J Korean Med Sci. 2023 Jul 24;38(29):e228. English.
Published online Jul 05, 2023.
© 2023 The Korean Academy of Medical Sciences.
Original Article

Developing an Attention Assessment Tool for Individuals With Autism Spectrum Disorder Using Timbre, Rhythm, and Pitch

Jin Hyung Lee,1 Hyunchan Hwang,1 Juri Yun,2 Hee Jun Kim,3 Seo-Koo Yoo,4 Un Sun Chung,5 and Doug Hyun Han1
    • 1Department of Psychiatry, Chung-Ang University Hospital, Seoul, Korea.
    • 2Department of Music Therapy, Ewha Womans University, Seoul, Korea.
    • 3Department of Oncology, Chung-Ang University Hospital, Seoul, Korea.
    • 4School of Social Welfare, Soongsil University, Seoul, Korea.
    • 5Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Kyungpook National University, Daegu, Korea.
Received November 15, 2022; Accepted March 28, 2023.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Background

Music is regarded as a beneficial tool for assessing the clinical symptoms and communication skills in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or autism. The present study developed a music-based attention test (MAT) for individuals with autism using music parameters and the algorithm of the comprehensive attention test (CAT).

Methods

We recruited 51 autistic individuals and 50 neurotypical individuals to participate in the CAT, MAT, and social intelligence tests. The reliability and validity of the MAT were assessed using exploratory factor analysis, concurrent validity, and criterion-related validity.

Results

The MAT had sound internal consistency (high Cronbach’s α = 0.948). In addition, the MAT had suitable concurrent validity in the correlation between CAT and MAT, as well as good criterion validity when attention was measured using the MAT and was compared between autistic individuals and neurotypical individuals. Attention evaluated using the MAT was associated with the social quotient in individuals with autism.

Conclusions

The MAT could be a relevant tool for gauging attention in individuals with ASD. Furthermore, attention determined using the MAT may be correlated with social quotient in autistic individuals. Future studies should consider that using music in the field of attention could improve the social quotient of individuals with autism.

Graphical Abstract

Keywords
Music Attention Test; Comprehensive Attention Test; Autism Spectrum Disorder; Social Quotient

INTRODUCTION

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or autism is characterized by social communication impairments that hamper interest and stereotypical behaviors,1 and attention changes may be present in autistic individuals.2, 3, 4 A meta-analysis reported that 22% of individuals with autism also met the diagnostic criteria for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in population-based cohorts and 34% met the criteria for ADHD in clinical settings.5

Attention is associated with social emotion and communication in the general population, and in individuals with developmental disorders.6 In fact, attention in individuals with autism is connected with the learning process, which may be influenced by emotions and the ability to speak, which can also be mediated by attention.7 For instance, high-intensity emotional information is prioritized for attention processing.8 Moreover, decreased attention in autistic individuals could be associated with greater difficulties in adapting to functioning, social cognition, and poor quality of life.9

Social cognition is a sub-topic of various branches of psychology that focuses on how humans process, store, and apply information in social interactions.10 Social cognition in autistic children is associated with attention-related metacognitive processes such as initiation and planning.11 Social quotient (SQ) was measured using the George Washington University Social Intelligence test.12 Social intelligence, a parallel concept of social quotient, is defined as the capacity to know oneself and to develop social awareness. Social skills are defined as tools that can facilitate verbal and nonverbal interaction and communication with others.13

Music has evolved over time and is organized by sounds and silences.14 Thus, ongoing changes in the elements of music, such as rhythmic patterns, tempi, and dynamics, draw listeners’ attention.15 Rhythmic stimuli in songs play a significant role in drawing attention and listener engagement, thereby promoting the acquisition of pre-academic concepts.16 Moreover, research evidence from music and neuroscience has demonstrated that music and rhythm impact numerous brain structures, promoting more efficient brain processes.17 Taken together, these findings suggest that music can be a suitable tool for assessing auditory attention.18, 19 However, there are few published music-based attention assessment tools.20

Music could be a relevant tool to judge clinical symptoms and communication skills in individuals with autism.21 Moreover, some autistic individuals have an interest and/or prominent talent in music.20, 21, 22 Heaton23 reported that autistic individuals showed better pitch discrimination than their global intelligence quotient (IQ) would indicate. Jiang and colleagues24 stated that individuals with autism showed superior melodic contour identification as compared to neurotypical individuals.

Additionally, music therapy can improve social skills and communication in autistic individuals.21, 24, 25 LaGasse26 described that music therapy could improve joint attention with peers and eye contact in individuals with autism. Romani and colleagues27 suggested that music could elicit special attention in autistic children. Neurotypical children showed a similar pattern of attention in response to music, speech, and environmental noise, whereas autistic children showed an enhanced reaction to music in response to speech and environmental noise.

Based on previous findings on the relationships among attention, ASD, and music characteristics, we developed an attention test tool using music factors. We hypothesized that the music attention test (MAT) would be internally reliable and valid for evaluating attention in autistic individuals. In addition, attention measured using the MAT could be correlated with social intelligence in individuals with autism.

METHODS

Participants

As suggested by the minimum sample size in factor analysis, the rule of 100 was applied to the total sample size in this study.28, 29 A subject-to-variable (STV) ratio of 3:6 is an acceptable standard.30 Therefore, we recruited 51 autistic individuals (50 participants [Subjects]/13 items [Variables] = 3.84) and 50 neurotypical control participants. Case-control with a frequency matching design was used to evaluate the attention between autistic subjects and neurotypical control subjects.

Fifty-four young autistic individuals were screened through advertisements. Of these 54, three were excluded because they could not understand the performance rule of the MAT. Thus, we had 51 autistic individuals who completed the study protocol. The inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) age 18 years or older, 2) ASD based on the Diagnostic Statistical Manual (DSM-5) or International Classification of Disease-10 (ICD-10), and 3) autism grades 2 and 3 for the individuals with disabilities based on the Korean government’s criteria for judging the level of the individuals with disabilities. The exclusion criteria were as follows: 1) history of head trauma, 2) history of other medical diseases including multiple sclerosis, brain tumor, cerebrovascular accident, and serious or chronic medical illness, and 3) autism grade 1 for the individuals with disabilities based on the Korean government’s criteria for judging the level of the individuals with disabilities.

We also recruited 50 age- and sex-matched healthy neurotypical control participants through advertisements. They were screened using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Disorder version 5 (SCID-5) and interviewed by two psychiatrists.

Development and expert verification of the MAT

The development and expert verification of the MAT were conducted in four stages, with four panel members; two psychiatrists specializing in developmental health and two certified music therapists. In the first stage, the researchers met with the panel members to discuss the core concept of the CAT and relevant musical stimuli that could replace the visual tasks in the CAT. As the CAT requires participants to continually respond to the same or different visual stimuli as the target stimuli, we considered possible musical elements that could replace the target stimuli and explored the possibility of employing sounds with different musical timbres, rhythmic patterns, pitches, and harmonic structures.

In the second stage, the two researchers, with a combined experience of over 25 years in music therapy, designed a pilot test for each subset based on the above-mentioned musical elements. They observed and recorded the responses of five adults with autism or mild intellectual disability on the test items. For instance, the participants were asked to raise their fingers when they heard the same sound as the target sound presented at the beginning of each new section.

In the third stage, the panel members examined the responses from the field test and discussed the relevance of the musical elements in assessing each property of attention. Based on this analysis, they changed the selection of musical timbres, pitches, and the tempo of rhythmic patterns that comprised each item. They removed the harmonic structure, as the participants appeared to have more difficulty comprehending and completing the tasks presented. In the final stage, the panel members examined and verified each refined item for the MAT and provided additional input on updating and finalizing the test items.

Based on the analysis of the CAT (Supplementary Data 1), the MAT was designed to measure the same subsets of attention assessed in the CAT using musical factors, such as timbre, rhythm, and pitch (Table 1). The CAT is one of the gold standards for measuring attention in Korea.31, 32, 33 Established from the CAT algorithm, the MAT consisted of five testing subsets: simple selective attention, continuous inhibition, interference selection, divided attention, and working memory tests. Simple selective attention was determined using visual and timbre, timbre alone, rhythm, and pitch discrimination. Continuous inhibition was evaluated using timbre, rhythm, and pitch. Divided attention was gauged using a combination of visual images with timbre as well as a combination of rhythm and pitch. Working memory was appraised using a combination of images and timbre, a combination of timbre and rhythm, and a reverse combination of image and tone.

Table 1
Comparisons of sub-items between the CAT and MAT, including timing of items

In the selective attention test, participants were prompted to press the button labeled “same” whenever they saw or heard the specified target image and/or sound (e.g., an image of the instrument with a single tone of the same instrument, different rhythmic subdivisions of a beat played on a snare drum, and four pitches of C4 to F4 played on a digital synthesizer). In the continuous inhibition test, participants were asked to press a button labeled “different” whenever they heard any sound other than the specified target. The items presented in this subset also included timbre, rhythm, and pitch discrimination tasks. Similar to the interference selection test in CAT, this subset measured the subject’s ability to notice changes in the image in the center while ignoring the other two images placed on either side of the target image. We designed items for this subset with four rhythm instruments: maracas, clave, hand drum, and agogo bell. Two types of agogo bells—high-tone and low-tone—were presented alternately as the target, and the participants were encouraged to press either the “high” or “low” button after hearing each combination of tones.

In the first divided attention test, participants were asked to press the button labeled “repeated” if each stimulus presented either the same image or sound from the immediate past item (e.g., image of instruments or timbre). In the same format, the second divided attention test prompted subjects to press the “repeated” button every time the same rhythmic division or pitch played on a piano was presented. For the first working memory test, the participants remembered the orders and positions of the highlighted figures and then clicked on the figures in the same order they were presented. As a music-based test, each figure was paired with a tone of rhythm instrument, and the same tone was played when the subjects clicked on each figure. We postulated that these added sound stimuli would assist participants to crosscheck their memory of the visual sequence and make the experience more pleasant. The second working memory test presented the same task but prompted participants to respond in the reverse order. The third working memory test presents each visual figure with a different rhythmic pattern. Consequently, the participants had to play the same rhythmic pattern in the same sequence that was presented.

The MAT was developed using the Unity platform, and an Android tablet PC was used to perform the tests. Each subtest began with a detailed tutorial, using a visual guide with voice instructions. A trained researcher assisted each participant in proceeding with the test and provided additional explanations whenever required.

Assessment

Clinical symptoms and psychological scales

The clinical symptoms of autism were assessed using the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS).34 The IQ and social quotient in all participants (participants with ASD and neurotypical control participants) were evaluated using the Korean version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (K-WAIS)35 and George Washington University Social Intelligence Test,12 respectively. Attention-related clinical symptoms were measured using the Korean Adult Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale (K-AARS).36 Anxiety and depression were demonstrated using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI)37 and the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D),38 respectively.

The attention of all participants was assessed using the CAT and MAT. First, subjects were asked to undergo the CAT. After 30 minutes of rest, the participants were asked to undergo the MAT.

Attention ability assessment using the comprehensive attention test (CAT)

The CAT is a standardized computer software program for judging attentiveness in individuals aged 4–49 years (Supplementary Data 1).31, 32, 33 It consisted of five subsets: simple selective attention, continuous inhibition, interference selection, divided attention, and working memory. Except for working memory, the other four subsets had five indicators: omission errors, commission errors, mean reaction time, standard deviation of reaction time, and sensitivity coefficient. The working memory subset had two indicators: correct response number and memory span. More information on the CAT has been provided in previous studies.39 In this study, we considered the number of omission errors, commission errors, and correct response numbers in the working memory subset. Omission and commission errors in simple selective attention, continuous inhibition, interference selection, divided attention, and correct response numbers in the working memory subset could represent attention ability in individuals with ADHD.39, 40

Statistical analysis

Comparison of demographic characteristics between autistic and neurotypical control participants

We used independent sample t-tests to assess the differences in demographic characteristics, including age, sex, years of education, and economic status, as well as psychological status (including CARS, CES-D, BAI, K-AARS, IQ, and SQ) between autistic and neurotypical control subjects. The differences in the total and sub-categorical CAT scores between autistic and neurotypical control participants were demonstrated using an independent sample t-test. In addition, the correlation between CAT, K-AARS, and SQ in all participants was calculated using partial correlation, controlling for IQ, along with post hoc tests using the same restrictions.

Validation of MAT using exploratory factor analysis, concurrent validity, and partial correlation

Factor analysis for the 13 items of the MAT included Cronbach’s α as the consistency coefficient. The construct validity of the MAT was gauged using exploratory factor analysis (EFA), including the principal axis method of factor extraction method, maximum likelihood rotation method, and direct oblimin rotation with Kaiser normalization. To measure sampling adequacy, the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure and Bartlett’s test were used. Conventionally, KMO values between 0.8 and 1 and a significant P value for Bartlett’s test indicate sample adequacy.41, 42 The concurrent validity of the CAT and MAT was demonstrated using the Pearson correlation, and the criterion-related validity between the CAT and MAT was assessed using an independent sample t-test.

Correlation analyses among MAT, K-AARS, and SQ

Finally, the correlation among MAT, K-AARS, and SQ in all participants was calculated using partial correlation, controlling for IQ, along with post hoc tests using the same limitations. Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05. In the correlation analyses among CAT, K-AARS, and SQ in all participants, the statistical significance was set at P < 0.02 (0.05/3), considering multiple comparisons. For concurrent validity, statistical significance was set at P < 0.004 (0.05/13), also considering multiple comparisons. All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 24 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

Ethical statement

All participants were provided compensation of $50 for completing the study protocol. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Chung-Ang University (No. 1041078-202008-HRBM-235-01). Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and Institutional Committees on Human Experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

We purchased K-WAIS to clear copyright for this study and we confirmed that no authorization is needed to use other scales for research purpose.

RESULTS

Demographic characteristics

There were no significant differences in age, sex, years of education, or economic status between participants with ASD and neurotypical controls. However, autistic subjects showed lower scores on SQ and IQ, as well as higher scores on CARS, K-AARS, BAI, and CES-D, as compared to neurotypical participants (Table 2).

Comparison of CAT between participants with ASD and neurotypical controls

In the CAT, individuals with autism showed increased errors in all subcategories of simple omission, commission, inhibition omission, inhibition commission, interference selection omission, interference selection commission, divided attention omission, and divided attention commission, as compared with neurotypical participants. In addition, autistic individuals displayed decreased scores in forward working memory and backward working memory, as compared to neurotypical subjects (Table 2).

Correlations among CAT, SQ, and Clinical Attention Scale Scores

For all participants, SQ was negatively correlated with simple omission (r = −0.311, degrees of freedom [df] = 99, P = 0.001), commission (r = −0.334, df = 99, P < 0.001), inhibition omission (r = −0.325, df = 99, P < 0.001), inhibition commission (r = −0.356, df = 99, P < 0.001), interference selection omission (r = −0.306, df = 99, P = 0.002), interference selection commission (r = −0. 362, df = 99, P < 0.001), divided attention omission (r = −0.319, df = 99, P < 0.001), and divided attention commission (r = −0.382, df = 99, P < 0.001). In addition, SQ was positively correlated with forward working memory (r = 0.343, df = 99, P < 0.001) and backward working memory (r = 0.313, df = 99, P = 0.001) (Supplementary Table 1).

In autistic individuals, SQ was negatively correlated with inhibition omission (r = −0.404, df = 49, P = 0.004), interference selection commission (r = −0.416, df = 49, P = 0.002), and divided attention commission (r = −0.433, df = 49, P = 0.002). Moreover, SQ was positively correlated with forward (r = 0.480, df = 49, P = 0.001) and backward (r = 0.459, df = 49, P = 0.001) working memory.

In neurotypical participants, SQ was not associated with any item in the comprehensive test. For all participants, the K-AARS was not related to any items in the CAT.

Analysis of internal consistency of the MAT

The MAT consisting of 13 items is reliable, with a high Cronbach’s alpha (α = 0.948). When each item was eliminated from the total number of MAT items individually, the Cronbach’s α of the 13 items was above 0.940, meaning that none of the items compromised reliability within the MAT (Supplementary Table 2).

Construct validity of the MAT

In the EFA, the KMO was 0.913 and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was acceptable (χ2 = 1,210.796, P < 0.001). All 13 items were segregated into two factors (10 + 3 items). The eigenvalues of the 13 items were significant at > 0.4. Under factor one, 10 items of the MAT were segregated with 64.63% variance, 64.63% cumulative percentage, and 8.402 total eigenvalues (Table 2). The range of parameter estimates for factor one was 0.580 for the inhibition timbre item and 0.938 for the inhibition rhythm. Under factor two, three items of the MAT had 7.846% variance, 72.464% cumulative percentage, and 1.018 total eigenvalues. The range of parameter estimates for factor two was 0.693 for reverse working memory vision and timbre, and 0.944 for working memory vision and timbre (Table 3).

Concurrent validity of the CAT and MAT

Simple omission of the CAT was negatively correlated with simple vision (r = −0.546, df = 99, P < 0.001), simple timbre (r = −0.415, df = 99, P < 0.001), and simple rhythm (r = −0.811, df = 99, P < 0.001) of the MAT. Simple commission of the CAT was negatively associated with the simple vision of the MAT (r = −0.446, df = 99, P < 0.001; Table 4).

Inhibition omission of the CAT was negatively correlated with the inhibition timbre (r = −0.361, df = 99, P < 0.001) and inhibition rhythm (r = −0.339, df = 99, P = 0.001) of the MAT. Inhibition commission of the CAT was negatively related to inhibition timbre (r = −0.321, df = 99, P = 0.001), inhibition pitch (r = −0.676, df = 99, P < 0.001), and inhibition rhythm (r = −0.314, df = 99, P = 0.001) of the MAT (Table 4).

The interference selection omission of the CAT was negatively linked to the interference selection of the MAT (r = −0.301, df = 99, P = 0.002). The interference selection commission of the CAT was negatively connected to the interference selection of the MAT (r = 0.755, df = 99, P < 0.001; Table 4).

The divided omission of the CAT was negatively correlated with divided attention (vision and timbre; r = −0.319, df = 99, P = 0.002) of the MAT (Table 4). The divided commission of the CAT was negatively coupled with divided attention (vision and timbre; r = −0.758, df = 99, P < 0.001 and rhythm and pitch; r = −0.754, df = 99, P < 0.001; Table 4).

Working memory in the CAT was positively correlated with working memory subgroups (vision and timbre; r = 0.753, df = 99, P < 0.001, timbre and rhythm; r = 0.732, df = 99, P < 0.001, and reverse vision and timbre; r = 0.776, df = 99, P < 0.001). The working memory reverse of the CAT was positively related to working memory (vision and timbre; r = 0.790, df = 99, P < 0.001, timbre and rhythm; r = 0.754, df = 99, P < 0.001, and reverse vision and timbre; r = 0.812, df = 99, P < 0.001; Table 4).

Criterion-related validity by comparison of MAT between participants with ASD and neurotypical controls

In the MAT, autistic individuals showed decreased scores in all subcategories of simple vision, simple timbre, simple rhythm, simple pitch, inhibition timbre, inhibition pitch, inhibition rhythm, interference selection, divided attention vision and timbre, divided attention rhythm and pitch, working memory vision and timbre, working memory timbre and rhythm, and reverse working memory vision and timbre, compared to neurotypical control participants (Table 5).

Table 5
Comparison of MAT between patients with ASD and neurotypical controls

Correlations among MAT, SQ, and Clinical Attention Scale Scores

In all participants, SQ was positively correlated with all subcategories of simple vision (r = 0.399, df = 99, P < 0.001), simple timbre (r = 0.389, df = 99, P < 0.001), simple rhythm (r = 0.324, df = 99, P = 0.001), simple pitch (r = 0.444, df = 99, P < 0.001), inhibition timbre (r = 0.439, df = 99, P < 0.001), inhibition pitch (r = 0.408, df = 99, P < 0.001), inhibition rhythm (r = 0.439, df = 99, P < 0.001), interference selection (r = 0.422, df = 99, P < 0.001), divided attention vision and timbre (r = 0.422, df = 99, P < 0.001, and rhythm and pitch, r = 0.453, df = 99, P < 0.001), working memory (vision and timbre; r = 0.476, df = 99, P < 0.001, timbre and rhythm; r = 0.496, df = 99, P < 0.001, and reverse vision and timbre; r = 0.457, df = 99, P < 0.001) (Supplementary Table 3).

In autistic individuals, SQ was positively associated with subcategories of simple vision (r = 0.413, df = 49, P = 0.001), simple timbre (r = 0.449, df = 49, P < 0.001), simple rhythm (r = 0.401, df = 49, P = 0.004), simple pitch (r = 0.409, df = 49, P = 0.004), inhibition rhythm (r = 0.475, df = 49, P < 0.001), interference selection (r = 0.419, df = 49, P = 0.003), divided attention vision and timbre (r = 0.398, df = 49, P = 0.004), divided attention rhythm and pitch (r = 0.511, df = 49, P < 0.001), working memory vision and timbre (r = 0.523, df = 49, P < 0.001), working memory timbre and rhythm (r = 0.541, df = 49, P < 0.001), and reverse working memory vision and timbre (r = 0.562, df = 49, P < 0.001). In neurotypical control participants, SQ was not associated with any item in the MAT (Supplementary Table 3).

For all participants, the Clinical Attention Scale Scores of the K-AARS were not linked to any item in the MAT (Supplementary Table 3).

DISCUSSION

Our results suggest that the MAT has good internal consistency and concurrent validity. Additionally, attention assessed using the MAT was associated with SQ in autistic individuals. The internal consistency of the MAT for all 13 items was high (Cronbach’s α = 0.948), with each item having significant meanings in the EFA. Furthermore, the MAT showed good concurrent and criterion validity for monitoring attention in autistic individuals.

Many studies on music therapy have highlighted the correlations between musical factors and attention in individuals with typical development and communication difficulties.15, 43, 44, 45, 46 Musical aptitude is associated with working memory and selective attention.46 Pasiali and colleagues15 reasoned that musical attention control training would improve attentional control and selective attention in individuals with ASD. Wigram and Gold45 discovered that music therapy could progress emotional responsiveness, attention span, and behavioral control. Besson and colleagues17 argued that musical training not only enhances auditory attention but also promotes the categorization of information provided and improvements in decision making. In a brain study on music therapy, music therapy had a positive impact on selective attention skills.43 Strait and Kraus44 uncovered associations between musical training and functional brain networks in selective auditory attention. Based on the results of the present study, musical factors can be used to appraise attention in individuals with autism.

The SQ score was positively correlated with attention abilities assessed using the MAT in autistic individuals but not in neurotypical individuals. Music factors have been used to evaluate communication skills and encourage social intelligence in autistic individuals.21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 45, 46, 47 In healthy or neurotypical college students, music enhanced emotional perception during communication.48 In a systematic review of music therapy, music led to outcomes such as improved social interaction, verbal communication skills, initiating behavior, and social-emotional reciprocity.21 Boster and colleagues25 realized that music could improve social participation in individuals with communication disorders.

The rhythm and tune of music factors can assess the abilities of pitch discrimination and melodic contour identification.21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27 Music therapy can improve behavioral and social communication, brain connectivity, and parent-child relationships in individuals with ASD.46 In individuals with autism, difficulties in executive function, including attention, contribute to decreased social cognition.49 In addition, treatments targeting both executive function and social cognition are more effective for the treatment of autism than social skills training alone.50

Based on the results of the present study, attention abilities weighed using musical factors may predict SQ, including interpersonal relationships and communication, in individuals with ASD.

The present study has several limitations. First, the small sample size was insufficient to generalize the results even though the sample size was calculated before starting the study. In particular, the autistic participants in the study had a high level of ability to respond to the music attention task. Second, higher anxiety and lower IQ in autistic individuals could have impacted the results of the attention test. Hurford and colleagues51 stated that IQ would significantly influence the performance of the attention test. Thus, the current results should be interpreted with caution, considering the small sample size, limited inclusion criteria, and impact of anxiety and IQ.

In conclusion, the MAT could be a relevant tool for assessing attention in autistic individuals. Additionally, attention measured using the MAT may be associated with SQ in autistic individuals. Future studies should consider using music in the field of attention to improve the SQ of individuals with autism.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS

Supplementary Data 1

Explanation of the comprehensive attention test (CAT)

Click here to view.(92K, doc)

Supplementary Table 1

Correlations between CAT and SQ

Click here to view.(41K, doc)

Supplementary Table 2

Reliability of the music attention test

Click here to view.(40K, doc)

Supplementary Table 3

Correlations between MAT and SQ

Click here to view.(43K, doc)

Notes

Funding:This research was supported by Culture, Sports and Tourism R&D Program through the Korea Creative Content Agency (KOCCA) grant funded by the Ministry of Culture, Sports and Tourism (MCST) in 2020 (Project Number: R2020040186, Contribution Rate: 100%).

Disclosure:The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Author Contributions:

  • Conceptualization: Lee JH, Kim HJ, Yoo SK, Han DH.

  • Data curation: Lee JH, Yun J, Kim HJ, Yoo SK, Chung US.

  • Formal analysis: Yoo SK, Han DH.

  • Investigation: Lee JH, Yun J, Kim HJ, Chung US.

  • Methodology: Hwang H, Kim HJ.

  • Project administration: Hwang H.

  • Supervision: Han DH.

  • Validation: Hwang H.

  • Writing - original draft: Han DH.

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, D.C., USA: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2013.
    1. Fan J, Bernardi S, Van Dam NT, Anagnostou E, Gu X, Martin L, et al. Functional deficits of the attentional networks in autism. Brain Behav 2012;2(5):647–660.
    1. Petersen SE, Posner MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci 2012;35(1):73–89.
    1. Ravizza SM, Solomon M, Ivry RB, Carter CS. Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: the relationship of attention and motor deficits. Dev Psychopathol 2013;25(3):773–784.
    1. Lai MC, Kassee C, Besney R, Bonato S, Hull L, Mandy W, et al. Prevalence of co-occurring mental health diagnoses in the autism population: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2019;6(10):819–829.
    1. Maemonah S, Hamidah H, Notobroto HB, Sulystiono D, Widarti L. Factors affecting the ability to speak in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Public Health Res 2021;10(2):2236.
    1. Wan CY, Schlaug G. Neural pathways for language in autism: the potential for music-based treatments. Future Neurol 2010;5(6):797–805.
    1. Tyng CM, Amin HU, Saad MN, Malik AS. The influences of emotion on learning and memory. Front Psychol 2017;8:1454.
    1. Sikora DM, Vora P, Coury DL, Rosenberg D. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms, adaptive functioning, and quality of life in children with autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics 2012;130 Suppl 2:S91–S97.
    1. Park M, Song JJ, Oh SJ, Shin MS, Lee JH, Oh SH. The relation between nonverbal IQ and postoperative CI outcomes in cochlear implant users: preliminary result. BioMed Res Int 2015;2015:313274
    1. Miranda A, Berenguer C, Roselló B, Baixauli I, Colomer C. Associations with Executive Functions. Social cognition in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Associations with executive functions. Front Psychol 2017;8(8):1035.
    1. Moss FA, Hunt T, Omwake KT, Woodward LJ. In: Manual for the George Washington University Series Social Intelligence Test, Revised form. 2nd ed. Washington, D.C., USA: Center for Psychological Service; 1955.
    1. Dowd TP, Tierney J. In: Teaching Social Skills to Youth: A Step-by-Step Guide to 182 Basic to Complex Skills Plus Helpful Teaching Techniques. Boys Town, NE, USA: Boys Town Press; 2017.
    1. Radocy R, Boyle D. In: Psychological Foundations of Musical Behavior. 4th ed. Springfield, IL, USA: Charles C. Thomas Publisher; 2003.
    1. Pasiali V, LaGasse AB, Penn SL. The effect of musical attention control training (MACT) on attention skills of adolescents with neurodevelopmental delays: a pilot study. J Music Ther 2014;51(4):333–354.
    1. Geist K, Geist EA. Do Re Mi, 1-2-3: That’s how easy math can be: using music to support emergent mathematics. Young Child 2008;63(2):20–25.
    1. Besson M, Chobert J, Marie C. Transfer of training between music and speech: common processing, attention, and memory. Front Psychol 2011;2:94.
    1. Jeong E. In: Development and validation of a music-based attention assessment for patients with traumatic brain injury [doctoral thesis]. Coral Gables, FL, USA: University of Miami; 2011.
    1. Jeong E, Lesiuk TL. Development and preliminary evaluation of a music-based attention assessment for patients with traumatic brain injury. J Music Ther 2011;48(4):551–572.
    1. Waldon EG, Jacobsen SL, Gattino G. In: Music Therapy Assessment: Theory, Research, and Application. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Publishers; 2018.
    1. Geretsegger M, Elefant C, Mössler KA, Gold C. Music therapy for people with autism spectrum disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2014;2014(6):CD004381
    1. Heaton P. Pitch memory, labelling and disembedding in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2003;44(4):543–551.
    1. Heaton P, Williams K, Cummins O, Happé F. Autism and pitch processing splinter skills: a group and subgroup analysis. Autism 2008;12(2):203–219.
    1. Jiang J, Liu F, Wan X, Jiang C. Perception of melodic contour and intonation in autism spectrum disorder: evidence from Mandarin speakers. J Autism Dev Disord 2015;45(7):2067–2075.
    1. Boster JB, Spitzley AM, Castle TW, Jewell AR, Corso CL, McCarthy JW. Music improves social and participation outcomes for individuals with communication disorders: a systematic review. J Music Ther 2021;58(1):12–42.
    1. LaGasse AB. Effects of a music therapy group intervention on enhancing social skills in children with autism. J Music Ther 2014;51(3):250–275.
    1. Romani M, Martucci M, Castellano Visaggi M, Prono F, Valente D, Sogos C. What if sharing music as a language is the key to meeting halfway? Absolute pitch, pitch discrimination and Autism Spectrum Disorder. Clin Ter 2021;172(6):577–590.
    1. Gorsuch RL. In: Factor Analysis. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Erlbaum; 1983.
    1. Hatcher L. In: A Step-by-Step Approach to Using the SAS® System for Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling. Cary, NC, USA: SAS Institute, Inc.; 1994.
    1. Cattell RB. In: The Scientific Use of Factor Analysis. New York, NY, USA: Plenum; 1978.
    1. Huh HN, Kang SH, Hwang SY, Yoo HK. Developmental trajectories of attention in normal Korean population. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2019;30(2):66–73.
    1. Seo JM, Lee JS, Kim SY, Kim HW. Diagnostic significance of comprehensive attention test in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011;22(4):246–252.
    1. Yoo HI, Lee J, Kang SH, Park EH, Jung J, Kim BN, et al. Standardization of the comprehensive attention test for the Korean children and adolescents. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009;20(2):68–75.
    1. Ozonoff S, Goodlin-Jones BL, Solomon M. Evidence-based assessment of autism spectrum disorders in children and adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2005;34(3):523–540.
    1. Wechsler D. In: Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-IV). 4th ed. San Antonio, TX, USA: Pearson; 2009.
    1. Hong M, Lee YS, Kim B, Joung YS, Yoo HK, Kim EJ, et al. Clinical utility and cut-off scores of the Korean adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder rating scale. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2019;30(3):116–120.
    1. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988;56(6):893–897.
    1. Heo EH, Choi KS, Yu JC, Nam JA. Validation of the center for epidemiological studies depression scale among Korean adolescents. Psychiatry Investig 2018;15(2):124–132.
    1. Hong JS, Lee YS, Hong M, Kim B, Joung YS, Yoo HK, et al. Cognitive developmental trajectories in adult ADHD patients and controls: a comparative study. J Atten Disord 2022;26(3):391–407.
    1. Han DH, Lee YS, Na C, Ahn JY, Chung US, Daniels MA, et al. The effect of methylphenidate on Internet video game play in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Compr Psychiatry 2009;50(3):251–256.
    1. Kaiser HF, Rice J. Little jiffy, mark IV. Educ Psychol Meas 1974;34(1):111–117.
    1. Bartlett MS. Properties of sufficiency and statistical tests. Proc R Soc Lond A Math Phys Sci 1937;160(901):268–282.
    1. LaGasse AB, Manning RC, Crasta JE, Gavin WJ, Davies PL. Assessing the impact of music therapy on sensory gating and attention in children with autism: a pilot and feasibility study. J Music Ther 2019;56(3):287–314.
    1. Strait DL, Kraus N. Can you hear me now? Musical training shapes functional brain networks for selective auditory attention and hearing speech in noise. Front Psychol 2011;2:113.
    1. Wigram T, Gold C. Music therapy in the assessment and treatment of autistic spectrum disorder: clinical application and research evidence. Child Care Health Dev 2006;32(5):535–542.
    1. Williams TI, Loucas T, Sin J, Jeremic M, Aslett G, Knight M, et al. A randomised controlled feasibility trial of music-assisted language telehealth intervention for minimally verbal autistic children-the MAP study protocol. Pilot Feasibility Stud 2021;7(1):182.
    1. Lee JH, Yun J, Hwang H, Kim SM, Han DH. The study of the identification of the Musical Passages for an emotion perception scale for people with developmental disabilities. J Korean Med Sci 2023;38(5):e30
    1. Jiang J, Meng Q, Ji J. Combining music and indoor spatial factors helps to improve college students’ emotion during communication. Front Psychol 2021;12:703908
    1. Paul S, Arora A, Midha R, Vu D, Roy PK, Belmonte MK. Autistic traits and individual brain differences: functional network efficiency reflects attentional and social impairments, structural nodal efficiencies index systemising and theory-of-mind skills. Mol Autism 2021;12(1):3.
    1. Kenworthy L, Anthony LG, Naiman DQ, Cannon L, Wills MC, Luong-Tran C, et al. Randomized controlled effectiveness trial of executive function intervention for children on the autism spectrum. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2014;55(4):374–383.
    1. Hurford DP, Fender AC, Boux JL, Swigart CC, Boydston PS, Butts SR, et al. Examination of the effects of intelligence on the test of variables of attention for elementary students. J Atten Disord 2017;21(11):929–937.

Metrics
Share
Tables

1 / 5

Funding Information
PERMALINK