé, J., Compagnie, S., Alexandre, L., McCallum, S., and Giuliano, F. (2013). Inhibition of ejaculation by the non-peptide oxytocin receptor antagonist GSK557296: a multi-level site of action. Br. J. Pharmacol. 169, 1477–1485. doi: 10.1111/bph.12198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clément, P., Bernabé, J., Gengo, P., Denys, P., Laurin, M., Alexandre, L., et al. (2007). Supraspinal site of action for the inhibition of ejaculatory reflex by dapoxetine. Eur. Urol. 51, 825–832. doi: 10.1016/j.eururo.2006.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clément, P., Laurin, M., Compagnie, S., Facchinetti, P., Bernabé, J., Alexandre, L., et al. (2012). Effect of dapoxetine on ejaculatory performance and related brain neuronal activity in rapid ejaculator rats. J. Sex. Med. 9, 2562–2573. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02884.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coolen, L. M., Olivier, B., Peters, H. J., and Veening, J. G. (1997). Demonstration of ejaculation-induced neural activity in the male rat brain using 5-HT1A agonist 8-OH-DPAT. Physiol. Behav. 62, 881–891. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00258-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, T. M., Golde, T. E., and Lagier-Tourenne, C. (2018). Animal models of neurodegenerative diseases. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1370–1379. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0236-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jong, T. R., Pattij, T., Veening, J. G., Waldinger, M. D., Cools, A. R., and Olivier, B. (2005). Effects of chronic selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors on 8-OH-DPAT-induced facilitation of ejaculation in rats: comparison of fluvoxamine and paroxetine. Psychopharmacology 179, 509–515. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2186-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jonge, F. H., Oldenburger, W. P., Louwerse, A. L., and Van de Poll, N. E. (1992). Changes in male copulatory behavior after sexual exciting stimuli: effects of medial amygdala lesions. Physiol. Behav. 52, 327–332. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(92)90279-b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewsbury, D. A. (1969). Copulatory behaviour of rats (Rattus norvegicus) as a function of prior copulatory experience. Anim. Behav. 17, 217–223. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(69)90004-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dominguez, J. M., Brann, J. H., Gil, M., and Hull, E. M. (2006). Sexual experience increases nitric oxide synthase in the medial preoptic area of male rats. Behav. Neurosci. 120, 1389–1394. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.6.1389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everitt, B. J., Cador, M., and Robbins, T. W. (1989). Interactions between the amygdala and ventral striatum in stimulus-reward associations: studies using a second-order schedule of sexual reinforcement. Neuroscience 30, 63–75. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90353-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faulkner, L. D., Dowling, A. R., Stuart, R. C., Nillni, E. A., and Hill, J. W. (2015). Reduced melanocortin production causes sexual dysfunction in male mice with POMC neuronal insulin and leptin insensitivity. Endocrinology 156, 1372–1385. doi: 10.1210/en.2014-1788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferraz, M. M. D., Quintella, S. L., Parcial, A. L. N., and Ferraz, M. R. (2016). The effects of sildenafil after chronic L-NAME administration in male rat sexual behavior. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 146–147, 13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2016.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiorino, D. F., and Phillips, A. G. (1999). Facilitation of sexual behavior in male rats following d-amphetamine-induced behavioral sensitization. Psychopharmacology Berl. 142, 200–208. doi: 10.1007/s002130050880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgiadis, J. R., Kringelbach, M. L., and Pfaus, J. G. (2012). Sex for fun: a synthesis of human and animal neurobiology. Nat. Rev. Urol. 9, 486–498. doi: 10.1038/nrurol.2012.151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil, M., Bhatt, R., Picotte, K. B., and Hull, E. M. (2013). Sexual experience increases oxytocin receptor gene expression and protein in the medial preoptic area of the male rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1688–1697. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giuliano, F., and Clément, P. (2006). Serotonin and premature ejaculation: from physiology to patient management. Eur. Urol. 50, 454–466. doi: 10.1016/j.eururo.2006.05.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giuliano, F., and Rampin, O. (2004). Neural control of erection. Physiol. Behav. 83, 189–201. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grønli, J., Murison, R., Fiske, E., Bjorvatn, B., Sørensen, E., Portas, C. M., et al. (2005). Effects of chronic mild stress on sexual behavior, locomotor activity and consumption of sucrose and saccharine solutions. Physiol. Behav. 84, 571–577. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamed, A., Daszczuk, P., Kursa, M. B., Turzynska, D., Sobolewska, A., Lehner, M., et al. (2016). Non-parametric analysis of neurochemical effects and Arc expression in amphetamine-induced 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalization. Behav. Brain Res. 312, 174–185. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.05.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansen, S., Larsson, K., Carlsson, S. G., and Sourander, P. (1978). The development of sexual behavior in the rat: role of preadult nutrition and environmental conditions. Dev. Psychobiol. 11, 51–61. doi: 10.1002/dev.420110109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, V. S., and Sachs, B. D. (1975). Copulatory behavior in male rats following amygdaloid lesions. Brain Res. 86, 514–518. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90906-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawley, W. R., Grissom, E. M., Belkin, M. N., James, T. F., and Dohanich, G. P. (2013). Decreased sexual motivation and heightened anxiety in male long-evans rats are correlated with the memory for a traumatic event. Arch. Sex. Behav. 42, 659–668. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-0017-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández, A., and Fernández-Guasti, A. (2018). Male rats with same-sex preference show higher immobility in the forced swim test, but similar effects of fluoxetine and desipramine than males that prefer females. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 171, 39–45. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2018.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, J. W., and Elias, C. F. (2018). Neuroanatomical framework of the metabolic control of reproduction. Physiol. Rev. 98, 2349–2380. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00033.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holy, T. E., and Guo, Z. (2005). Ultrasonic songs of male mice. PLoS Biol. 3:e386. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hueletl-Soto, M. E., Carro-Juarez, M., and Rodriguez-Manzo, G. (2012). Fluoxetine chronic treatment inhibits male rat sexual behavior by affecting both copulatory behavior and the genital motor pattern of ejaculation. J. Sex. Med. 9, 1015–1026. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2011.02339.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, E. M., Lumley, L. A., Matuszewich, L., Dominguez, J., Moses, J., and Lorrain, D. S. (1994). The roles of nitric oxide in sexual function of male rats. Neuropharmacology 33, 1499–1504. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(94)90054-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hull, E. M., and Rodríguez-Manzo, G. (2017). “1.01-male sexual behavior,” in Hormones, Brain and Behavior, 3rd Edn., eds D. W. Pfaff and M. Joëls (Oxford: Academic Press), 1–57.

Google Scholar

Kang, K. K., Ahn, G. J., Sung, J. H., Kim, S. H., Kim, H., and Lee, S. (2013). Ejaculatory responses are inhibited by a new chemical entity, DA-8031, in preclinical rodent models of ejaculation. Urology 81, 920.e13–928.e13. doi: 10.1016/j.urology.2012.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, K. K., Sung, J. H., Kim, S. H., and Lee, S. (2014). Effect of DA-8031, a novel oral compound for premature ejaculation, on male rat sexual behavior. Int. J. Urol. 21, 325–329. doi: 10.1111/iju.12256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsson, K. (1956). Conditioning and Sexual Behavior in the Male Albino Rat. Oxford, England: Almqvist and Wiksell.

Google Scholar

Larsson, K. (1959). Experience and maturation in the development of sexual behavior in male puberty rat. Behaviour 14, 101–107. doi: 10.1163/156853959x00027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsson, K., and Ahlenius, S. (1999). Brain and sexual behavior. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 19, 292–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09274.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Moëne, O., and Ågmo, A. (2019). Modeling human sexual motivation in rodents: some caveats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:187. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y. C., Salamone, J. D., and Sachs, B. D. (1997). Lesions in medial preoptic area and bed nucleus of stria terminalis: differential effects on copulatory behavior and noncontact erection in male rats. J. Neurosci. 17, 5245–5253. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.17-13-05245.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, T. A. (2018). Considering our methods: methodological issues with rodent models of appetite and obesity research. Physiol. Behav. 192, 182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.02.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHenry, J. A., Bell, G. A., Parrish, B. P., and Hull, E. M. (2012). Dopamine D1 receptors and phosphorylation of dopamine- and cyclic AMP-regulated phosphoprotein-32 in the medial preoptic area are involved in experience-induced enhancement of male sexual behavior in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 126, 523–529. doi: 10.1037/a0028707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McVary, K. T., Rathnau, C. H., and McKenna, K. E. (1997). Sexual dysfunction in the diabetic BB/WOR rat: a role of central neuropathy. Am. J. Physiol. 272, R259–R267. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1997.272.1.R259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendelson, S. D., and Gorzalka, B. B. (1987). An improved chamber for the observation and analysis of the sexual behavior of the female rat. Physiol. Behav. 39, 67–71. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90345-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendelson, S. D., and Pfaus, J. G. (1989). Level searching: a new assay of sexual motivation in the male rat. Physiol. Behav. 45, 337–341. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90136-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miwa, Y., Nagase, K., Oyama, N., Akino, H., and Yokoyama, O. (2011). Effect of corticotropin-releasing factor receptor antagonist on psychologically suppressed masculine sexual behavior in rats. J. Sex. Med. 8, 688–695. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2010.02055.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molenda-Figueira, H. A., Bell, M. R., De Lorme, K. C., and Sisk, C. L. (2017). Pubertal pair-housing facilitates adult sexual behavior in male rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 59, 111–117. doi: 10.1002/dev.21475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mulvihill, K. G., and Brudzynski, S. M. (2018). Individual behavioural predictors of amphetamine-induced emission of 50 kHz vocalization in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 350, 80–86. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novati, A., Yu-Taeger, L., Gonzalez Menendez, I., Quintanilla Martinez, L., and Nguyen, H. P. (2018). Sexual behavior and testis morphology in the BACHD rat model. PLoS One 13:e0198338. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0198338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nutsch, V. L., Will, R. G., Robison, C. L., Martz, J. R., Tobiansky, D. J., and Dominguez, J. M. (2016). Colocalization of mating-induced fos and d2-like dopamine receptors in the medial preoptic area: influence of sexual experience. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:75. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olayo-Lortia, J., Ferreira-Nuño, A., Velázquez-Moctezuma, J., and Morales-Otal, A. (2014). Further definition on the multiple partner choice arena: a potential animal model for the study of premature ejaculation. J. Sex. Med. 11, 2428–2438. doi: 10.1111/jsm.12637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oosting, R. S., Chan, J. S. W., Olivier, B., and Banerjee, P. (2016). Vilazodone does not inhibit sexual behavior in male rats in contrast to paroxetine: a role for 5-HT1A receptors? Neuropharmacology 107, 271–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.03.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pattij, T., de Jong, T. R., Uitterdijk, A., Waldinger, M. D., Veening, J. G., Cools, A. R., et al. (2005a). Individual differences in male rat ejaculatory behaviour: searching for models to study ejaculation disorders. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 724–734. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04252.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pattij, T., Olivier, B., and Waldinger, M. D. (2005b). Animal models of ejaculatory behavior. Curr. Pharm. Des. 11, 4069–4077. doi: 10.2174/138161205774913363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfaff, D. W. (2017). How the Vertebrate Brain Regulates Behavior: Direct From the Lab. Cambridge, MA and London, England: Harvard University Press.

Pfaus, J. G., Erickson, K. A., and Talianakis, S. (2013). Somatosensory conditioning of sexual arousal and copulatory behavior in the male rat: a model of fetish development. Physiol. Behav. 122, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfaus, J. G., Kippin, T. E., and Centeno, S. (2001). Conditioning and sexual behavior: a review. Horm. Behav. 40, 291–321. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfaus, J. G., Mendelson, S. D., and Phillips, A. G. (1990). A correlational and factor analysis of anticipatory and consummatory measures of sexual behavior in the male rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 15, 329–340. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(90)90058-h

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfaus, J. G., and Phillips, A. G. (1991). Role of dopamine in anticipatory and consummatory aspects of sexual behavior in the male rat. Behav. Neurosci. 105, 727–743. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.105.5.727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfaus, J. G., and Wilkins, M. F. (1995). A novel environment disrupts copulation in sexually naive but not experienced male rats: reversal with naloxone. Physiol. Behav. 57, 1045–1049. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)00394-k

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portillo, W., and Paredes, R. G. (2019). Motivational drive in non-copulating and socially Monogamous Mammals. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:238. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pultorak, J. D., Kelm-Nelson, C. A., Holt, L. R., Blue, K. V., Ciucci, M. R., and Johnson, A. M. (2016). Decreased approach behavior and nucleus accumbens immediate early gene expression in response to Parkinsonian ultrasonic vocalizations in rats. Soc. Neurosci. 11, 365–379. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1086434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramírez-Rodríguez, R., Tecamachaltzi-Silvaran, M. B., Díaz-Estrada, V. X., Chena-Becerra, F., Herrera-Covarrubias, D., Paredes-Ramos, P., et al. (2017). Heterosexual experience prevents the development of conditioned same-sex partner preference in male rats. Behav. Processes 136, 43–49. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D. (1978). “Conceptual and neural mechanisms of masculine copulatory behavior,” in Sex and Behavior: Status and Prospectus, eds T. E. McGill, D. A. Dewsbury and B. D. Sachs (Boston, MA: Springer), 267–295.

Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D. (2000). Contextual approaches to the physiology and classification of erectile function, erectile dysfunction and sexual arousal. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 541–560. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00022-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D., and Barfield, R. J. (1974). Copulatory behavior of male rats given intermittent electric shocks: theoretical implications. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 86, 607–615. doi: 10.1037/h0036176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D., and Barfield, R. J. (1976). “Functional analysis of masculine copulatory behavior in the rat,” in Advances in the Study of Behavior, eds J. S. Rosenblatt, R. A. Hinde and E. Shaw (New York: Academic Press), 91–154.

Google Scholar

Sachs, B. D., and Bialy, M. (2000). Female presence during postejaculatory interval facilitates penile erection and 22-kHz vocalization in male rats. Behav. Neurosci. 114, 1203–1208. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.6.1203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadeghzadeh, M., Shirpoor, A., Khalaji, N., Naderi, R., Samadi, M., and Rasmi, Y. (2018). The effect of chronic ethanol consumption on sexual motivation and behavior of adult male wistar rats in the copulatory phase. Addict. Health 10, 190–197. doi: 10.22122/ahj.v10i3.577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanna, F., Bratzu, J., Piludu, M. A., Corda, M. G., Melis, M. R., Giorgi, O., et al. (2017). Dopamine, noradrenaline and differences in sexual behavior between roman high and low avoidance male rats: a microdialysis study in the medial prefrontal cortex. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:108. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanna, F., Contini, A., Melis, M. R., and Argiolas, A. (2015). Role of dopamine D4 receptors in copulatory behavior: studies with selective D4 agonists and antagonists in male rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 137, 110–118. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2015.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanna, F., Corda, M. G., Melis, M. R., Piludu, M. A., Giorgi, O., and Argiolas, A. (2014). Male roman high and low avoidance rats show different patterns of copulatory behaviour: comparison with sprague dawley rats. Physiol. Behav. 127, 27–36. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schober, J., Weil, Z., and Pfaff, D. (2011). How generalized CNS arousal strengthens sexual arousal (and vice versa). Horm. Behav. 59, 689–695. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seizert, C. A. (2018). The neurobiology of the male sexual refractory period. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 92, 350–377. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snoeren, E. M. S., Veening, J. G., Olivier, B., and Oosting, R. S. (2014). Serotonin 1A receptors and sexual behavior in male rats: a review. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 121, 102–114. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefanick, M. L., and Davidson, J. M. (1987). Genital responses in noncopulators and rats with lesions in the medical preoptic area or midthoracic spinal cord. Physiol. Behav. 41, 439–444. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90078-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swaney, W. T., Dubose, B. N., Curley, J. P., and Champagne, F. A. (2012). Sexual experience affects reproductive behavior and preoptic androgen receptors in male mice. Horm. Behav. 61, 472–478. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tenk, C. M., Wilson, H., Zhang, Q., Pitchers, K. K., and Coolen, L. M. (2009). Sexual reward in male rats: effects of sexual experience on conditioned place preferences associated with ejaculation and intromissions. Horm. Behav. 55, 93–97. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valcourt, R. J., and Sachs, B. D. (1979). Penile reflexes and copulatory behavior in male rats following lesions in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Brain Res. Bull. 4, 131–133. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(79)90068-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Furth, W. R., Wolterink-Donselaar, I. G., and van Ree, J. M. (1994). Endogenous opioids are differentially involved in appetitive and consummatory aspects of sexual behavior of male rats. Am. J. Physiol. 266, 606–613. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1994.266.2.R606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldinger, M. D., and Olivier, B. (2005). Animal models of premature and retarded ejaculation. World J. Urol. 23, 115–118. doi: 10.1007/s00345-004-0493-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Will, R. G., Hull, E. M., and Dominguez, J. M. (2014). Influences of dopamine and glutamate in the medial preoptic area on male sexual behavior. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 121, 115–123. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2014.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wöhr, M., and Schwarting, R. K. W. (2007). Ultrasonic communication in rats: can playback of 50-kHz calls induce approach behavior? PLoS One 2:e1365. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaragoza, C., Gomez-Guerrero, C., Martin-Ventura, J. L., Blanco-Colio, L., Lavin, B., Mallavia, B., et al. (2011). Animal models of cardiovascular diseases. J. Biomed. Biotechnol. 2011:497841. doi: 10.1155/2011/497841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sexual motivation, general arousal, sexual arousal, ultrasonic vocalizations, depression, anxiety, metabolic disorders, male behavior

Citation: Bialy M, Bogacki-Rychlik W, Przybylski J and Zera T (2019) The Sexual Motivation of Male Rats as a Tool in Animal Models of Human Health Disorders. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:257. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00257

Received: 30 July 2019; Accepted: 30 October 2019;
Published: 26 November 2019.

Edited by:

Fabrizio Sanna, University of Cagliari, Italy

Reviewed by:

Berend Olivier, Utrecht University, Netherlands
José R. Eguibar, Meritorious Autonomous University of Puebla, Mexico

Copyright © 2019 Bialy, Bogacki-Rychlik, Przybylski and Zera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michal Bialy, michalbialy@yahoo.com

Download