Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton July 7, 2020

Learning formulaic creativity: Chunking in verbal art and speech

  • Cristóbal Pagán Cánovas

    Cristóbal Pagán Cánovas is a Ramón y Cajal Assistant Professor at the English Department of the University of Murcia, an Alexander von Humboldt Fellow in Quantitative Linguistics at the University of Tübingen, and a member of the International Distributed Little Red Hen Lab. He works on cognition and poetics, conceptual integration, embodiment, time across language and the arts, oral poetry, and multimodal communication.

    EMAIL logo
From the journal Cognitive Semiotics

Abstract

The chunking problem is central to linguistics, semiotics, and poetics: How do we learn to organize a language into patterns and to use those patterns creatively? Linguistics has mainly offered two answers, one based on rule inference through innate capacities for processing and the other based on usage and on outstanding capacities for memory and retrieval. Both views are based on induction and compositionality. The Parry–Lord theory of oral composition-in-performance has argued that oral singers produce complex poems out of rehearsed improvisation through the mastery of a system of formulas, chunks that integrate phrasal, metrical, and semantic structures. The framework of formulaic creativity proposed here argues that the cognitive study of oral poetics can provide crucial insights into the chunking problem. I show the major connections between Parry–Lord and usage-based cognitive linguistics, mainly Construction Grammar and Frame Semantics. However, these approaches still remain compositional and thus struggle to model creativity and learning in oral poetry and everyday speech. The alternative is to explore a model of formulaic creativity not based on compositional patterns, but on wide learning for connecting discriminative perceptual features directly to semantic contrasts within a complex dynamic system, without the intermediation of a set of discrete units.


Corresponding author: Cristóbal Pagán Cánovas, Universidad de Murcia, Murcia, Spain, E-mail: .

Special Issue: Construction Grammar and Creativity edited by Thomas Hoffmann


Funding source: Alexander von Humboldt Foundation Fellowship for Advanced Researchers

Funding source: Ramón y Cajal grant

Award Identifier / Grant number: RYC-2016-20872

About the author

Cristóbal Pagán Cánovas

Cristóbal Pagán Cánovas is a Ramón y Cajal Assistant Professor at the English Department of the University of Murcia, an Alexander von Humboldt Fellow in Quantitative Linguistics at the University of Tübingen, and a member of the International Distributed Little Red Hen Lab. He works on cognition and poetics, conceptual integration, embodiment, time across language and the arts, oral poetry, and multimodal communication.

References

Arnold, Denis, Fabian Tomaschek, Konstantin Sering, Florence Lopez & R. Harald Baayen. 2017. Words from spontaneous conversational speech can be recognized with human-like accuracy by an error-driven learning algorithm that discriminates between meanings straight from smart acoustic features, bypassing the phoneme as recognition unit. PloS One 12(4). e0174623. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174623.Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, R. Harald & Michael Ramscar. 2015. Abstraction, storage, and naive discriminative learning. In Dąbrowska, Ewa & Dagmar Divjak (eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics, 100–119. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110292022-006Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, R. Harald, Cyrus Shaoul, Jon Willits & Michael Ramscar. 2016. Comprehension without segmentation: A proof of concept with naive discriminative learning. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience 31(1). 106–128. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2015.1065336.Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, R. Harald, Yu-Ying Chuang, Elnaz Shafaei-Bajestan & James P. Blevins. 2019. The discriminative lexicon: A unified computational model for the lexicon and lexical processing in comprehension and production grounded not in (de)composition but in linear discriminative learning. Complexity 2019. 1–39.10.1155/2019/4895891Search in Google Scholar

Bakker, Egbert J. 1997. Poetry in speech: Orality and homeric discourse. Ithaka: Cornell University Press.10.7591/9781501722776Search in Google Scholar

Bakker, Egbert J. 2006. Pointing at the past: From formula to performance in homeric poetics. Washington, D.C.: Center for Hellenic Studies.Search in Google Scholar

Blevins, James P. 2016. Word and paradigm morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199593545.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Boden, Margaret A. 2009. Computer models of creativity. AI Magazine 30(3). 23–34. https://doi.org/10.1609/aimag.v30i3.2254.Search in Google Scholar

Bonifazi, Anna & David F. Elmer. 2012a. Composing lines, performing acts: Clauses, discourse acts, and melodic units in a South Slavic epic song. In Minchin, Elizabeth (ed.), Orality, literacy, and performance in the ancient world, 89–109. Leiden: Brill.10.1163/9789004217751_005Search in Google Scholar

Bonifazi, Anna & David F. Elmer. 2012b. The meaning of melody: Remarks on the performance-based analysis of Bosniac epic song. In Harris, Joseph and Barbara Hillers (eds.), Child's children: Ballad study and its legacies, 293–309. Trier: Wissenschaftlicher Verlag Trier.Search in Google Scholar

Bonifazi, Anna. 2008. Memory and visualization in homeric discourse markers. In Mackay, Anne (ed.), Orality, literacy, memory in the ancient Greek and Roman world, 35–64. Leiden and Boston: Brill.10.1163/ej.9789004169913.i-284.15Search in Google Scholar

Bonifazi, Anna. 2012. Homer's versicolored fabric: The evocative power of ancient Greek epic word making. Washington, D.C.: Center for Hellenic Studies.Search in Google Scholar

Bozzone, Chiara 2014a. Constructions: A new approach to formularity, discourse, and syntax in homer. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6kg0q4cx.pdf (accessed 15 November 2015).Search in Google Scholar

Bozzone, Chiara. 2014b. New perspectives on formularity. In Proceedings of the 21st annual UCLA Indo-European conference, Bremen, 27–44. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6kg0q4cx.pdf. (accessed 15 November 2015).Search in Google Scholar

Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: The MIT Press.10.21236/AD0616323Search in Google Scholar

Coulson, Seana. 2001. Semantic leaps: Frame-shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511551352Search in Google Scholar

Dabrowska, Ewa. 2014. Recycling utterances: A speaker's guide to sentence processing. Cognitive Linguistics 25(4). 617–653. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2014-0057.Search in Google Scholar

Edwards, Mark W. 1988. Homer and oral tradition: The formula, part II. Oral Tradition 3. 11–60. https://doi.org/10.1353/ort.2004.0011.Search in Google Scholar

Fauconnier, Gilles & Mark Turner. 2002. The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind's hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.Search in Google Scholar

Fillmore, Charles J. & Beryl T. Atkins. 1992. Towards a frame-based lexicon: The case of RISK. In Lehrer Adrienne & Eva Feder Kittay (eds.), Frames, fields, and contrasts: New essays in semantic and lexical organization, 75–102. New York: Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Fillmore, Charles J., P. Kay & M. C. O'Connor. 1988. Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let alone. Language 64(3). 501–538. https://doi.org/10.2307/414531.Search in Google Scholar

Fillmore, Charles J. 1982. Frame semantics. In Linguistic Society of Korea (ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm, 111–137. Seoul: Hanshin Publishing Company.10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/00424-7Search in Google Scholar

Foley, John M. 1986. Tradition and the collective talent: Oral epic, textual meaning, and receptionalist theory. Cultural Anthropology 1(2). 203–222. https://doi.org/10.1525/can.1986.1.2.02a00060.Search in Google Scholar

Foley, John M. 1991. Immanent art: From structure to meaning in traditional oral epic. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Foley, John M. 2002. How to read an oral poem. Champaign: University of Illinois Press.Search in Google Scholar

Foley, John M. 2004. Comparative oral traditions. In Euskal Herriko Bertsozale Elkarte (ed.), Ahozko inprobisazioa munduan, 19–38. San Sebastian: Euskal Herriko Bertsozale Elkartea.Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, Adele E. 2006. Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199268511.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Hannun, Awni, Carl Case, Jared Casper, Bryan Catanzaro, Greg Diamos, Erich Elsen, Ryan Prenger, Sanjeev Satheesh, Shubho Sengupta, Adam Coates & Andrew Y. Ng. 2014. Deep speech: Scaling up end-to-end speech recognition. https://arxiv.org/abs/1412.5567.Search in Google Scholar

Harris, Randy A. 1995. The linguistics wars. New York: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Hoffmann, Thomas & Graeme Trousdale (eds.). 2013. The Oxford handbook of construction grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396683.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Hoffmann, Thomas. 2020. Construction grammar and creativity: Evolution, psychology and cognitive science. Cognitive Semiotics 13(1). https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem-2020-2018.Search in Google Scholar

Jackendoff, Ray. 1992. Semantic structures. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Kendall, Elisabeth. 2016. Jihadist propaganda and its exploitation of the Arab poetic tradition. In Kendall, Elisabeth (ed.), Reclaiming Islamic tradition: Modern interpretations of the classical heritage, 223–246. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.10.1515/9781474403122-012Search in Google Scholar

Lakoff, George. 1987. Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 2008. Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195331967.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 2009. Investigations in cognitive grammar. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110214369Search in Google Scholar

Lord, Albert Bates. 1960. The singer of tales. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Lord, Albert Bates. 1991. Epic singers and oral tradition. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.10.7591/9781501731921Search in Google Scholar

Lord, Albert Bates. 1995. The singer resumes the tale. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Minchin, Elizabeth. 2001. Homer and the resources of memory: Some applications of cognitive theory to the Iliad and the Odyssey. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Nagler, Michael N. 1967. Towards a generative view of the oral formula. Transactions and Proceedings of the American Philological Association 98. 269–311. https://doi.org/10.2307/2935878.Search in Google Scholar

Nikiforidou, Kiki. 2010. Viewpoint and construction grammar: The case of past + now. Language and Literature 19(3). 265–284. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963947010370253.Search in Google Scholar

Ong, Walter J. 1982. Orality and literacy. London and New York: Routledge.10.4324/9780203328064Search in Google Scholar

Pagán Cánovas, C, & Mark T. 2016. Generic integration templates for fictive communication. In Pascual, Esther and Sergeiy Sandler (eds.), The conversation frame: Forms and functions of fictive interaction, 45–62. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.55.03pagSearch in Google Scholar

Pagán Cánovas, C, & M Antović. 2016a. Construction grammar and oral formulaic theory. In Pagán Cánovas, C, & M Antović. (eds.), Oral poetics and cognitive science, 79–98. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110348538-006Search in Google Scholar

Pagán Cánovas, C, & M Antović. 2016b. Introduction: Oral poetics and cognitive science. In Pagán Cánovas, C, & M Antović (eds.), Oral Poetics and Cognitive Science, 1–11. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110348538-002Search in Google Scholar

Pagán Cánovas, C, & M Antović. 2016c. Formulaic creativity: Oral poetics and cognitive grammar. Language and Communication 47. 66–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2015.12.001.Search in Google Scholar

Parry, Milman. 1930/1971. Making of homeric verse: The collected papers of Milman Parry. Adam Parry (ed.), Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Propp, Vladimir. 1928. Morphology of the folktale, 2nd edn. Austin: University of Texas Press.Search in Google Scholar

Ramscar, Michael & Robert F. Port. 2016. How spoken languages work in the absence of an inventory of discrete units. Language Sciences 53. 58–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2015.08.002.Search in Google Scholar

Ramscar, Michael, Ching Chu Sun, Peter Hendrix & Harald Baayen. 2017. The mismeasurement of mind: Life-span changes in paired-associate-learning scores reflect the ‘cost’ of learning, not cognitive decline. Psychological Science 28(8). 1171–1179. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617706393.Search in Google Scholar

Saussure, Ferdinand de. 1916. Cours de linguistique générale. Paris: Payot.Search in Google Scholar

Schank, Roger C. & Robert P. Abelson. 1977. Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Search in Google Scholar

Steen, Francis & Mark Turner. 2013. Multimodal construction grammar. In Borkent, Michael, Barbara Dancygier & Jennifer Hinnell (eds.), Language and the creative mind. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Search in Google Scholar

Tomasello, Michael. 2005. Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.10.2307/j.ctv26070v8Search in Google Scholar

Trousdale, Graeme. 2020. Creativity, reuse, and regularity in music and language. Cognitive Semiotics 13(1). https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem-2020-2021.Search in Google Scholar

Turin, Mark, Claire Wheeler & Eleanor Wilkinson (eds.). 2013. Oral literature in the digital age: Archiving orality and connecting with communities. Cambridge: Open Book Publishers.10.2307/j.ctt5vjtkqSearch in Google Scholar

Turner, Mark. 2001. Cognitive dimensions of social science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Turner, Mark. 2015. Blending in language and communication. In Dabrowska, Eva & Dagmar Divjak (eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110292022-011Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2020-07-07

© 2020 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 1.6.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/cogsem-2020-2023/html
Scroll to top button