Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 08 May 2019
Sec. Learning and Memory
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00095

A Central Component of the N1 Event-Related Brain Potential Could Index the Early and Automatic Inhibition of the Actions Systematically Activated by Objects

J. Bruno Debruille1,2* Molly Touzel1 Julia Segal1 Christine Snidal1 Louis Renoult3
  • 1Department of Neuroscience, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 3School of Psychology, University of East Anglia, Norwich, United Kingdom

Stimuli of the environment, like objects, systematically activate the actions they are associated to. These activations occur extremely fast. Nevertheless, behavioral data reveal that, in most cases, these activations are then automatically inhibited, around 100 ms after the occurrence of the stimulus. We thus tested whether this early inhibition could be indexed by a central component of the N1 event-related brain potential (ERP). To achieve that goal, we looked at whether this ERP component is larger in tasks that could increase the inhibition and in trials where reaction times (RTs) happen to be long. The illumination of a real space bar of a keyboard out of the dark was used as a stimulus. To maximize the modulation of the inhibition, the task participants had to perform was manipulated across blocks. A look-only task and a count task were used to increase inhibition and an immediate press task was used to decrease it. ERPs of the two block-conditions where presses had to be prevented and where the largest central N1s were predicted were compared to those elicited in the press task, differentiating the ERPs to the third of the trials where presses were the slowest from the ERPs to the third of the trials with the fastest presses. Despite larger negativities due to lateralized readiness potentials (LRPs) and despite greater attention likely in immediate press-trials, central N1s were found to be minimal for the fastest presses, intermediate for the slowest ones and maximal for the two no-press conditions. These results thus provide a strong support for the idea that the central N1 indexes an early and short lasting automatic inhibition of the actions systematically activated by objects. They also confirm that the strength of this automatic inhibition spontaneously fluctuates across trials and tasks. On the other hand, just before N1s, parietal P1s were found larger for fastest presses. They might thus index the initial activation of these actions. Finally, consistent with the idea that N300s index late inhibition processes, that occur preferentially when the task requires them, these ERPs were quasi absent for fast presses trials and much larger in the three other conditions.

Highlights

– Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) elicited by a real object

– Smaller parietal P1s and larger central N1s for slowest than for fastest motor responses

– Even larger central N1s for tasks without such responses

– Central N1s may index early inhibition of stimulus-activated actions

– Lateralized readiness potential (LRP) could index a suppression of prolonged motor inhibition

– N300s could index late inhibition of stimulus-activated actions

Introduction

Nowadays, affordances is a word often used to designate the possible actions that are systematically and pre-consciously primed by objects (Tucker and Ellis, 1998)1. Such actions are now central to theories of embodied cognition (for a review, see Matheson et al., 2015). Systematically activated “motor representations” of objects are considered integral to the identification of an object (Clark, 1999; Wilson, 2002). These systematic activations were shown by Tucker and Ellis (1998), as well as others (e.g., Borghi and Riggio, 2008; Phillips and Ward, 2002), in experiments in which subjects viewed pictures of graspable objects whose handle was oriented either to the left or to the right. Responses were faster with the left hand for pictures of objects with handles oriented to the left whereas right-hand responses were faster when the handle of the objects was oriented to the right. Although these compatibility effects seem to exist even when attention is not focused on affordances (Symes et al., 2006), their sizes are increased by attending to the action-related features of the object (Tipper, 2010). For instance, focusing on a cup’s handle elicited larger compatibility effects than focusing on unrelated features, such as the color of the object (Tipper et al., 2006).

Results of imaging studies corroborated the idea that objects systematically activate sensory-motor brain regions and thus motor representations. A PET study of object’s perception showed activations in the inferior parietal lobule, the supplementary motor area, the dorsal and ventral precentral gyri and the cerebellum in comparison to viewing non-objects (Grèzes and Decety, 2002). Studies using manipulable objects have shown that the observation of tools, even without having to use them, trigger strong left dorsal premotor cortex activation (Grafton et al., 1997). The ventral premotor cortex and the left posterior parietal cortex were also activated by viewing and naming pictures of tools compared to control items in a study by Chao and Martin (2000). Other works have provided evidence for a functional separation of the dorsal pathway (Rizzolatti et al., 1998). It has been divided into a dorso-dorsal subpathway that controls actions and a ventro-dorsal pathway that codes the representations of actions (Rizzolatti and Matelli, 2003; Buxbaum and Kalénine, 2010; Binkofski and Buxbaum, 2013). The former runs from visual areas (V6) and the superior parietal lobe to the dorsal premotor cortex. The latter projects from the medial superior temporal area into the inferior parietal lobe.

However, most of the times, performing the actions that are systematically primed by the occurrence of stimuli is contextually inappropriate. This is true not only for trying to grab an object whose picture appears on a screen but also, in everyday life, for acting in a way that would not be consistent with our goals. Given that all objects of our environment seem to prime the actions they are associated to, inhibition of all those unwanted actions must be a quasi-ceaseless mechanism of wide importance (Miller, 2000; Ridderinkhof, 2002a,b; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004; Sumner et al., 2007). This means that performing actions at the right time might then depend on a temporary suppression of the ceaseless inhibition. Many facts are consistent with the existence of this ceaseless inhibition. For instance, when subjects have to press some buttons on a board while ignoring others, the motor representations associated with the to-be-ignored buttons are inhibited (e.g., Tipper et al., 1992). Or, when certain lesions disrupt the inhibition of affordances, in anarchic limb behaviors for example, the patient would act upon the affordance to grasp a cup with the hand congruent with handle orientation, rather than following the instruction to always grasp with the hand closest to the cup (Riddoch et al., 1998). Similarly, frontal lesions result in “utilization behaviors,” which are defined as the difficulty in resisting the use of an object within reach (e.g., Archibald et al., 2001). When discovering the pen of the doctor on his/her desk, these patients will grab it and use it.

The results obtained in yet another tradition of research objectivize the existence of such a systematic inhibition of actions. These results show that selective motor inhibition occurs automatically even without conscious perception and voluntary effort (Eimer and Schlaghecken, 2003; Sumner et al., 2007; Atas and Cleeremans, 2015). They were obtained by using brief presentations of elementary visual objects (primes), which were immediately masked by another visual stimulus before a target appears. The primes were either compatible or incompatible with the target. In this type of design, compatible primes delayed reaction times (RTs) to targets. Recent works show that these RT increases reveal an automatic motor inhibition rather than a competition with an incorrect “opposite-to-prime” response (Vainio et al., 2011; Ocampo and Finkbeiner, 2013; Atas and Cleeremans, 2015).

Electrophysiological studies of object affordances have shed light on the mechanisms underlying the inhibition of affordances. The disappearance of a graspable object was found to enhance mu-rhythm synchronization (Schuck et al., 2010) while its viewing had induced a mu-rhythm desynchronization. In a quite different domain, a study by Debruille et al. (2012) investigated the inhibition of the social affordances that are automatically elicited by faces, such as making eye contact, smiling, saying hello and starting a conversation. For that purpose, they used the face of a confederate with whom making eye contacts was appropriate and the face of a dummy for whom this automatically activated affordance had to be inhibited. A block design was used in order to increase the focus on activations that occur systematically and would thus have to be automatically inhibited when inappropriate. Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) elicited by the two face stimuli were then compared. Those of the dummy included larger central N300s than those of the face of the confederate. This was interpreted as indexing the greater amount of affordance inhibition required for the dummy. Interestingly, these ERPs had the same latency and the same central scalp distribution as the NoGo N2 potential (Falkenstein et al., 1999; Ocklenburg et al., 2011), an ERP that is precisely elicited in conditions where (simple) actions associated to simple stimuli have to be prevented and which has thus been associated to their inhibition (Jodo and Kayama, 1992; Bruin and Wijers, 2002; Roche et al., 2005).

Nevertheless, these N300- and No-Go-N2-ERPs onset around 200 ms post-stimulus onset. They thus start too late to index mechanisms that could prevent the actions systematically activated by the stimulus. Indeed, these systematic activations occur extremely early. For instance, when participants are observing graspable objects, the increase in muscle excitability starts as early as 120 ms after the onset of the presentation (Franca et al., 2012). Similar delays were obtained in a study where participants learned to associate the contraction of the tibialis anterior muscle of each ankle in responses to an auditory cue (Schneider et al., 2004) or when measuring facial action (i.e., spontaneous mimicry) in response to the presentation of happy and neutral faces (Korb et al., 2010). Actions associated to a stimulus thus appear to be activated in the brain in as little as 100 ms after the onset of the stimulus. Indeed, at least two dozen ms have to be subtracted from the 120 ms delays mentioned above in order to take into account the time for the brain output to reach and excite the muscles recorded and, for the first two studies, for the movement to attain a measurable size. Brain mechanisms that prevent affordance from being acted upon thus have to start as early as 100 ms. Without such mechanisms, we could start acting each time a stimulus associated to an action occurs in our environment. This can also be deduced from the duration of the presentation of the primes in the studies that were mentioned above in order to show that selective motor inhibition occurs automatically even without conscious perception and voluntary effort (Eimer and Schlaghecken, 2003; Sumner et al., 2007; Vainio et al., 2011; Ocampo and Finkbeiner, 2013; Atas and Cleeremans, 2015). The elementary visual objects used as primes were presented for only 30–70 ms before being masked.

The high temporal resolution of the ERP technique makes it an ideal tool for exploring such a fast inhibition mechanism. The N1 ERP elicited by visual stimuli onsets at a latency compatible with the early inhibition necessary to prevent immediate action upon fast affordance activation. Although its posterior, that is, occipital components, are known to index processing of the physical features of the visual stimuli, the functional significance of its central components is not well known. Most interestingly, these more anterior components have been previously related to the processing of affordances in two studies. The first, that of Proverbio et al. (2007), reports that manipulable objects elicited larger central N1s than animal entities, suggesting that anterior motor regions might be involved in action-centered representations of tools. The second is that of Debruille et al. (2012) whose N300 results have been mentioned above. In this study, ERPs in the N1 time windows were more negative for the dummy, with whom social affordance were inappropriate, than for the real person. Also noteworthy, the N1 has been shown to be of larger amplitude for stop- than for go-signals (Kenemans, 2015; Senderecka, 2016; Raud and Huster, 2017) and for successful than for unsuccessful stop-signal conditions (e.g., Bekker et al., 2005; Wild-Wall et al., 2008; Pires et al., 2014). It has thus been proposed as an index of a directed (or learned) action-inhibition.

Therefore, the first goal of the present study was to test whether the central N1 could index the early and automatic inhibition that objects would spontaneously trigger just after they activate the actions they are associated to. This early automatic inhibition was hypothesized to be of very short duration, that is to last only until the occurrence of the second negativity, that is, the N300 or the No-Go N2, which seems to index a later, more context-dependent, inhibition.

The second goal of the current study was to search for a candidate ERP index of the fast and systematic activation of actions (or affordances) that occurs before the automatic inhibition. We thus explored whether the ERP that precedes it, that is, the P1, could index it. This idea was based on the parieto-central P1s that were related to initial action activation by Kiefer et al. (2011). Here, we assumed that greater activations would lead to faster actions. We thus hypothesized larger parietal P1s for trials with short than for those with long RTs, as the threshold for real action would be reached faster.

The third goal was to examine the N300 elicited by the display of an object and see whether its amplitude would vary as an index of the late and context-dependent inhibition of the actions automatically activated by stimuli, as seemed to be the case for the face of the dummy in Debruille et al. (2012). Accordingly, we looked at whether this amplitude would be large in the tasks where subjects have to prevent performing the action than in tasks where they have to perform it.

To achieve these three goals, we designed a study with one simple and highly familiar object: the spacebar of a computer keyboard. It was assumed that, for anyone who grew up in the age of computers, this stimulus systematically activates the action of pressing it, just as a coffee cup handle affords grasping, an idea already used in other recent studies (e.g., Horváth, 2013). The present study was conducted in a completely dark room and the spacebar of a keyboard placed in front of the subject was illuminated sporadically, thus appearing out of darkness. The method used to manipulate the automatic inhibition was based on the fact that some automatic mechanisms (e.g., those of the knee reflex) can be influenced and the action, prevented, reduced or, on the contrary, amplified. A block design was used to maximize this modulation. The space bar was the only stimulus used and participants had to process it in exactly the same way throughout each block in order to reinforce their strategy. Two tasks were used to maximize the inhibition of presses, a look only task and a count the number of appearances task. A task where participant had to immediately press the space bar upon its appearance was used to decrease automatic inhibition. Larger central N1s were thus predicted in the two former tasks than in this latter one.

Given that participants’ attention spontaneously fluctuates across the trials of an experiment, it was assumed that both the strength of the systematic activation and the strength of the early automatic inhibition would also fluctuate across trials. In the immediate press task, these fluctuations should lead to faster presses when activation is strong and inhibition weak and to slower presses when activation is weak and inhibition strong (somewhat like in Atas and Cleeremans, 2015). The ERPs elicited by the illumination of the space bar were thus averaged separately according to whether the press was slow or fast. Larger central N1s were predicted for the former than for the latter.

Importantly, two very well-known effects should drive N1 amplitudes in the direction opposite to these predictions. Firstly, the attention effect, as, relative to the look-only and the count task, the immediate press task stimulates vigilance as participants have to press as fast as possible. It should thus be associated to greater attention and thus to larger N1s, an ERP effect known to be maximal at occipital electrode sites (Luck et al., 1993; Hillyard et al., 1995; Johannes et al., 1995; Hillyard and Anllo-Vento, 1998). Secondly, the lateralized readiness potentials (LRPs) elicited by press trials. They will be responsible for more negative ERPs at central sites over the hemisphere contralateral to the pressing-hand just before the pressing (Coles et al., 1988; Miller and Hackley, 1992). This could thus be mistaken for larger central N1s as RTs in simple tasks where no choice has to be made are known to be roughly between 200 and 300 ms (Niemi and Näätänen, 1981). The LRPs that precede them will thus be within the N1 time window. In these conditions, observing larger central N1s for the two tasks where the pressing had to be prevented would thus be all the more demonstrative for the inhibition hypothesis.

On the other hand, in order to look for an index of the prior activation, we also compared ERPs of the four conditions (i.e., fast-presses, slow-presses, count and look only) in the time window preceding these N1s. Finally, N300s were explored to see if their amplitudes would also be minimal when subjects act upon the activated affordance, that is, in the press task.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Nine females and 11 males with a mean age of 24 (range 18–30) were recruited as participants. They all learned about the experiment through classified ad websites. Right-handed, they had at least some university education and had normal- or glasses-corrected to normal vision. None reported any personal or family history of severe neurological or psychiatric disorder. Participants were excluded if they consumed more than twelve drinks of alcoholic beverages per week or if they used recreational drugs, except if they used marijuana less than once per week. They read and signed an informed consent form approved by the Douglas Institute Research and Ethics Board and were compensated for their time $15 per hour.

Procedure

Subjects performed the tasks seated in front of a computer in a completely dark room. The screen and the keyboard of the computer were concealed with cardboard, such that when the screen of the computer turned from black to white, its emanated light, which lasted 500 ms, was directed onto the spacebar so that only this object (and the cardboard) was visible. Inter-stimulus intervals varied randomly between 1.4 and 2.3 s. During these intervals, the screen of the monitor was black and subjects could not see the spacebar or any other object, given the total darkness of the room. All 20 subjects completed an experiment that consisted of three block-tasks, each one including 100 occurrences of the space bar. They were instructed to rest their right index finger just in front of the spacebar so as to be ready to press it without obscuring its illumination. In the “Press” task, subjects pressed the spacebar as fast as possible as soon as it appeared. In the “Count” task, they gazed at the spacebar and counted the number of times it appeared, giving a final report of their count at the end of the block. In the “Look only” task, subjects looked at the spacebar as it appeared and disappeared and were instructed to never direct their gaze away from it. The order of these three block-tasks was counterbalanced across subjects.

Data Acquisition

The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded with tin electrodes mounted in an elastic cap (Electrocap International, Eaton, OH, USA) at 28 of the sites of the extended International 10–20 system (Electrode Position Nomenclature Committee, 1991). The reference electrode was placed on the right earlobe. The active electrode sites were grouped in a sagittal subset, which included Fz, FCz, Cz and Pz; a parasagittal subset, which comprised FP1/2, F3/4, FC3/4, C3/4, CP3/4, P3/4, and O1/2; and a lateral subset, which consisted of F7/8, FT7/8, T3/4, TP7/8 and T5/6. The EEG was amplified 20,000 times by Contact Precision amplifiers. The half amplitudes cut-offs of their high- and low-pass filters were set at 0.1 and 100 Hz, respectively, with an additional electronic notch filter to remove 60 Hz contamination. Signals were digitized on-line at a 256 Hz sampling rate and stored for subsequent averaging using the I-wave (version 5.24) software package. Behavioral data, consisting of response times for the press-task, were recorded using the E-prime (version 2.0) software.

Data Processing

EEG epochs of the 100 trials of each block-task were examined. Trials of the press-task in which response time exceeded 500 ms or in which there was no response were excluded, leading to an average of 5.6 trials being excluded. Trials contaminated by eye movements, excessive myogram, amplifier saturations or analog to digital clipping were removed off-line when analog to digital clipping exceeded a 100 ms duration or when the absolute value of the amplitude exceeded 100 μV. For the press task, the third of the trials in which subjects responded the fastest were separated from the third of the trials in which subjects responded the slowest. This was done so as to use this fast-press condition as the control one. Indeed, as the early inhibition appears to be automatic, it was assumed to be always present, but minimal in the case of the fast-press trials. ERPs to stimuli were computed in each subject in the “Count” “Look Only” “Fast Press” and “Slow Press” conditions by averaging the EEG epochs of these trials in each task using a −200–0 ms baseline and looking up to 600 ms post-stimulus onset.

Measures and Analyses

Visual inspection of event-related potentials (ERPs) revealed differences between conditions in the P1, N1 and N300 time windows. To assess the statistical significance of these differences, mean ERP voltages were first measured in time windows centered on their peaks, that is, in the 80–120, the 120–200 and the 300–500 ms time window, respectively. These measures were entered in three repeated-measures analysis of variances (ANOVAs) using a multivariate approach. The ANOVAs for the sagittal subset of electrodes included two within-subject factors: (a) condition, which had four levels: “Fast Press” vs. “Slow Press” vs. “Count” vs. “Look Only”); and (b) electrode site. In the ANOVAs dedicated to the parasagittal and lateral subset of electrodes, a hemiscalp (left vs. right) factor was added. Other ANOVAs were then conducted on the region of interest (e.g., C3, Cz and C4 for the central N1s). To test the hypothesis of a difference between fast and slow presses, an additional ANOVA was conducted comparing only the fast-press and the slow-press conditions. A similar ANOVA was also conducted to determine if there was any difference between “Count” and “Look Only” conditions. The Greenhouse and Geisser (1959) correction for lack of sphericity was used to correct degrees of freedom for the factors that had more than two levels (i. e., condition and electrode). In this case, original degrees of freedom are reported with corrected p-values.

Results

Behavioral Data

Average response times in the press condition were 206 ms (SD = 35.9) for the 3rd of the trials which induced the fastest responses and 291 ms (SD = 34.5) for the 3rd of the trials which induced the slowest ones. In the “Count” task, subjects reported an average of 100.5 illuminations (SD = 2.6) out of the 100 illuminations of the space bar.

Electrophysiological Data

Visual Inspection

Given that block design experiments are now much less frequently used, ERPs will first be described qualitatively in order to pinpoint their particular characteristics. Figure 1, where negative polarity is plotted upward, shows the unsmoothed grand average ERPs of the four conditions. It first displays an early difference (e.g., at Pz and P4) where parietal P1s seem larger for fast presses (green lines) than for other conditions. Then, in the following time window, at sagittal and right para-sagittal sites, the first sizeable negative deflections, the N1s, appear smaller for these fast presses, intermediate for slow presses (red lines) and maximal for the count- (blue lines) and the look-only- (black lines) block-condition. Importantly, these smallest N1s for fast presses appear unlikely to be due to larger P1s, as the latter were maximal at parietal sites. At more anterior sites (e.g., C4, FC4 and FCz), no larger P1s can be seen whereas N1s for fast presses clearly appear smaller than N1s for other conditions. Noteworthy, the bilaterally large N1 deflections for no press trials can be observed not only over central-, but also over frontal-sites (Fz, F3, F4, F7 and F8) as depicted by Figures 1, 2. In the fast press trials, negativities in the time window of the N1 deflections appear smaller over central and frontal sites, except at the left central ones where a LRP is likely to make the ERPs more negative.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Unsmoothed grand average (n = 20) of the event-related brain potentials (ERPs) elicited by the occurrence of the space bar of a keyboard out of the dark. Negative polarity is plotted upward. Green lines are for the fastest reaction-times (RTs) trials of the block-task where the subjects had to press the space bar as fast as possible. Red lines are for the slowest RTs trials of that task. Blue lines are for the block-task where participants had to count mentally the number of appearances of that space bar. Black lines are for the block-task where they just had to look at the space bar. The order of these three tasks was counterbalanced across subjects. *p < 0.05;**p < 0.001.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results of the unsmoothed subtractions of the grand averages of the ERPs elicited by the occurrence of the space bar when RTs were the fastest from the ERPs when RTs were the slowest (red lines), from the ERPs in the count block-task (blue lines) and from the ERPs of the look-only block-task (black lines). These subtractions were performed to distinguish the N1-effect from the effect on late ERPs.

This contrasts with what can be observed at occipital sites, where N1s look larger for both press-trials than for the two other conditions, especially at O1. These N1s are followed by a P2, which can be best individualized at Fz and FCz, around 200 ms. Later, peaking around 300 ms, is a large positive wave maximal at Pz, which clearly appears to be a P3b occurring before the second negative deflection, as can be seen in simple tasks where only one stimulus occurs repeatedly and where no choice has to be made (Donchin et al., 1978). This late posterior positivity appeared much larger for fast presses than for the three other conditions, in line with the well-known sensitivity of this potential to task relevance and attention. These P3bs precede sizable negative waves (second negativities) maximal around 400 ms at Fz, which seemed almost abolished for fast-presses and large for all other conditions. These latter differences appeared maximal at FCz, Cz and were smaller at Pz.

Given that the larger P3b for fastest presses could occur earlier in some participants than in others, they could overlap N1s. There was thus a possibility for the N1s amplitude to appear small for fast presses not because of weaker N1 processes. To eliminate this possibility, we subtracted the ERPs of the fast presses from the ERPs to every other condition. Figure 2 illustrates the results of these subtractions. There, the effect on the N1s corresponds to an early deflection that clearly differs from the one corresponding to the large effect on latter ERPs. Moreover, at some electrode sites (i.e., FCz, C4, CP4 and P4) these two deflections are separated by a return to the baseline.

Analyses

Parietal P1 Time Window (80–120 ms)

The ANOVA conducted with the mean ERP voltages at the 80–120 ms time window for the sagittal subset of electrodes did not reveal any main effect of condition nor any significant interaction of this factor with electrode. Given the a priori hypothesis we had for the parietal site, a one-way ANOVA was attempted at Pz. There, P1s only tended to be larger for fast press trials than for the three other conditions at Pz (F(3,57) = 2.9, p = 0.063). Similarly, the ANOVA conducted for the parasagittal subset did not reveal any main effect of condition nor any significant interaction of this factor with electrode or hemiscalp. Given the a priori hypothesis and the visual inspection, a one-way ANOVA was conducted at P4. It was found that P1s were only marginally larger for fast press trials than for the three other conditions (F(3,57) = 3.0, p = 0.044). The ANOVA performed for the lateral subset did not reveal any statistically significant difference.

N1 Time Windows (120–200 ms)

The ANOVAs revealed a main effect of condition at the sagittal (F(3,57) = 6.8, p = 0.0005), parasagittal (F(3,57) = 6.6, p = 0.0007) and lateral (F(3,57) = 7.0, p = 0.0004) subsets of electrodes. Significant interactions between condition and electrode were observed only for the parasagittal subset (F(18,342) = 2.9, p = 0.03) where differences appeared somewhat reversed at occipital sites (O1/2). Analyses run at the sites of interest for the central N1 (FC3/4, C3/4 and CP3/4) revealed similar results for the main effect of condition (F(3,57) = 5.3, p = 0.003), which interacted with hemiscalp (F(3,57) = 3.2, p = 0.045) in accordance with the much smaller differences between conditions seen over the left- than over the right-hemiscalp in Figure 1. The post hoc analyses run for each hemiscalp at these electrode sites to find the source of this condition × hemiscalp interaction revealed a significant effect of condition over the right hemiscalp (F(3,57) = 7.5, p = 0.00025) and a barely significant one over the left hemiscalp (F(3,57) = 2.9, p = 0.04). Further analyses showed larger central N1s at these sites (FC4, C4 and CP4) for slow- than for fast-press-trials (F(1,19) = 8.5, p = 0.009), larger central N1s for look-only N1s than for the fast-press condition (F(1,19) = 11.4, p = 0.003) and for count than for fast-presses (F(1,19) = 8.2, p = 0.01). Conversely, ANOVAs including just the “Count” and “Look Only” conditions revealed no significant effect of condition and thus, no significant difference in N1 amplitude between these two tasks, at any subsets.

Another post hoc ANOVA was conducted in the same 120–200 ms time window at occipital electrodes (O1/2) to test the reversal and thus to see whether posterior N1s were larger for the task requiring more attention, that is, for the press task. This was somewhat the case (F(3,57) = 3.21, p = 0.05). This modestly significant effect was larger over the left than over the right hemiscalp (F(3,57) = 3.22, p = 0.046).

N300 Time Windows (300–500 ms)

The ANOVAs revealed significantly larger N300s at the sagittal (F(3,57) = 16.9, p < 0.00001), parasagittal (F(3,57) = 14.8, p < 0.00001) and lateral (F(3,57) = 10.8, p = 0.00001) subset of electrodes for the three conditions other than that of the fast presses. At the parasagittal subset of electrodes, this effect tended to interact with electrode F(18,342) = 2.9, p = 0.06 and with electrode and hemiscalp F(18,342) = 2.4, p = 0.022. Nevertheless, no post hoc on specific electrode was run as the difference was so likely to be due to both greater N300s for no-press trials at central electrodes and larger P300bs (or LPP or P3b, see Figure 1) maximal at Pz for fastest-presses. Both N300 and P300b effects were fully expectable, given the literature and were, most probably, found here, as illustrated by Figures 1, 2, which shows not only large differences at central and fronto-central electrodes, where N300 are usually maximal, but also at Pz, where the peak of the P300b is maximal.

Discussion

To test whether the amplitude of the central N1 ERP indexes the strength of the early and automatic inhibition of the actions systematically activated by real objects, we used a novel paradigm in which the space bar of a computer keyboard was illuminated in order to appear out of the dark and to activate the action of pressing it. A block task-design was used to maximize the modulation of automatic inhibition. In each block, participants had to process the only stimulus, the space bar, in exactly the same way to re-enforce the processing strategy. Two of the three blocks were aimed at increasing inhibition. There, the task of subjects was to prevent themselves from pressing the space bar. In one of these two blocks, they had to count the number of times it appeared. In the other, they just had to look at it. The third block was used to decrease the automatic inhibition of the action. There, the task was to press the space bar as fast as possible. On the other hand, just as attention, the strength of activations and inhibitions was assumed to fluctuate from trial to trial within the same block and presses were assumed to be performed faster when their activations were stronger and their inhibitions weaker. The third of the trials where participants pressed it the fastest were thus isolated from the third of the trials where they pressed it the slower in order to use them as the control condition and to see if central N1 amplitudes would be minimal for them, intermediate for slowest presses and maximal in the count-block and the look-only block.

The space bar was pressed on average about 250 ms after the onset of its illumination. Motor responses were thus output by the brain in 200 ms or less and thus before the onset of the N300 (Figure 1). In accordance with the idea that the central N1 indexes an early automatic inhibition, the amplitudes of this ERP were found to be the largest for the two tasks where subjects had to prevent themselves from pressing the space bar. They were intermediate for the slow-press trials and smallest for the fast-press trials.

These effects appear remarkable because they were observed despite two factors moving ERPs in the direction opposite to the predictions. Firstly, pressing the space bar as fast as possible induced greater attention relative to the two other tasks and thus larger N1s, particularly at occipital scalp sites (Luck et al., 1990, 1993; Hillyard et al., 1995; Johannes et al., 1995; Hillyard and Anllo-Vento, 1998), in accordance with the results observed in the present work at O1/2. Secondly, the space bar pressing done with the right hand induced LRPs and thus larger negativities at the left central scalp sites (C3) just before the action and thus during the N1 time window (Coles et al., 1988; Miller and Hackley, 1992). This is illustrated by Figure 1, where there was virtually no difference across the four conditions at this electrode site, and barely any at FC3, in the N1 deflections. At these sites, LRPs have compensated weaker N1 processes. On the contrary, at right central sites, no or smaller LRPs increased the negativity of ERPs of the N1 time window for fast press trials. Most likely, this is why much smaller N1 deflections can be observed in this condition, in accordance with the early inhibition hypothesis.

The results thus strongly support the idea that the central N1s evoked by stimuli that are associated to actions indexes an early and automatic inhibition of the actions these stimuli just activated. The strength of this inhibition would fluctuate from trial to trial, allowing fast-presses when weaker, slower presses when stronger and preventing presses when maximum, as in the count- and the look-only condition. On the other hand, the larger central N1s for the look-only- and for the count-task support the idea that the early and automatic inhibition is somewhat dependent on the task instructions and is boosted when subjects have to prevent the action. This task sensitivity would be consistent with findings showing that even automatic processes can be modulated to some extent by the task (e.g., Danion, 2013), just as the knee reflex can be prevented, reduced, or, on the contrary, amplified.

However, results can be viewed from a wider perspective. As mentioned in the introduction, it is likely that, whenever the system detects that an action can be done, an inhibition has to prevent it until it is time to perform this action. This was probably the case as soon as our participants arrived in the testing room and saw the space bar they could press. Seeing this stimulus instantly activated the press action. A continuous inhibition of the pressing action had to take place until the experiment started and until the space bar was illuminated during the press-block. This illumination had to temporarily dampen this continuous inhibition so that the press action could be performed. On the contrary, in no-press trials, this continuous inhibition had to be reinforced each time the stimulus occurs, as this occurrence systematically (re)activates the action and, importantly, does so for both hands. It is this reinforcement that could be responsible for the bilaterally large N1 deflections observed for no press trials not only over central-, but also over frontal-sites (Fz, F3, F4, F7 and F8) as depicted by Figures 1, 2. Inappropriate weaker reinforcements would account for the slightly more negative ERPs observed for slow- than for fast-presses over F3, Fz, F4 and F8.

In contrast, during fast press trials, no such reinforcement would have occurred, hence the smaller negativities in the time window of the N1 deflections observed over central and frontal sites, except at the left central ones (C3) where an LRP would make the ERPs more negative. This LRP could index the temporary dampening of the continuous inhibition so that the press action can be performed, as mentioned above. In other terms, the LRP could index yet another inhibition mechanism. As it would be made exclusively for the index of the right hand, the finger with which participants had to respond, it would account for the (left) distribution of the LRP over the scalp. This distribution is much more restricted than that of the central N1, the index of the reinforcement of the inhibition. The LRP would prevent ERPs of the fast-presses to be less negative than those of the other conditions only at left central sites (C3).

The second negative deflections, the N300s, appeared after the P3b, which can be the case in designs where only one stimulus is presented repeatedly and where no choice as to be made by participants (Donchin et al., 1978). N300s appeared in this study as large negative going deflections peaking around 400 ms post onset at central scalp sites. The lateness of their peak (i.e., 400 ms) could be due to the unusual presentations used here, that is, that of a stimulus suddenly occurring out of the dark, to which the subject was thus adapted during most of the experiment. These N300s were largest in the condition-blocks where presses had to be prevented, that is, for the look- and the count-task. This appears consistent with the idea that the N300 would, like the Nogo N2, indexes a late inhibition of actions, which is not automatic but occurs in tasks and contexts that require it and can relay the early inhibition indexed by the central N1. The N300 duration, which appears here to be longer than that of the N300s obtained for a unique stimulus repeated in an entire block of trials (Debruille et al., 2012), could be due to the prolonged duration of the presentations of the space bar. The brain continued to be activated by the presentation of a real 3D object for 500 ms, maintaining the need for inhibition.

Although a real object was used as a stimulus here, it was appearing in very special conditions. Only the space bar was visible, not the rest of the keyboard. Moreover, the room was in complete darkness most of the times. Finally, the task of the subject was not to write a text and to press the space bar between two words, as is normally done with a space bar. It was to press it suddenly, completely outside of the context of its normal use. In such conditions, the appearance of the object may thus elicit an N300 even when the task instruction specifies to act upon the affordance and press the space bar as fast as possible. This could account for the remaining N300 deflections that can be seen in the ERPs of the press task and would be compatible with the idea that the N300 would, like the NoGo N2, indexes a late inhibition of actions, which is not automatic but occurs in tasks and contexts that require it. Note that, together with the attentional blink (Raymond et al., 1992) and/or some motor refractory period, these remaining N300s activities might account for the absence of a second press in the case of fast-press trials. In effect, given that the space bar remained visible for 500 ms, it continued to activate the action of pressing it, which could have triggered a second press.

The large N1 and N300 differences found between fast- and slow-press trials reveal how much ERPs can differ from one trial to the next for the same stimulus presented in the same task. The inhibitory processes they index would thus spontaneously fluctuate in strength along time, just like vigilance. Another thing that may be inferred from the data is that even the late inhibition, that is, the one indexed by the N300s, would not last for a very long time. This can be deducted from the presence of the large N300s in the look-only- and in the count-condition, which show that N300s occurred at many of the presentations of the space bar along the trials of these blocks. If the N300 inhibition had a long duration, no more N300 processes would have been needed after a few trials in these blocks and no large N300s would have been observed on the averages. The N1- and N300-inhibitions could provide the system with just enough time to fully process the meaning of the occurrence of the stimulus according to its context before acting. This meaning process is known to be indexed by the N400 ERP (for reviews, see Debruille, 2007; Kutas and Federmeier, 2011; and Debruille et al., 2008; Shang and Debruille, 2013), which peaks around 450 ms for objects that are presented normally, that is, not in a complete darkness (see for instance Ganis and Kutas, 2003; Mudrik et al., 2009).

On the other hand, parietal P1s were marginally larger for fastest presses than for other condition-blocks. This is reminiscent of the larger centro-parietal P1 found by Kiefer et al. (2011). It thus seems possible to hypothesize that these centro-parietal P1s index the beginning of the activations of representations of the actions associated to stimuli. Further studies using stimuli activating actions less elementary than just pressing a computer key might be necessary to have larger P1 differences whose analysis will lead to clear-cut results. Note that the possibility that these larger P1s could, by overlap across participants, be responsible for the smaller central N1s can be eliminated by examining the sites at which no larger P1s can be seen for fast-presses and where N1s are still smaller (e.g., at FCz and FC4).

In conclusion, the results of this study suggest that components of the central N1 may index an immediate inhibition of actions whose prior activations could be reflected by larger parieto-central P1s. Further studies with methods such as the one used by Rabovsky and McRae (2014) or by Cisek (2006), should be performed to see whether the amount of inhibition in the hidden layer(s) of their neuro-computational model could predict the size of the anterior N1s. In any case, the results indicate that after the start of the activation of the actions, the occurrence of an object triggers two stages of inhibition, an early and systematic one, and a later one. On the other hand, the present results and the experimental design in which they were obtained suggest that the LRP itself could index yet another kind of inhibition, namely, a temporary suppression of the continuous inhibition that is needed to prevent acting at inappropriate times.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of The Tri-Council Policy Statement: Ethical Conduct for Research Involving Humans (TCPS or the Policy) used by the Research and Ethics Board of the Douglas University Institute, which also approved the written informed consent form for all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Research and Ethics Board of the Douglas University Institute.

Author Contributions

JD had the initial idea of central N1 and motor inhibition. He imagined the principle of the experiment and wrote drafts of the manuscript. MT designed the details of the experiment, wrote the stimulus sequence, ran half of the participants and agreed on the manuscript. JS and CS finished the testing, processed the ERPs and the RTs, performed the measures, ran the statistical analyses and agreed on the manuscript. LR had key ideas as to the writing of the manuscript and corrected last versions.

Funding

This study was supported by the 2014-PR-171935 grant from the Fonds Québécois de la Recherche sur la Nature et les Technologies (FQRNT) allocated to JD, Principal Investigator (PI). LR was supported by grant 132/14 from the BIAL foundation.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are thankful to Mathieu Brodeur and Paul Cisek for their inputs. We also thank Julie Henry for proof-reading the manuscript and the reviewers. This study has first been released as a pre-print (Touzel et al., 2018).

Footnotes

  1. ^ Although Gibson (1977) first introduced the term to refer to the physical properties of the environment that are associated with actions, such as the grasping afforded by a stick or the warming afforded by a fire.

References

Archibald, S. J., Mateer, C. A., and Kerns, K. A. (2001). Utilization behavior: clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychol. Rev. 11, 117–130. doi: 10.1023/A:1016673807158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atas, A., and Cleeremans, A. (2015). The temporal dynamic of automatic inhibition of irrelevant action. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 289–305. doi: 10.1037/a0038654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bekker, E. M., Kenemans, J. L., Hoeksma, M. R., Talsma, D., and Verbaen, M. N. (2005). The pure electrophysiology of stopping. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 55, 191–198. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binkofski, F., and Buxbaum, L. J. (2013). Two action systems in the human brain. Brain Lang. 127, 222–229. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A., and Riggio, L. (2008). Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances. Brain Res. 1253, 117–128. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruin, K. J., and Wijers, A. A. (2002). Inhibition, response mode and stimulus probability: a comparative event-related potential study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 1172–1182. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00141-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buxbaum, L. J., and Kalénine, S. (2010). Action knowledge, visuomotor activation and embodiment in the two action systems. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1191, 201–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05447.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, L. L., and Martin, A. (2000). Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream. Neuroimage 12, 478–484. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisek, P. (2006). Integrated neural processes for defining potential actions and deciding between them: A computational model. J. Neurosci. 26, 9761–9770. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5605-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science? Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 345–351. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01361-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coles, M. G. H., Gratton, G., and Donchin, E. (1988). Detecting early communication: using measures of movement-related potentials to illuminate human information processing. Biol. Psychol. 26, 69–89. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(88)90014-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danion, F. (2013). Superposition of automatic and voluntary aspects of grip force control in humans during object manipulation. PLoS One 8:e79341. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Debruille, J. B. (2007). The N400 potential could index a semantic inhibition. Brain Res. Rev. 56, 472–477. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Debruille, J. B., Brodeur, M. B., and Franco Porras, C. (2012). N300 and social affordances: a study with a real person and a dummy as stimuli. PLoS One 7:e47922. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Debruille, J. B., Ramirez, D., Wolf, Y., Schaefer, A., Nguyen, T. V., Bacon, B. A., et al. (2008). Knowledge inhibition and N400: a within-and a between-subjects study with distractor words. Brain Res. 1187, 167–183. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donchin, E., Ritter, W., and McCallum, W. C. (1978). “Cognitive psychophysiology: the endogenous components of the ERP,” in Event-Related Brain Potentials in Man, 349–411. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-155150-6.50019-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M., and Schlaghecken, F. (2003). Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming. Biol. Psychol. 64, 7–26. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(03)00100-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Electrode Position Nomenclature Committee. (1991). American electroencephalographic society guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 8, 200–202. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199104000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., and Hohsbein, J. (1999). ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychol. 101, 267–291. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00008-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franca, M., Turella, L., Canto, R., Brunelli, N., Allione, L., Andreasi, N. G., et al. (2012). Corticospinal facilitation during observation of graspable objects: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. PLoS One 7:e49025. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganis, G., and Kutas, M. (2003). An electrophysiological study of scene effects on object identification. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 16, 123–144. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00244-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, J. (1977). “The theory of affordances,” in Perceiving, Acting and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology, eds R. Shaw, J. Bransford (NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 67–82.

Grafton, S. T., Fadiga, L., Arbib, M. A., and Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. Neuroimage 6, 231–236. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenhouse, G. W., and Geisser, S. (1959). On Methods of Analysis of Profile Data. Psychometrika 24, 1582–1589. doi: 10.1007/BF02289823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grèzes, J., and Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia 40, 202–222. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00089-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillyard, S. A., and Anllo-Vento, L. (1998). Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 781–787. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillyard, S. A., Mangun, G. R., Woldorff, M. G., and Luck, S. J. (1995). Neural systems mediating selective attention. Cogn. Neurosci. ed. M. S. Gazzaniga (Cambridge, MA: Mit Press), 665–681.

Google Scholar

Horváth, J. (2013). Action-sound coincidence-related attenuation of auditory ERPs is not modulated by affordance compatibility. Biol. Psychol. 93, 81–87. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jodo, E., and Kayama, Y. (1992). Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 82, 477–482. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(92)90122-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johannes, S., Munte, T. F., Heinze, H. J., and Mangun, G. R. (1995). Luminance and spatial attention effects on early visual processing. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2, 189–205. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(95)90008-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenemans, J. L. (2015). Specific proactive and generic reactive inhibition. Neurosci. Biobehavi. Rev. 56, 115–126. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiefer, M., Sim, E.-J., Helbig, H., and Graf, M. (2011). Tracking the time course of action priming on object recognition: evidence for fast and slow influences of action on perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1864–1874. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korb, S., Grangean, D., and Sherer, K. R. (2010). Timing and voluntary suppression of facial mimicry to smiling faces in a Go/NoGo task—an EMG study. Biolo. Psychol. 85, 347–349. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 621–647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S., Fan, S., and Hillyard, S. A. (1993). Attention-related modulation of sensory-evoked brain activity in a visual search task. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 188–195. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., Heinze, H. J., Mangun, G. R., and Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 75, 528–542. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90138-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matheson, H., White, N., and McMullen, P. (2015). Accessing embodied object representations from vision: a review. Psychol. Bull. 141, 511–524. doi: 10.1037/bul0000001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K. (2000). The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 59–65. doi: 10.1038/35036228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J., and Hackley, S. A. (1992). Electrophysiological evidence for temporal overlap among contingent mental processes. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 121, 195–209. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.121.2.195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mudrik, L., Lamy, D., and Deouell, L. Y. (2009). ERP evidence for context congruity effects during simultaneous object-scene processing. Neuropsychologia 48, 507–517. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niemi, P., and Näätänen, R. (1981). Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychol. Bull. 89, 133–162. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.89.1.133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ocampo, B., and Finkbeiner, M. (2013). The negative compatibility effect with relevant masks: a case for automatic motor inhibition. Front. Psychol. 4:822. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ocklenburg, S., Gunturkun, O., and Beste, C. (2011). Lateralized neural mechanisms underlying the modulation of response inhibition processes. Neuroimage 55, 1771–1778. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, J. C., and Ward, R. (2002). S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: time course and specificity of response activation. Vis. Cogn. 9, 540–558. doi: 10.1080/13506280143000575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pires, L., Leitao, J., Guerrini, C., and Simoes, M. R. (2014). Event-related brain potentials in the study of inhibition: cognitive control, source localization and age-related modulations. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 461–490. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9275-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proverbio, A. M., Del Zotto, M., and Zani, A. (2007). The emergence of semantic categorization in early visual processing: ERP indices of animal vs. BMC Neurosci. 8:24. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-8-24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabovsky, M., and McRae, K. (2014). Simulating the N400 ERP component as semantic network error: insights from a feature-based connectionist model of word meaning. Cognition 132, 68–89. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raud, L., and Huster, R. J. (2017). The temporal dynamics of response inhibition and their modulation by cognitive control. Brain Topogr. 30, 486–501. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0566-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., and Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 18, 849–860. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002a). “Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: empirical clarification through distributional analyses,” in Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance (Vol. XIX), eds W. Prinz and B. Hommel (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 494–519.

Google Scholar

Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2002b). Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: Activation and suppression in conflict tasks. Psychol. Res. 66, 312–323. doi: 10.1007/s00426-002-0104-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Wijnen, J., and Burle, B. (2004). “Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots,” in Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, ed. M. Posner (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 369–377.

Google Scholar

Riddoch, M. J., Edwards, M. G., Humphreys, G. W., West, R., and Heafield, T. (1998). Visual affordances direct action: neuropsychological evidence from manual interference. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 15, 645–683. doi: 10.1080/026432998381041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Matelli, M. (2003). Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions. Exp. Brain Res. 153, 146–157. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1588-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Luppino, G., and Matelli, M. (1998). The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 106, 283–296. doi: 10.1016/s0013-4694(98)00022-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roche, R. A. P., Garavan, H., Foxe, J. J., and O’Mara, S. M. (2005). Individual differences discriminate event-related potentials but not performance during response inhibition. Exp. Brain Res. 160, 60–70. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1985-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, C., Lavoie, B. A., Barbeau, H., and Capaday, C. (2004). Timing of cortical excitability changes during the reaction time of movements superimposed on tonic motor activity. J. Appl. Physiol. 97, 2220–2227. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00542.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuck, S., Bayliss, A. P., Klein, C., and Tipper, S. P. (2010). Attention modulates motor system activation during observation: evidence for inhibitory rebound. Exp. Brain Res. 205, 235–249. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2358-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senderecka, M. (2016). Threatening visual stimuli influence response inhibition and error monitoring: an event-related potential study. Biol. Psychol. 113, 24–36. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shang, M., and Debruille, J. B. (2013). N400 processes inhibit inappropriately activated representations: adding a piece of evidence from a high-repetition design. Neuropsychologia 51, 1989–1997. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumner, P., Nachev, P., Morris, P., Peters, A. M., Jackson, S. R., Kennard, C., et al. (2007). Human medial frontal cortex mediates unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Neuron 54, 697–711. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Symes, E., Ellis, R., and Tucker, M. (2006). Visual object affordances: object orientation. Acta Psychol. 124, 238–255. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tipper, S. P. (2010). From observation to action simulation: the role of attention, eye-gaze, emotion and body state. The Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 2081–2105. doi: 10.1080/17470211003624002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tipper, S. P., Lortie, C., and Baylis, G. C. (1992). Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 18, 891–905. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.18.4.891

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tipper, S. P., Paul, M. A., and Hayes, A. E. (2006). Vision-for-action: the effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 398–493. doi: 10.3758/bf03193875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Touzel, M., Snidal, C., Segal, J., Renoult, L., and Debruille, J. B. (2018). A central component of the N1 event-related brain potential could index the early and automatic inhibition of the actions systematically activated by objects. bioRxiv:341057 [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/341057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, M., and Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 830–846. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.3.830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vainio, L., Hammarén, L., Hausen, M., Rekolainen, E., and Riskilä, S. (2011). Motor inhibition associated with the affordance of briefly displayed objects. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 64, 1094–1110. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.538221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wild-Wall, N., Falkenstein, M., and Hohnsbein, J. (2008). Flanker interference in young and older participants as reflected in event-related potentials. Brain Res. 1211, 72–84. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 625–636. doi: 10.3758/bf03196322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: affordances, actions associated to stimuli, ERPs, N1, LRP, P1, early activation, automatic inhibition

Citation: Debruille JB, Touzel M, Segal J, Snidal C and Renoult L (2019) A Central Component of the N1 Event-Related Brain Potential Could Index the Early and Automatic Inhibition of the Actions Systematically Activated by Objects. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:95. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00095

Received: 11 September 2018; Accepted: 17 April 2019;
Published: 08 May 2019.

Edited by:

Carlos Tomaz, Universidade Ceuma, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Paula Rowe, City University of London, United Kingdom
Carlos Enrique Uribe, Universidade de Brasília, Brazil

Copyright © 2019 Debruille, Touzel, Segal, Snidal and Renoult. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: J. Bruno Debruille, bruno.debruille@mcgill.ca

Download