Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton October 19, 2017

The perception and interpretation of contrastive focus by Polish children and adults

  • Joanna Śmiecińska EMAIL logo

Abstract

The study aimed at testing Polish children’s and adult’s ability to identify and interpret three different contrastive focus types; narrow final focus, narrow non-final focus, and broad focus. In the first experiment eight-year-old children (N = 15) and young adults (N = 15, age range 19–23) were asked to spot the focus in short utterances, and produce contrastive elements in response to three types of inputs with three distinct focus types. In the second experiment eight and four year old children (N = 14 and N = 15, respectively) and young adults (N = 16) listened to two types of utterances – with final contrastive and non-final contrastive focus and were asked to select a picture that best matched the implied meaning of the auditory input. Although in each group there were individuals with ceiling results and chance results, on average the adults outperformed the children in all the tasks. Implications of the findings for testing prosody development are discussed.


Joanna Śmiecińska Faculty of English Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznań Collegium Novum al. Niepodległości 4 61-874 Poznań Poland

References

Bauman, S., M. Grice and S. Steindamm. 2006. “Prosodic marking of focus domains – Categorical or gradient?” Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2006, Dresden, Germany. 301–304.Search in Google Scholar

Chomsky, N. and M. Halle. 1968. The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Search in Google Scholar

Crystal, D. 1979. “Prosodic development”. In: Fletcher, P. and M. Garman (eds.), Language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 33–48.Search in Google Scholar

Crystal, D. 1982. Profiling linguistic disability. London: Arnold.Search in Google Scholar

Cruttenden, A. 1974. “An experiment involving comprehension of intonation in children from 7 to 10”. Journal of Child Language 1. 221–231.10.1017/S0305000900000660Search in Google Scholar

Diehl, J.J. and R. Paul. 2009. “The assessment and treatment of prosodic disorders and neurological theories of prosody”. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 11(4). 287–292.10.1080/17549500902971887Search in Google Scholar

Fairbanks, G. and W. Pronovost. 1939. “An experimental study of the pitch characteristics of the voice during the expression of emotion”. Speech Monographs 6. 87–104.10.1080/03637753909374863Search in Google Scholar

Fairbanks, G. and L.W. Hoaglin. 1941. “An experimental study of the durational characteristics of the voice during the expression of emotion”. Speech Monographs 8. 85-90.10.1080/03637754109374888Search in Google Scholar

Gualmini, A., S. Maciukaite and S. Crain. 2003. “Children’s insensitivity to contrastive stress in sentences with only”. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 8(1), Article 8. Available at: <http://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol8/iss1/8>.Search in Google Scholar

Grassmann, S. and M. Tomasello. 2010. “Prosodic stress on a word directs 24-month-olds’ attention to a contextually new referent”. Journal of Pragmatics Journal of Pragmatics 42(11). 3098–3105.10.1016/j.pragma.2010.04.019Search in Google Scholar

Gussenhoven, C. 1983. “Testing the reality of focus domains”. Language and Speech 26. 61–80.10.1177/002383098302600104Search in Google Scholar

Liberman, P. 1960. “Some acoustic correlates of word stress in American English”. Journal of the . Acoustic Society of America 32. 451–454.10.1121/1.1908095Search in Google Scholar

Morton J. and W. Jassem. 1964. “Acoustic correlates of stress”. Language and Speech 8(3). 159–181.10.1177/002383096500800303Search in Google Scholar

PEPS-C. 2017. Profiling Elements of Prosody in Speech-Communication. <http://www.peps-c.com/>. Last accessed 06 Feb 2017.Search in Google Scholar

Peppé, S., J. Maxim and B. Wells. 2000. “Prosodic variation in Southern British English”. Language and Speech 43. 309–334.10.1177/00238309000430030501Search in Google Scholar

Peppé, S. and J. McCann. 2003. “Profiling elements of prosody in speech-communication (PEPS-C)”. Available at: <http://www.peps-c.com/index.html>. Last accessed 08 Sep 2015.Search in Google Scholar

Peppé, S. 2009. “Why is prosody in speech-language pathology so difficult?” International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 11(4). 258–271.10.1080/17549500902906339Search in Google Scholar

Peppé, S. 2017. “Prosody and Autism Spectrum Disorder”.Available at <http://www.qmu.ac.uk/casl/ProsodyinASD/PEPS-C.htm>. Last accessed 16 Feb 2017.Search in Google Scholar

Rump, H.H. and R. Collier. 1996. “Focus conditions and the prominence of pitch-accented syllables”. Language and Speech 39. 1–17.10.1177/002383099603900101Search in Google Scholar

Selkirk, E. 1984. Phonology and syntax. The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Schirmer, A. and S.A. Kotz. 2006. “Beyond the right hemisphere: Brain mechanisms mediating vocal emotional processing”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(1). 24–30.10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.009Search in Google Scholar

Wells, B., S. Peppé and N. Goulandris. 2004. “Intonation development from five to thirteen”. Journal of Child Language 31(4). 749–778.10.1017/S030500090400652XSearch in Google Scholar

Wysocka, M. 2012. Prozodia mowy w percepcji dzieci [Speech prosody in children’s perception]. Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Marii Curie-Skłodowskiej.Search in Google Scholar

Appendix A

Mean results for Experiment 1 – “Provide focus” (focus), and “Spot the focus”, (focus stronger).

Gr. 1: Gr. 2:children adultsNMeanSDSE95% conf. for meanMin.Max.
Lower Lim.Upper Lim.
Broad focus1.150.801.370.360.041.5605
2.154.001.890.492.955.0517
Total302.402.300.421.543.2607
% broad focus11510.0017.174.430.4919.51.062.5
21550.0023.626.1036.9263.0812.587.5
Total3030.0028.735.2519.2740.73.087.5
Final focus1150.871.360.350.121.6204
2155.272.340.613.976.5618
Total303.072.920.531.974.1608
% final focus11510.8316.954.381.4520.22.050.0
21565.8329.307.5749.6182.0612.5100.0
Total3038.3336.546.6724.6951.98.0100.0
Non-final focus1157.530.640.177.187.8968
2157.800.560.157.498.1168
Total307.670.610.117.447.8968
% non-final focus11594.178.002.0789.7498.6075.0100.0
21597.507.011.8193.62101.3875.0100.0
Total3095.837.581.3893.0098.6675.0100.0
All types raw scores1153.070.670.172.703.442.34.3
2155.691.250.325.006.384.07.7
Total304.381.660.303.765.002.37.7
% all types11538.338.362.1633.7042.9729.254.2
21571.1115.634.0462.4679.7750.095.8
Total3054.7220.723.7846.9862.4629.295.8
Broad focus stronger1154.732.400.623.406.071.08.0
2156.200.860.225.726.685.07.0
Total305.471.930.354.756.191.08.0
% broad focus stronger11559.1730.057.7642.5275.8112.5100.0
21577.5010.772.7871.5383.4762.587.5
Total3068.3324.064.3959.3577.3212.5100.0
Final focus stronger1157.470.520.137.187.757.08.0
2157.800.560.147.498.116.08.0
Total307.630.560.107.437.846.08.0
% final focus stronger11593.336.461.6789.7696.9187.5100.0
21597.507.011.8193.62101.3875.0100.0
Total3095.426.951.2792.8298.0175.0100.0
Non-final focus stronger1154.071.870.483.035.101.08.0
2157.800.410.117.578.037.08.0
Total305.932.320.425.076.801.08.0
% non-final focus stronger11550.8323.376.0337.8963.7812.5100.0
21597.505.181.3494.63100.3787.5100.0
Total3074.1728.985.2963.3584.9912.5100.0
All types stronger11516.273.750.9714.1918.3410.023.0
21521.801.080.2821.2022.4019.023.0
Total3019.033.910.7117.5720.4910.023.0
% all focus stronger11567.7815.634.0359.1276.4341.6795.83
21590.834.511.1688.3493.3379.1795.83
Total3079.3116.282.9773.2285.3941.6795.83

Appendix B

An example of a “broadly contrastive” vs. “narrowly contrastive non-final” visual stimulus for audio file (6) – ‘But I wanted a pink cat’. The choice is between the yellow cat (the correct answer), which narrowly contrasts with the adjective ‘pink’, and a brown dog, which contrasts broadly with the whole noun phrase – ‘a pink cat’. (Colour online.)

An example of “broadly contrastive” vs “narrowly contrastive final” visual stimulus for audio file (7) – ‘But I wanted a pink cat’. The choice is between the final narrow focus reading – a pink bunny (the correct answer) and a broad focus reading – a brown dog. (Colour online.)

Appendix 3

Mean results in age groups in a comprehensive pilot study on Polish children’s prosody skills (Śmiecińska under review). All the figures represent percentage values. The Focus task is boldfaced. There were no significant differences between the age groups as far as the Focus results are concerned.

Age groupNLike MeanQuestion MeanComma MeanFocus MeanSentence MeanNonword MeanUpdown MeanSum of prosody Mean
1. 3;6–4;92098.6039.3551.2571.8883.0079.2052.0067.90
2. 5;0–5;92498.8358.9264.0678.6588.6579.1358.3375.22
3. 6;0–7;922100.0073.4165.3966.4594.7094.3666.3680.10
4. 8;1–10;920100.00100.0072.5078.7399.5099.1592.0091.70
5. 20–3026100.00100.0078.3780.29100.00100.0097.6993.76
Total1199.5075.1466.8675.4493.4090.5573.9382.12
2

Published Online: 2017-10-19
Published in Print: 2017-10-26

© 2017 Faculty of English, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland

Downloaded on 23.4.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/psicl-2017-0018/html
Scroll to top button