You are viewing a javascript disabled version of the site. Please enable Javascript for this site to function properly.
Go to headerGo to navigationGo to searchGo to contentsGo to footer
In content section. Select this link to jump to navigation

Improving Our Understanding of Driving Changes in Preclinical and Early Symptomatic Alzheimer’s Disease: The Role of Naturalistic Driving Studies

Abstract

Research on how preclinical and early symptomatic Alzheimer’s disease (AD) impacts driving behavior is in its infancy, with several important research areas yet to be explored. This paper identifies research gaps and suggests priorities for driving studies over the next few years among those at the earliest stages of AD. These priorities include how individual differences in demographic and biomarker measures of AD pathology, as well as differences in the in-vehicle and external driving environment, affect driving behavior. Understanding these differences is important to developing future interventions to increase driving safety among those at the earliest stages of AD.

INTRODUCTION

The detrimental effects of symptomatic Alzheimer disease (AD) on driving are well known, with higher crash risks [1, 2] and greater morbidity and mortality [3]. Since the cause of most dementia conditions is underlying Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [4], much dementia and driving research focuses on how AD, specifically, effects driving behavior. Newer assessment techniques over the past few decades have seen earlier and earlier diagnosis of symptomatic AD, including at the mild cognitive impairment [5–7] (MCI)/very mild dementia stage, allowing examination of driving at the earliest onset of clinical symptoms. These studies indicate that persons with very mild dementia show a restricted driving space, making fewer trips, traveling shorter distances, and visiting fewer unique destinations; drive slower; and are less likely to drive at night, compared to cognitively normal persons [8, 9]. Longitudinal studies indicate that having very mild dementia versus cognitive normality predicts a faster time to failing a road test in the future [10].

More recently, the advent of molecular biomarkers has enabled examination of the effect of driving in preclinical AD, when AD neuropathology is present but the individual is cognitively normal [11–13]. The most commonly-used biomarkers to date reflect the presence of lesions and/or abnormal protein levels characteristic of biological AD, amyloid and tau. These biomarkers can be obtained via cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), positron emission tomography (PET) imaging using specialized radiotracers, and plasma [14–18]. Preclinical AD may begin up to 20 years before AD dementia symptoms occur [19–22]. Studies indicate that persons with preclinical AD already show a pattern of driving restriction similar to, albeit to a lesser degree, those with very early AD dementia [23, 24]. Further, persons with preclinical AD also show more errors on an on-road driving test [25] and have a more rapid time to failing a road test in the future [26, 27].

Ultimately most driving research in early AD has a long-term goal of contributing to improved safety for older drivers and others on the road. Although road tests and simulators were previously considered to be the “gold standard” in assessing driving skills, the extent to which these measures represent driving as it occurs in everyday life is unclear. Rapid advances in technology have led to the increasing use of sensors to study naturalistic driving, as people drive in their own vehicles and their own environments, where and when they choose [28–30] (see Singh and Kathura, [30] for a review of naturalistic driving work). Although naturalistic driving studies reflect actual driving behavior, they may not be as informative as other methodologies regarding all aspects of an individual’s ability to drive, particularly when visual information regarding the driving environment is not captured. For example, simulator studies may be better at assessing reaction time to threatening stimuli when faced with emergency situations. Several large, longitudinal studies of naturalistic driving behavior, such as the Strategic Highway Research Program (SHRP 2) [31] and the 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study [32, 33], have been completed or are underway. Some of these studies concentrate specifically on driving among older adults, including those who may have/develop cognitive impairment or dementia due to any cause, such as the Candrive II/Ozcandrive [34] and the American Automobile Association’s LongROAD study [35]. Sensors used in these studies provide data about where and when an individual drives on an essentially continuous basis (e.g., every 1 second). Some sensor systems include cameras which record visual and sound information from both inside and outside the vehicle [29]. Sensor data can be transmitted wirelessly from the vehicle to cell phone towers, and then to servers for storage.

Given the widespread availability of these sensor-based methods, and the rich data that they yield on an ongoing basis, this position paper primarily concentrates on recommendations for future research on naturalistic driving. Future driving research needs for those at the earliest stages of AD can broadly be categorized as study of the effects of individual differences among drivers, as well as interactions between the driver, the vehicle, and the driving environment.

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AMONG DRIVERS: AD BIOMARKERS

Research examining relationships between molecular biomarkers of AD and cognition has intensified in the previous few years. Indeed, the research framework proposed by the National Institute on Aging— Alzheimer’s Association for defining AD is based solely on the abnormality of AD biomarkers (amyloid, tau, neurodegeneration) [13], and much research has been devoted to examining how these biomarkers are associated with global cognitive performance as well as different domains of cognition, including executive functioning, memory, attention, and processing speed [17, 36, 37]. However, research examining links between biomarkers and driving behavior is only beginning.

First, there is little research to date on associations between driving behaviors in early symptomatic AD and molecular biomarkers. Examination of biomarker levels in studies among persons with early AD dementia symptoms is important to ensure that sample participants actually have AD, as opposed to some other condition that may impact cognition and driving behavior, such as medication-related effects, depression, frontotemporal dementia, or Lewy body disease. Second, research is needed to compare whether the extent to which the emerging AD-biomarker relationships in preclinical AD are similar to, and differ from, those in early symptomatic AD. And third, several models of biological AD evolution, as they relate to biomarker changes and the appearance of symptoms with time, have been proposed. These models generally suggest that decline in CSF amyloid levels is among the earliest changes, and is followed by increases in amyloid plaques, then by increased CSF tau levels, then by increased tangles in the brain, followed by neurodegeneration, and then cognitive symptoms [11–13, 38].

More research is needed to determine where to place complex functional activities like driving, which involve physical, cognitive, and behavioral components, on this AD development timeline. The earliest existing driving research using molecular AD biomarkers suggests that, within a single sample of cognitively normal older adults, driving errors are unrelated to low CSF amyloid levels alone (i.e., at the very early stages of the AD pathological process), but occur when the ratio of CSF tau to amyloid is high, indicating increased tau levels in the presence of decreased amyloid [25], and when imaging indicates that substantial fibrillar amyloid plaques are present [25]. Relatedly, high levels of imaged fibrillar plaques are associated with risky driving behaviors (self-reported traffic violations and accidents) [39]. Other cross-sectional and longitudinal research indicates that driving difficulties, defined as receiving a Marginal Pass or Fail rating on a standardized road test, are most associated with abnormality of both amyloid and tau pathology, rather than amyloid alone [26, 40]. However, naturalistic and self-reported driving research indicates that AD-associated differences in driving behavior, namely driving restriction, occur even when amyloid abnormalities alone are considered [24].

Recent work using sensors indicates that, in addition to driving restriction behaviors, the amount of “jerk” in driving style is a feature of amyloid abnormality among cognitively normal persons [41]. Jerk, i.e., sudden changes in acceleration, increases the likelihood of being involved in safety critical events such as roadway departures, rear end collisions, and sideswipes [42]. Since associations between jerk in driving style and early or preclinical AD do not appear to have been previously reported, this new work suggests that naturalistic driving methodologies may reveal novel driving variables important at early stages in the AD continuum. Taken together, this work supports the hypothesis that subtle changes in driving behavior are present at the earliest stages of preclinical AD when amyloid levels are abnormal, with more salient changes associated with road safety (e.g., failing a driving test) occurring when tau abnormalities are also present. Future research aimed at testing this hypothesis is needed.

As noted earlier, the few studies that exist show clear associations between driving behaviors and preclinical AD defined using traditional AD biomarkers: CSF and imaging measures of amyloid and tau pathology. However, newer biomarkers that reflect other AD-related pathologies are being developed and tested at a rapid pace. These include markers of neurodegeneration such as neurofilament light polypeptide (NFL) [43], neurogranin, visinin-like protein 1 (VILIP-1); markers of neuroinflammation reflected in β2-microglobulin, progranulin, and soluble triggering receptor expressed on myeloid cells 2 (sTREM2) levels; and proteins related to the degradation and removal of pathologic Aβ and pathologic tau proteins such as clusterin. As science uncovers the increasingly-important role of these additional pathophysiological mechanisms, incorporation of some of these novel biomarkers into the current research framework for defining AD are being proposed [44]. Future research should examine how these newer biomarkers relate to different driving behaviors among cognitively normal persons as well as those with early AD dementia symptoms.

One long-sought-after objective of AD research, the development of sensitive, specific, and accurate blood biomarkers has become a reality over the last few years [14, 45]. These plasma biomarkers are a welcome new research tool, as they are likely to be more accessible than CSF and imaging biomarkers which typically must be obtained at specialized research centers. They are also likely to be more acceptable to participants than the more invasive traditional biomarkers, and although currently expensive, are expected to become more affordable in the future. Since late 2020, amyloid-based plasma biomarkers have been commercially available to physicians for clinical use to help detect biological AD, and tau-based commercial blood biomarkers are under development [45–47]. However, plasma biomarkers are not currently in clinical use in most places in the world. A literature search indicates that there are no published studies examining whether, and to what extent, these plasma biomarkers are related to driving behavior. Likewise, studies are needed that compare plasma AD biomarkers with CSF and imaging measures in predicting driving behavior. This is an important and unexplored area of research.

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN DRIVING BEHAVIOR: DEMOGRAPHICS

Because past research has shown that preclinical and very mild AD dementia predict cross-sectional and longitudinal driving behavior, researchers are beginning to explore whether driving behavior can be used to identify the earliest stages of AD. Early studies indicate that statistical models made up of combinations of naturalistic driving behaviors can accurately identify which participants have preclinical AD (measured via CSF amyloid levels in cognitively normal participants) [41, 48] and distinguish older adult participants with and without AD dementia [49]. No studies have yet used driving behavior to predict the onset of incident symptomatic AD.

For purposes of identifying preclinical and early AD, the predictive value of these driving behavior models could possibly be enhanced by including variables reflecting individual differences between older drivers. For example, the addition of age in predictive models is known to significantly enhance the ability to identify biological AD captured via imaging in both molecular biomarkers [50] (e.g., CSF, plasma) and driving behavior [41] models. However, the main and interactive effects of demographic variables such as gender, race, place (e.g., rural versus urban, New York versus Kentucky) and number and type of comorbidities (e.g., diabetes, depression) with AD biomarkers on driving behavior are largely unknown.

INTERACTIONS BETWEEN DRIVER CHARACTERISTICS AND THE IN-VEHICLE ENVIRONMENT

The higher rates of crashes among older adult drivers compared to younger adults are thought to be at least partially influenced by physical and cognitive changes that accompany aging, such as vision, reaction time, and attention [51–53]. These changes are even more pronounced among persons with early symptomatic [54] and preclinical [36, 55–58] AD and may interact with the increasingly complex in-vehicle environment.

This environment includes advanced driver assistance systems (ADAS) which are abundant in modern vehicles. ADAS systems are designed to increase safety and decrease crashes by alerting the driver via sound, visual, and/or vibrotactile stimuli, and include lane keeping assist, emergency braking, and pedestrian and blind spot detection. Although ADAS systems may decrease crash risk generally across adults, there is sparse research as to how individual driver characteristics that accompany aging and early AD may influence the effectiveness of these warning systems [51]. It is possible that instead of improving safety, they may startle and distract drivers who need to devote more attentional resources to driving than others, slow driver responses, and unintentionally reduce road safety [59]. Research is needed to determine how sound type, duration, and level; and differences in visual images (e.g., placement of images, contrast, color) of ADAS systems interact with demographic characteristics and AD pathology. This may help in increasing the effectiveness of ADAS symptoms in preventing crashes. For example, Saito et al. (2021) report substantial differences in driver behaviors across older and younger age groups and driving conditions when investigating the effects of an ADAS system in approaching blind intersections. Since the type and timing of warning signals can be manipulated (although not currently a standard feature on most vehicles) a simple intervention to increase ADAS effectiveness may be to set the signal type according to the driver’s age.

In addition to ADAS systems, other characteristics of newer vehicles add to the complexity of the in-vehicle environment. The number of controls on the steering wheel and column has increased from a simple horn press and turn signals to now allow for manipulation of windshield wipers and fluid, cruise control, telephone, and radio. Smart, in-dash touch screens can also be used to perform these features and may also allow changing of the in-vehicle temperature and lighting levels as well as access to infotainment options such as audiobooks, news, and weather reports. Although in-dash screens allow a multitude of functions to be accessed easily, they also require that that the driver divide their attention, at least for a brief time, between the road and the screen. The ability to divide attention between the roadway and the in-vehicle environment declines with older adult age, particularly when a motor response (as is needed to operate a touch-screen) is required [60].

Most in-dash touch screens include a navigational system. GPS navigation may prove helpful in wayfinding and preventing getting lost, which is more likely to occur in the later stages of AD, but more research is needed as to how these systems impact driving in the preclinical and early AD/MCI stages. Existing studies suggest that GPS-based navigation may aid in finding the destination among persons with early AD/MCI, especially when auditory-only directions are used, with driving performance declining as reliance visual GPS output increases [61]. Smart phone-based GPS navigational systems are also widely used and offer similar advantages to touch-screen based systems. However, there has been great concern about the detrimental effects of distracted driving that occurs with cell phone use [62, 63]. Although much of this research has focused on cell phone use among younger adults [64], there is some evidence that, like teenagers, the driving of adults aged 65+ may be especially affected by concomitant cell phone use [65]. Simulator studies suggest that older adults with MCI have slower reaction times and a greater probability of being in an accident when distracted by cell phone use compared to those with normal cognition [66, 67]. Some research has found that conversation with a passenger impairs simulated driving among older adults with MCI and mild AD [68], although other studies have not [66]. Whether these greater driving effects due to cell phone use and passenger conversation shown in MCI occur during the preclinical AD stage is an open question.

INTERACTIONS BETWEEN DRIVER CHARACTERISTICS AND THE EXTERNAL DRIVING ENVIRONMENT

There are a few existing studies that have examined how early AD interacts with aspects of the external driving environment such as weather. Naturalistic driving data suggest that persons with questionable/very mild and mild AD are less likely to drive in inclement weather, and more likely to drive in sunny weather, compared to nondemented controls [69]. However, two studies using self-report data indicate that individuals with MCI do not differ from those with normal cognition with regard to driving in rain or “bad weather” [70, 71]. Additionally, seasonality research indicates that the highest likelihood of US crash deaths occurs June through October, months with generally good weather [72].

Research is needed to determine whether the disparate findings are due to the different presumed etiologies of the clinical groups studied (AD specifically versus MCI generally), or to the different methodologies (naturalistic driving versus self-report). Driving in other types of weather conditions in early symptomatic AD such as snow, ice, glare, fog, etc. should also be explored. Data derived from in-vehicle sensors to monitor naturalistic driving can be linked together with large weather databases to indicate the type of weather condition at the time that an individual is driving in an area and capture their driving behavior under those particular weather conditions. This would presumably generate more accurate data compared to self-report and allow for exploration of driving during different weather types. No studies could be found that looked at weather and seasonality effects on driving among persons with preclinical AD.

Much research is needed to understand how preclinical and early symptomatic AD interact with other external driving factors, such as road type (e.g., interstate versus two-lane highway), traffic density (e.g., rush hour), intersection behavior, and awareness of objects (e.g., pedestrians, other vehicles) in the driving environment. Driving behavior is known to differ from city to city as well as in rural compared to urban environments [73, 74]. Examining how the presence of preclinical and early AD changes driving behavior in these different types of geographic locations is also a fruitful area of enquiry.

CONCLUSIONS

Research on how preclinical and early symptomatic AD impact driving behavior is in its infancy, with several important research areas yet to be explored. These include how individual differences in demographic and biomarker measures of AD pathology, as well as differences in the in-vehicle and external driving environment, affect driving behavior. Understanding these differences is important to developing future interventions to increase driving safety among those at the earliest stages of AD.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

An earlier draft of this manuscript was commissioned by the National Institutes of Health/National Institutes on Aging’s Division of Behavioral and Social Research.

FUNDING

Dr. Roe’s work on this article was supported by Roe Research LLC and the National Institute of Health/National Institute on Aging (grant numbers: R01AG056466, R01AG068183, R01AG067428) and by the BrightFocus Foundation (A2021142S).

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The author has no conflict of interest to report.

REFERENCES

[1] 

Meuleners LB , Ng J , Chow K , Stevenson M ((2016) ) Motor vehicle crashes and dementia: A population-based study. J Am Geriatr Soc 64: , 1039–1045.

[2] 

Chee JN , Rapoport MJ , Molnar F , Herrmann N , O’Neill D , Marottoli R , Mitchell S , Tant M , Dow J , Ayotte D , Lanctôt KL , McFadden R , Taylor J-P , Donaghy PC , Olsen K , Classen S , Elzohairy Y , Carr DB ((2017) ) Update on the risk of motor vehicle collision or drivingimpairment with dementia: A collaborative international systematicreview and meta-analysis.. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 25: , 1376–1390.

[3] 

National Highway Traffic Administration (2011) Motor Vehicle Fatalities. https://crashstats.nhtsa.dot.gov/Api/Public/ViewPublication/811753. Accessed July 4, 2022.

[4] 

((2020) ) 2020 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement 16: , 391–460.

[5] 

Flicker C , Ferris SH , Reisberg B ((1991) ) Mild cognitive impairment in the elderly: Predictors of dementia. Neurology 41: , 1006–1009.

[6] 

Albert MS , DeKosky ST , Dickson D , Dubois B , Feldman HH , Fox NC , Gamst A , Holtzman DM , Jagust WJ , Petersen RC , Snyder PJ , Carrillo MC , Thies B , Phelps CH ((2011) ) The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 7: , 270–279.

[7] 

Petersen RC , Smith GE , Waring SC , Ivnik RJ , Tangalos EG , Kokmen E ((1999) ) Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 56: , 303–308.

[8] 

Eby DW , Silverstein NM , Molnar LJ , LeBlanc D , Adler G ((2012) ) Drivingbehaviors in early stage dementia: A study using in-vehicletechnology. Accid Anal Prev 49: , 330–337.

[9] 

Davis R , Owens M ((2021) ) Self-regulation of driving behaviors in persons with early-stage Alzheimer’s disease. J Gerontol Nurs 47: , 21–27.

[10] 

Duchek JM , Carr DB , Hunt L , Roe CM , Xiong C , Shah K , Morris JC ((2003) ) Longitudinal driving performance in early stage dementia of the Alzheimer type. J Am Geriatr Soc 51: , 1342–1347.

[11] 

Dubois B , Hampel H , Feldman HH , Scheltens P , Aisen P , Andrieu S , Bakardjian H , Benali H , Bertram L , Blennow K , Broich K , Cavedo E , Crutch S , Dartigues JF , Duyckaerts C , Epelbaum S , Frisoni GB , Gauthier S , Genthon R , Gouw AA , Habert MO , Holtzman DM , Kivipelto M , Lista S , Molinuevo JL , O’Bryant SE , Rabinovici GD , Rowe C , Salloway S , Schneider LS , Sperling R , Teichmann M , Carrillo MC , Cummings J , Jack CR ((2016) ) Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: Definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria. Alzheimers Dement 12: , 292–323.

[12] 

Sperling RA , Aisen PS , Beckett LA , Bennett DA , Craft S , Fagan AM , Iwatsubo T , Jack CR Jr , Kaye J , Montine TJ , Park DC , Reiman EM , Rowe CC , Siemers E , Stern Y , Yaffe K , Carrillo MC , Thies B , Morrison-Bogorad M , Wagster MV , Phelps CH ((2011) ) Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 7: , 280–292.

[13] 

Jack CR Jr , Bennett DA , Blennow K , Carrillo MC , Dunn B , Haeberlein SB , Holtzman DM , Jagust W , Jessen F , Karlawish J , Liu E , Molinuevo JL , Montine T , Phelps C , Rankin KP , Rowe CC , Scheltens P , Siemers E , Snyder HM , Sperling R ((2018) ) NIA-AA Research Framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 14: , 535–562.

[14] 

Blennow K , Zetterberg H ((2018) ) Biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease: Current status and prospects for the future. J Intern Med 284: , 643–663.

[15] 

Zetterberg H , Blennow K ((2021) ) Moving fluid biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease from research tools to routine clinical diagnostics. Mol Neurodegener 16: , 10.

[16] 

Guzman-Martinez L , Maccioni RB , Farías GA , Fuentes P , Navarrete LP ((2019) ) Biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res 16: , 518–528.

[17] 

Mahaman YAR , Embaye KS , Huang F , Li L , Zhu F , Wang J-Z , Liu R , Feng J , Wang X ((2022) ) Biomarkers used in Alzheimer’s disease diagnosis, treatment, and prevention. Ageing Res Rev 74: , 101544.

[18] 

Scheltens P , De Strooper B , Kivipelto M , Holstege H , Chételat G , Teunissen CE , Cummings J , van der Flier WM ((2021) ) Alzheimer’sdisease. Lancet 397: , 1577–1590.

[19] 

Bateman RJ , Xiong C , Benzinger TLS , Fagan AM , Goate A , Fox NC , Marcus DS , Cairns NJ , Xie X , Blazey TM , Holtzman DM , Santacruz A , Buckles V , Oliver A , Moulder K , Aisen PS , Ghetti B , Klunk WE , McDade E , Martins RN , Masters CL , Mayeux R , Ringman JM , Rossor MN , Schofield PR , Sperling RA , Salloway S , Morris JC ((2012) ) Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 367: , 795–804.

[20] 

Buchhave P , Minthon L , Zetterberg H , Wallin ÅK , Blennow K , Hansson O ((2012) ) Cerebrospinal fluid levels of β-amyloid 1-42, but not of tau, are fully changed already 5 to 10 years before the onset of Alzheimer dementia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69: , 98–106.

[21] 

Roe CM , Fagan AM , Grant EA , Hassenstab J , Moulder KL , Maue Dreyfus D , Sutphen CL , Benzinger TL , Mintun MA , Holtzman DM , Morris JC ((2013) ) Amyloid imaging and CSF biomarkers in predicting cognitive impairment up to 7.5 years later. Neurology 80: , 1784–91.

[22] 

Stomrud E , Minthon L , Zetterberg H , Blennow K , Hansson O ((2015) ) Longitudinal cerebrospinal fluid biomarker measurements in preclinical sporadic Alzheimer’s disease: A prospective 9-year study. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 1: , 403–411.

[23] 

Babulal GM , Stout SH , Benzinger TLS , Ott BR , Carr DB , Webb M , Traub CM , Addison A , Morris JC , Warren DK , Roe CM ((2019) ) A naturalistic study of driving behavior in older adults and preclinical Alzheimer disease: A pilot study. J Appl Gerontol 38: , 277–289.

[24] 

Roe CM , Stout SH , Rajasekar G , Ances BM , Jones JM , Head D , Benzinger TLS , Williams MM , Davis JD , Ott BR , Warren DK , Babulal GM ((2019) ) A 2.5-year longitudinal assessment of naturalistic driving in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 68: , 1625–1633.

[25] 

Roe CM , Barco PP , Head DM , Ghoshal N , Selsor N , Babulal GM , Fierberg R , Vernon EK , Shulman N , Johnson A , Fague S , Xiong C , Grant EA , Campbell A , Ott BR , Holtzman DM , Benzinger TL , Fagan AM , Carr DB , Morris JC ((2017) ) Amyloid imaging, cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers predict driving performance among cognitively normal individuals. Alzheimer Assoc Disord 31: , 69–72.

[26] 

Roe CM , Babulal GM , Stout SH , Carr DB , Williams MM , Benzinger TLS , Fagan AM , Holtzman DM , Ances BM , Morris JC ((2018) ) Using the A/T/N framework to examine driving in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Geriatrics (Basel) 3: , 23.

[27] 

Roe CM , Babulal GM , Head DM , Stout SH , Vernon EK , Ghoshal N , Garland B , Barco PP , Williams MM , Johnson A , Fierberg R , Fague MS , Xiong C , Mormino E , Grant EA , Holtzman DM , Benzinger TLS , Fagan AM , Ott BR , Carr DB , Morris JC ((2017) ) Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and longitudinal driving decline. Alzheimers Dement (N Y) 3: , 74–82.

[28] 

Al-Hussein WA , Kiah MLM , Yee PL , Zaidan BB ((2021) ) A systematic review on sensor-based driver behaviour studies: Coherent taxonomy, motivations, challenges, recommendations, substantial analysis and future directions. PeerJ Comput Sci 7: , 1–50.

[29] 

Ziakopoulos A , Tselentis D , Kontaxi A , Yannis G ((2020) ) A critical overview of driver recording tools. J Safety Res 72: , 203–212.

[30] 

Singh H , Kathuria A ((2021) ) Analyzing driver behavior under naturalistic driving conditions: A review. Accid Anal Prev 150: , 105908.

[31] 

Antin JF (2011) Design of the in-vehicle driving behavior and crash risk study: In support of the SHRP 2 naturalistic driving study, Transportation Research Board. https://onlinepubs.trb.org/onlinepubs/shrp2/shrp2pbS05.pdf. Accessed July 4, 2022.

[32] 

Dingus TA , Klauer S , Neale V , Petersen A , Lee S , Sudweeks J , Perez M , Hankey J , Ramsey D , Gupta S (2006) The 100-car naturalistic driving study, Phase II-results of the 100-car field experiment. https://www.nhtsa.gov/sites/nhtsa.gov/files/100carmain.pdf. Accessed July 4, 2022.

[33] 

Neale V , Klauer S , Knipling R , Dingus T , Holbrook G , Petersen A (2002) The 100 car naturalistic driving study, Phase I-experimental design. https://trid.trb.org/view/643552. Accessed July 4, 2022.

[34] 

Marshall SC , Man-Son-Hing M , Bédard M , Charlton J , Gagnon S , Gélinas I , Koppel S , Korner-Bitensky N , Langford J , Mazer B , Myers A , Naglie G , Polgar J , Porter MM , Rapoport M , Tuokko H , Vrkljan B , Woolnough A ((2013) ) Protocol for Candrive II/Ozcandrive, amulticentre prospective older driver cohort study. Accid AnalPrev 61: , 245–252.

[35] 

Li G , Eby DW , Santos R , Mielenz TJ , Molnar LJ , Strogatz D , Betz ME , DiGuiseppi C , Ryan LH , Jones V , Pitts SI , Hill LL , DiMaggio CJ , LeBlanc D , Andrews HF , Bogard S , Chihuri S , Engler AM , Feng M , Gessner R , Grabowski JG , Guralnik J , Hewitt B , Johnson A , Kostyniuk LP , Lang BH , Leu C , Merlel D , Nyquist LV , Parnham T , Scott K , Renée M , Ventura M , Yung R , Zanier N , Zakrajsek J ((2017) ) Longitudinal Research on Aging Drivers (LongROAD): Study design andmethods. Inj Epidemiol 4: , 22.

[36] 

Xiao Z , Wu X , Wu W , Yi J , Liang X , Ding S , Zheng L , Luo J , Gu H , Zhao Q , Xu H , Ding D ((2021) ) Plasma biomarker profiles and the correlation with cognitive function across the clinical spectrum of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther 13: , 123.

[37] 

Timmers T , Ossenkoppele R , Verfaillie SCJ , van der Weijden CWJ , Slot RER , Wesselman LMP , Windhorst AD , Wolters EE , Yaqub M , Prins ND , Lammertsma AA , Scheltens P , van der Flier WM , van BNM ((2019) ) Amyloid PET and cognitive decline in cognitively normal individuals: The SCIENCe project. Neurobiol Aging 79: , 50–58.

[38] 

Jack C , Holtzman D ((2013) ) Biomarker modeling of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuron 80: , 1347–1358.

[39] 

Ott BR , Jones RN , Noto RB , Yoo DC , Snyder PJ , Bernier JN , Carr DB , Roe CM ((2017) ) Brain amyloid in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease is associated with increased driving risk. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 6: , 136–142.

[40] 

Roe CM , Babulal GM , Mishra S , Gordon BA , Stout SH , Ott BR , Carr DB , Ances BM , Morris JC , Benzinger TLS ((2017) ) Tau and amyloid positron emission tomography imaging predict driving performance among older adults with and without preclinical Alzheimer Disease. J Alzheimers Dis 61: , 509–513.

[41] 

Bayat S , Babulal GM , Schindler SE , Fagan AM , Morris JC , Mihailidis A , Roe CM ((2021) ) GPS driving: A digital biomarker for preclinical Alzheimer disease. Alzheimers Res Ther 13: , 115.

[42] 

Khattak ZH , Fontaine MD , Li W , Khattak AJ , Karnowski T ((2021) ) Investigating the relation between instantaneous driving decisions and safety critical events in naturalistic driving environment. Accid Anal Prev 156: , 106086.

[43] 

Mattsson N , Cullen NC , Andreasson U , Zetterberg H , Blennow K ((2019) ) Association between longitudinal plasma neurofilament light and neurodegeneration in patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol 76: , 791–799.

[44] 

Hampel H , Cummings J , Blennow K , Gao P , Jack CR , Vergallo A ((2021) ) Developing the ATX(N) classification for use across the Alzheimer disease continuum. Nat Rev Neurol 17: , 580–589.

[45] 

Leuzy A , Mattsson-Carlgren N , Palmqvist S , Janelidze S , Dage JL , Hansson O ((2022) ) Blood-based biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease. EMBO Mol Med 14: , e14408.

[46] 

Alzforum (2020) Plasma AβTest Wins Approval—Are p–Tau Tests Far Behind? https://www.alzforum.org/news/research-news/plasma-av-test-wins-approval-are-p-tau-tests-far-behind.

[47] 

Precivity AD (2020) Alzheimer’s Breakthrough: C2N First to Offer a Widely Accessible Blood Test. https://precivityad.com/news/alzheimers-breakthrough.

[48] 

Babulal GM , Johnson A , Fagan AM , Morris JC , Roe CM ((2021) ) Identifying preclinical Alzheimer’s disease using everyday driving behavior: Proof of concept. J Alzheimers Dis 79: , 1009–1014.

[49] 

Di X , Shi R , Diguiseppi C , Eby DW , Hill LL , Mielenz TJ , Molnar LJ , Strogatz D , Andrews HF , Goldberg TE , Lang BH , Kim M , Li G ((2021) ) Using naturalistic driving data to predict mild cognitive impairment and dementia: Preliminary findings from the longitudinal research on aging drivers (longroad) study. Geriatrics (Basel) 6: , 45.

[50] 

Schindler SE , Bollinger JG , Ovod V , Mawuenyega KG , Li Y , Gordon BA , Holtzman DM , Morris JC , Benzinger TLS , Xiong C , Fagan AM , Bateman RJ ((2019) ) High-precision plasma β-amyloid 42/40 predicts current and future brain amyloidosis. Neurology 93: , e1647–e1659.

[51] 

Young KL , Koppel S , Charlton JL ((2017) ) Toward best practice in Human Machine Interface design for older drivers: A review of current design guidelines. Accid Anal Prev 106: , 460–467.

[52] 

Falkenstein M , Karthaus M , Brüne-Cohrs U ((2020) ) Age-related diseases and driving safety. Geriatr Switz 5: , 1–28.

[53] 

Karthaus M , Falkenstein M ((2016) ) Functional changes and driving performance in older drivers: Assessment and interventions. Geriatrics (Basel) 1: , 10.3390.

[54] 

Albers MW , Gilmore GC , Kaye J , Murphy C , Wingfield A , Bennett DA , Boxer AL , Buchman AS , Cruickshanks KJ , Devanand DP , Duffy CJ , Gall CM , Gates GA , Granholm AC , Hensch T , Holtzer R , Hyman BT , Lin FR , McKee AC , Morris JC , Petersen RC , Silbert LC , Struble RG , Trojanowski JQ , Verghese J , Wilson DA , Xu S , Zhang LI ((2015) ) At the interface of sensory and motor dysfunctions and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 11: , 70–98.

[55] 

Medina LD , Woo E , Rodriguez-Agudelo Y , Chaparro Maldonado H , Yi D , Coppola G , Zhou Y , Chui HC , Ringman JM ((2021) ) Reaction time andresponse inhibition in autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Cogn 147: , 105656.

[56] 

Wu SZ , Masurkar AV , Balcer LJ ((2020) ) Afferent and efferent visual markers of Alzheimer’s disease: A review and update in early stage disease. , 10. Front Aging Neurosci 12: , 3389.

[57] 

Aschenbrenner AJ , Gordon BA , Fagan AM , Schindler SE , Balota DA , Morris JC , Hassenstab JJ , Tales A ((2020) ) Neurofilament light predicts decline in attention but not episodic memory in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 74: , 1119–1129.

[58] 

Jacob ME , O’Donnell A , Samra J , Gonzales MM , Satizabal C , Pase MP , Murabito JM , Beiser A , Seshadri S ((2022) ) Grip strength, gait speed and plasma markers of neurodegeneration in asymptomatic middle-aged and older adults. J Frailty Aging 11: , 291–298.

[59] 

Ziefle M , Pappachan P , Jakobs E-M , Wallentowitz H (2008) Visual and auditory interfaces of advanced driver assistant systems for older drivers. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-70540-68. Accessed July 4, 2022..

[60] 

Wikman A-S , Summala H ((2005) ) Aging and time-sharing in highway driving. Optom Vis Sci 82: , 716–723.

[61] 

Yi J , Lee HC-Y , Parsons R , Falkmer T ((2015) ) The effect of the global positioning system on the driving performance of people with mild Alzheimer’s disease. Gerontology 61: , 79–88.

[62] 

Overton TL , Rives TE , Hecht C , Shafi S , Gandhi RR ((2015) ) Distracted driving: Prevalence, problems, and prevention. Int J Inj Contr Saf Promot 22: , 187–192.

[63] 

Dingus TA , Guo F , Lee S , Antin JF , Perez M , Buchanan-King M , Hankey J ((2016) ) Driver crash risk factors and prevalence evaluation using naturalistic driving data. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113: , 2636–2641.

[64] 

Delgado MK , Wanner KJ , McDonald C ((2016) ) Adolescent cellphone use while driving: An overview of the literature and promising future directions for prevention. Media Commun 4: , 79–89.

[65] 

Guo F , Klauer SG , Fang Y , Hankey JM , Antin JF , Perez MA , Lee SE , Dingus TA ((2017) ) The effects of age on crash risk associated with driver distraction. Int J Epidemiol 46: , 258–265.

[66] 

Beratis IN , Pavlou D , Papadimitriou E , Andronas N , Kontaxopoulou D , Fragkiadaki S , Yannis G , Papageorgiou SG ((2017) ) Mild cognitive impairment and driving: Does in-vehicle distraction affect driving performance? . Accid Anal Prev 103: , 148–155.

[67] 

Pavlou D , Papantoniou P , Papadimitriou E , Vardaki S , Yannis G , Antoniou C , Golias J , Papageorgiou SG ((2016) ) Which are the effects of driver distraction and brain pathologies on reaction time and accident risk? . Adv Transp Stud 1: , 83–98.

[68] 

Economou A , Pavlou D , Beratis I , Andronas N , Papadimitriou E , Papageorgiou SG , Yannis G ((2020) ) Predictors of accidents in people with mild cognitive impairment, mild dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease and healthy controls in simulated driving. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 35: , 859–869.

[69] 

Festa EK , Ott BR , Manning KJ , Davis JD , Heindel WC ((2013) ) Effect of cognitive status on self-regulatory driving behavior in older adults: An assessment of naturalistic driving using in-car video recordings. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 26: , 10–18.

[70] 

O’Connor ML , Edwards JD , Bannon Y ((2013) ) Self-rated driving habits among older adults with clinically-defined mild cognitive impairment, clinically-defined dementia, and normal cognition. Accid Anal Prev 61: , 197–202.

[71] 

Feng YR , Meuleners L , Stevenson M , Heyworth J , Murray K , Maher S ((2020) ) Driver self-regulation practices in older drivers with and without mild cognitive impairment. Clin Interv Aging 15: , 217–224.

[72] 

Weast R ((2018) ) Temporal factors in motor-vehicle crash deaths: Ten years later. J Safety Res 65: , 125–131.

[73] 

Payyanadan RP , Lee JD , Grepo LC ((2018) ) Challenges for older drivers in urban, suburban, and rural settings. Geriatr Switz 3: , 10.3390.

[74] 

Strogatz D , Mielenz TJ , Johnson AK , Baker IR , Robinson M , Mebust SP , Andrews HF , Betz ME , Eby DW , Johnson RM , Jones VC , Leu CS , Molnar LJ , Rebok GW , Li G ((2020) ) Importance of driving and potential impact of driving cessation for rural and urban older adults. J Rural Health 36: , 88–93.