Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton January 20, 2022

Cross-generational differences in linguistic and cognitive spatial frames of reference in Negev Arabic

  • Letizia Cerqueglini ORCID logo EMAIL logo
From the journal Linguistics Vanguard

Abstract

This paper analyses the impact of landscape on linguistic and cognitive spatial frames of reference (FoRs) in three generations of speakers of Negev Arabic (NA): Traditional (TNA), Middle (MNA), and Young (YNA). When locating one object (the figure) with respect to a reference object (the ground), TNA strategies rely on pragmatic properties attributed to the ground (e.g. familiarity) and to axial constraints (alignment between figure, ground, and the observer’s visual field). When the ground is familiar and asymmetrical, intrinsic FoR is preferred; when it is familiar and symmetrical, a Speech Act Participant (SAP)-landmark strategy is preferred if figure, ground, and observer are aligned, otherwise absolute FoR is used; when the ground is unfamiliar, absolute FoR is preferred. Absolute FoR is not used in MNA, while in egocentric reference, a relative reflection strategy is used. MNA applies right–left distinctions using reflection and rotation strategies. YNA uses intrinsic FoR and relative reflection without predicable rules; absolute FoR and SAP-landmark FoR are absent; and the right–left axis applies to all ground objects. All generations apply geocentric FoRs in non-linguistic tasks within the Negev region, but outside this region, each used different strategies. Negev landscapes are fundamental constituents of NA FoRs across generations.


Corresponding author: Letizia Cerqueglini, Hebrew Language and Semitic Linguistics, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel, E-mail:

Funding source: Israel Science Foundation http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100003977

Award Identifier / Grant number: Grant no. 680/17

Acknowledgments

My sincere thanks go to the anonymous reviewers and to the editors of this volume for their valuable suggestions, and in particular to Prof. Bill Palmer, for his sustained guidance and support.

  1. Research funding: This research was financially supported by Israel Science Foundation Grant no. 680/17.

References

Blanc, Haim. 1970. The Arabic dialect of the Negev Bedouins. Jerusalem: IASH.Search in Google Scholar

Bohnemeyer, Jürgen. 2011. Spatial FoRs in Yucatec: Referential promiscuity and task specificity. Language Sciences 33. 892–914. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2011.06.009.Search in Google Scholar

Bohnemeyer, Jürgen. 2012. A vector space semantics for reference frames in Yucatec. In Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten (ed.), Proceedings of the sixth meeting on the semantics of under-represented languages in the Americas (SULA 6) and SULA-Bar, 15–34. Amherst: GLSA Publications.Search in Google Scholar

Bohnemeyer, Jürgen & Carolyn O’Meara. 2012. Vectors and frames of reference: Evidence from Seri and Yucatec. In Luna Filipović & Kasia Jaszczolt (eds.), Space and time across languages and cultures, 217–249. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.37.16bohSearch in Google Scholar

Brown, Penelope. 2006. Language, culture and cognition: The view from space. Zeitschrift für Germanistische Linguistik 34. 64–86. https://doi.org/10.1515/zgl.2006.005.Search in Google Scholar

Brown, Penelope & Stephen Levinson. 1993. “Uphill” and “downhill” in Tzeltal. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 3. 46–74. https://doi.org/10.1525/jlin.1993.3.1.46.Search in Google Scholar

Burenhult, Niclas & Stephen Levinson. 2008. Language and landscape: A cross-linguistic perspective. Language Sciences 30(2/3). 135–150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2006.12.028.Search in Google Scholar

Calderón, Eréndira, Stefano De Pascale & Evangelia Adamou. 2019. How to speak “geocentric” in an “egocentric” language: A multimodal study among Ngigua-Spanish bilinguals and Spanish monolinguals in a rural community of Mexico. Language Sciences 74. 24–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2019.04.001.Search in Google Scholar

Cerqueglini, Letizia. 2015. Object-based selection of spatial frames of reference in aṣ-Ṣāniʿ Arabic. Pisa: Pisa University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Cerqueglini, Letizia. 2019. Spatial frames of reference in Traditional aṣ-Ṣāniʕ Arabic: Preliminary observations of language-to-cognition correlation. Saggi e Studi Linguistici 57(1). 71–127.Search in Google Scholar

Cerqueglini, Letizia & Roni Henkin. 2016. Spatial language and culture: Cardinal directions in Negev Arabic. Anthropological Linguistics 58(2). 171–208. https://doi.org/10.1353/anl.2016.0026.Search in Google Scholar

Cerqueglini, Letizia & Roni Henkin. 2018. Referential complementarity in Traditional Negev Arabic. Journal of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 10. 83–114. https://doi.org/10.1163/18766633-00901002.Search in Google Scholar

Dasen, Pierre & Ramesh Mishra. 2010. Development of geocentric spatial language and cognition: An eco-cultural perspective (Cambridge Studies in Cognitive and Perceptual Development). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511761058Search in Google Scholar

Dunn, Vivien, Felicity Meakins & Cassandra Algy. 2021. Acquisition or shift: Interpreting variation in Gurindji children’s expression of spatial relations. In Enoch Aboh, Michael DeGraff & Cécile Vigouroux (eds.), Variation rolls the dice, 105–132. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/coll.59.05dunSearch in Google Scholar

Henkin, Roni. 2010. Negev Arabic: Dialectal, sociolinguistic, and stylistic variation. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.Search in Google Scholar

Johnson, Leslie & Eugene Hunn. 2010. Landscape ethnoecology: Concepts of biotic and physical space (Studies in Environmental Anthropology and Ethnobiology 14). New York: Berghahn Books.Search in Google Scholar

Levinson, Stephen. 1996. Language and space. Annual Review of Anthropology 25. 353–382. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.25.1.353.Search in Google Scholar

Levinson, Stephen. 2003. Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511613609Search in Google Scholar

Levinson, Stephen, Penelope Brown, Eve Danziger, Lourdes de León, John Haviland, Eric Pederson & Gunter Senft. 1992. Man and tree & space games. In Stephen Levinson (ed.), Space stimuli kit 1.2: November 1992, 7–14. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Li, Peggy & Lila Gleitman. 2002. Turning the tables: Language and spatial reasoning. Cognition 83(3). 265–294. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00009-4.Search in Google Scholar

Majid, Asifa, Melissa Bowerman, Sotaro Kita, Daniel Haun & Stephen Levinson. 2004. Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(3). 108–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.003.Search in Google Scholar

Mark, David, Barry Smith & Barbara Tversky. 1999. Ontology and geographic objects: An empirical study of cognitive categorization. In Christian Freksa & David Mark (eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (Lecture Notes in Computer Science No. 1661), 283–298. Berlin: Springer.10.1007/3-540-48384-5_19Search in Google Scholar

Meakins, Felicity. 2011. Spaced out: Inter-generational changes in the expression of spatial relations by Gurindji people. Australian Journal of Linguistics 31(1). 43–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/07268602.2011.532857.Search in Google Scholar

Meakins, Felicity & Cassandra Algy. 2016. Deadly reckoning: Changes in Gurindji children’s knowledge of cardinals. Australian Journal of Linguistics 36(4). 479–501. https://doi.org/10.1080/07268602.2016.1169973.Search in Google Scholar

Mishra, Ramesh, Pierre Dasen & Shanta Niraula. 2003. Ecology, language, and performance on spatial cognitive tasks. International Journal of Psychology 38. 366–383. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000187.Search in Google Scholar

Moore, Randi & Jürgen Bohnemeyer. 2016. The effect of topography on language and cognition in Isthmus Zapotec. Paper presented at the international conference “Geographic Grounding: Place, Direction and Landscape in the Grammars of the World”, University of Copenhagen.Search in Google Scholar

O’Meara, Carolyn. 2010. Seri landscape classification and spatial reference. Buffalo: University at Buffalo PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Palmer, Bill. 2015. Topography in language: Absolute frame of reference and the Topographic Correspondence Hypothesis. In Rick de Busser & Randy LaPolla (eds.), Language structure and environment: Social, cultural, and natural factors, 177–226. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/clscc.6.08palSearch in Google Scholar

Palmer, Bill, Alice Gaby, Jonathon Lum & Jonathan Schlossberg. 2018. Diversity in spatial language within communities: The interplay of culture, language and landscape in representations of space. In Stephan Winter, Monika Sester & Amy L. Griffin (eds.), 10th International conference on geographic information science, 53:1–53:8. Wadern: Schloss Dagstuhl.Search in Google Scholar

Palmer, Bill, Dorothea Hoffmann, Joe Blythe, Alice Gaby, Bill Pascoe & Maïa Ponsonnet. 2021. Frames of spatial reference in five Australian languages. Spatial Cognition and Computation 1–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2021.1929239.Search in Google Scholar

Palmer, Bill, Jonathon Lum, Jonathan Schlossberg & Alice Gaby. 2017. How does the environment shape spatial language? Evidence for sociotopography. Linguistic Typology 21(3). 457–491. https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2017-0011.Search in Google Scholar

Pederson, Eric. 1993. Geographic and manipulable space in two Tamil linguistic systems. In Andrew Frank & Irene Campari (eds.), Spatial information theory, 294–311. Berlin: Springer.10.1007/3-540-57207-4_20Search in Google Scholar

Pederson, Eric, Eve Danziger, David Wilkins, Stephen Levinson, Sotaro Kita & Gunter Senft. 1998. Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization. Language 74. 557–589. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1998.0074.Search in Google Scholar

Polian, Gilles & Jürgen Bohnemeyer. 2011. Uniformity and variation in Tseltal reference frame use. Language Sciences 33. 868–891. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2011.06.010.Search in Google Scholar

Schlossberg, Jonathan. 2018. Atolls, islands and endless suburbia: Space and landscape in Marshallese. Available at: https://www.academia.edu/37316327/Atolls_Islands_and_Endless_Suburbia_space_and_landscape_in_Marshallese_talk.Search in Google Scholar

Smith, Barry & David Mark. 2003. Do mountains exist? Towards an ontology of landforms. Environment and Planning B 30(3). 411–427. https://doi.org/10.1068/b12821.Search in Google Scholar

Talmy, Leonard. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/6847.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Wassmann, Jurg & Pierre Dasen. 1998. Balinese spatial orientation: Some empirical evidence of moderate linguistic relativity. The Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 4. 689–711. https://doi.org/10.2307/3034828.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2020-01-30
Accepted: 2021-05-10
Published Online: 2022-01-20

© 2021 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 12.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/lingvan-2020-0006/html
Scroll to top button