Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Keeping distance or getting closer: How others’ emotions shape approach-avoidance postural behaviors and preferred interpersonal distance

  • Angélique Lebert,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliations Université Paris Cité, Vision Action Cognition, Boulogne-Billancourt, France, Department of Psychiatry and Forensic Medicine, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Cerdanyola del Valles, Barcelona, Spain

  • Dorine Vergilino-Perez,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Université Paris Cité, Vision Action Cognition, Boulogne-Billancourt, France

  • Laurence Chaby

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft

    laurence.chaby@u-paris.fr

    Affiliations Sorbonne Université, CNRS, Institut des Systèmes Intelligents et de Robotique (ISIR), Paris, France, Université Paris Cité, Boulogne-Billancourt, France

Abstract

Understanding the influence of emotions on social interactions is important for a global understanding of the dynamics of human behavior. In this study, we investigated the interplay between emotions, spontaneous approach or avoidance tendencies, and the regulation of interpersonal distance. Fifty-seven healthy adults participated in a three-part experiment involving exposure to approaching or withdrawing emotional faces (neutral, happy, sad, fearful, disgusted, angry). The sequence began with an initial computerized stop-distance task, followed by a postural task in which participants’ approach or avoidance tendencies were quantified via center of pressure (CoP-Y) displacements on a force platform, and concluded with a final computerized stop-distance task. Our findings revealed a gradient in postural responses, with the most forward CoP-Y displacements for neutral and happy faces, indicative of approach tendencies. These were followed by lesser forward displacements for sad and fearful faces, and most pronounced backward displacements for disgusted and angry faces, indicating avoidance. Furthermore, we observed modulations in participants’ preferred interpersonal distance based on emotional cues, with neutral and happy faces associated with shorter distances, and disgusted and angry faces linked to larger distances. Despite these similar results, no direct correlation was found between CoP-Y and preferred interpersonal distance, underscoring a dissociation between spontaneous and voluntary social behaviors. These results contribute to a better understanding of how emotional expressions shape social interactions and underscore the importance of considering emotional cues, postural action tendencies, and interpersonal distance in facilitating successful social interactions.

Introduction

In human social interactions, the body and the space immediately surrounding it are of utmost importance [1]. Personal space is defined as the area around the body where intrusion can cause feelings of discomfort [2, 3] or even threats [4], especially when the distance to others is approximately 100 cm and below [5]. This potential discomfort triggers physiological arousal [69] and may result in bodily behaviors that evoke fight, flight, or immobilization responses [10], leading individuals to regulate their social distance from others [11]. However, it remains unclear whether this social distance for interacting with others differs from more spontaneous bodily behaviors, such as approach or avoidance tendencies.

Interpersonal distance can be considered as a nonverbal form of communication [12] which can be dynamically regulated depending on whether an encounter represents a threat or an opportunity for social engagement. Such regulations rely mainly on nonverbal emotional signals from others [13], including gaze [6, 14, 15] and facial expressions [1619]. This is primarily due to the adaptive [20, 21] and motivational [22, 23] values of emotions, shaping individuals’ relationships with the world and enabling action. Traditionally, the motivational theory of emotions has suggested that emotional stimuli automatically elicit behavioral reactions—positive stimuli leading to approach behaviors and negative initiating avoidance [23, 24]. However, this straightforward relationship is increasingly being questioned, and it is still unclear whether anger and/or fear prime an approach or avoidance behavior, challenging the traditional framework [2527]. Furthermore, recent studies indicate that the impact of emotions on behavior may be contingent on the relevance of the task to the participants’ goals [2830], or on individual characteristics such as personality traits [31, 32]). Finally, it has been shown that emotions perceived in others can also influence behavior based on the potential for future interactions. This can manifest in affiliative tendencies, like the desire to approach someone pleasant [33]. However, such responses are not limited to positive emotions. Contrary to intuitive assumptions, expressions of fear or sadness can also elicit prosocial behaviors and be interpreted as cues for social engagement and connection. It has been illustrated that fear, while potentially indicating a threat, can function as an affiliative stimulus leading to approach behaviors like helping [34]. Similarly, it has been highlighted that perceptions of emotional distress can trigger supportive actions like comforting someone who seems sad [35].

The assessment of preferred interpersonal distance is a crucial component of the study of social behavior. In laboratory settings, tasks have been developed to allow participants to adjust their distance from others based on their level of comfort. One commonly used paradigm is the ‘stop-distance’ task, which involves stopping an approaching partner at the point where they still feel comfortable with the other’s proximity [3638]. Other measures have also been employed to assess preferred interpersonal distance, including paper-and-pencil methods [3941] and computer-based or virtual reality tasks, in which participants had to voluntarily adjust their preferred distance in response to stimuli representing a virtual partner [5, 4244]. However, computer-based or virtual reality tasks offer the possibility of manipulating partner characteristics such as age, gender or facial expression, while offering better experimental control and replicability [45]. This makes them valuable tools for investigating interpersonal distance adjustments in diverse situations.

In social contexts, facial expressions serve as social cues [46, 47], providing valuable information about the emotional states of others, and facilitating interpersonal communication by adjusting and coordinating with them [48]. Studies have shown that emotional facial expressions can influence both estimation and adjustment of interpersonal distance, with emotional expressions judged closer than neutral expressions [17]. Positive and negative facial expressions have also been found to affect judgments of proximity, with friendly faces or faces with happy or neutral expressions leading to shorter preferred interpersonal distances, and negative expressions, such as anger leading to larger distances [18, 19, 44]. Note that most of these studies only explored a limited range of emotions, usually two or three, and have frequently overlooked basic emotions such as disgust, which can be perceived as a threat, particularly in situations involving contamination or illness [49]. Interestingly, in the era of COVID-19, recent studies have examined how wearing face masks —by reducing the probability of contamination and enhancing trustworthiness— can offer a more nuanced perspective on our understanding of interpersonal distance regulation [50, 51]. In this context, Scerrati et al. (2022) [52] highlighted the influence of the pandemic on individuals’ assessment of social proximity.

Although individuals generally feel uneasy in close proximity to strangers, they are more likely to approach someone who appears happy or in distress, while avoiding those who appear angry [53, 54]. When faced with potential threats, individuals may also exhibit freezing behavior, characterized by reduced body motion and increased muscles tension [55, 56], which is thought to facilitate perceptual and attentional processes aimed at triggering appropriate actions [24, 57]. The inclination to seek positive social interactions and avoid potentially negative ones may be explained by theoretical frameworks that describe action tendencies and approach-avoidance behaviors in humans [58, 59]. Thus, evaluation of the emotional situation can lead to action tendencies that are not necessarily overt actions, but rather readiness or preparation for action [23, 60, 61]. These tendencies may manifest in bodily mobilization and posture, aimed at achieving a more favorable or less unfavorable situation for the individual [62, 63].

Following this logic, several authors have suggested that body movements are a direct and ecologically valid way to study action tendencies related to approach and avoidance behaviors. Classical approaches have utilized manual tasks in which participants are instructed to either pull a lever or a joystick toward themselves (i.e., approach) or push it away from themselves (i.e., avoidance) in response to different emotional stimuli [64, 65]. More recent studies have examined whole-body changes by instructing participants to step toward or move away from a screen showing emotional facial expressions [66, 67], while others have focused on more implicit changes reflected in postural shifts when participants are in quiet standing [68, 69]. Incorporating posturography in studies of approach-avoidance behaviors is useful, as it provides insights into the underlying mechanisms of these behaviors and their relationship with emotional processing. Posturography allows recording and quantifying small body sway reflected by center of pressure (CoP) displacements, while the participant remains stationary on a force platform [70, 71]. Metrics, such as the projection of the center of pressure on the anteroposterior axis (CoP-Y), have been utilized to study action tendencies objectively and are particularly suitable for assessing spontaneous preparation for approach-avoidance behaviors [72]. Specifically, a forward displacement of the CoP-Y is associated with approach tendency, whereas a backward displacement is associated with avoidance tendency [69].

Despite the potential of posturography to elucidate the interplay between emotion and postural control, the variability in findings has made it difficult to draw definitive conclusions. For example, studies of postural control during static standing have yielded conflicting findings. Gea et al. (2014) [73] found that approach behaviors were reflected by forward displacements of the CoP-Y axis when participants were exposed to dynamic facial expressions of happiness or pain. In contrast, Lebert et al. (2020) [32] reported no such effects when utilizing a range of static emotional faces and videos. Other authors have reported a reduction in the CoP displacements amplitude when exposed to unpleasant pictures, indicating a potential freezing response characterized by body immobilization [74, 75]. Additionally, in tasks where participants are required to initiate a step at a stimulus, faster reaction times have been observed for pleasant as compared to unpleasant visual stimuli [66, 76]. Furthermore, Mirabella et al. (2022) [30] reported a similar outcome when taking into account the arousal factor, observing no effect of it on the obtained results. Note that some other studies suggest that differences in arousal are stronger predictors of postural change than differences in valence [67, 77].

Despite the conflicting evidence, potentially due to factors such as arousal level and task relevance, measures like COP-Y displacements and step initiations continue to serve as valuable indicators of action tendencies by either decreasing (i.e., approach) or increasing (i.e., avoidance) the distance between the emotional stimulus location and the self [72, 78, 79]. Thus, it would be worthwhile to investigate how simulated changes in interpersonal distance, using approaching and withdrawing faces, may affect postural control. The introduction of dynamic facial stimuli could address the methodological limitations seen in prior studies, which have mainly used static images, thereby enhancing ecological validity. This choice is pivotal given that dynamic changes in facial position are more the rule than the exception in real-world interactions. To the best of our knowledge, this question, which has implications for both the theoretical domains of approach-avoidance action tendencies and interpersonal distance preference, is yet to be investigated.

Furthermore, while emotions can shape action tendencies, it has also been demonstrated that action tendencies can influence the categorization of emotional information. In a series of experiments involving emotional words, Neumann et al. (2000) [80] showed that passively perceiving or actively executing movements toward or away from the body can bias the categorization of positive and negative affective information, with a tendency to facilitate the categorization of positive emotions for approach and negative emotions for avoidance movements. In line with this, several studies have investigated how apparent approach and avoidance movements of emotional faces, simulating changes in interpersonal distance, affect participants’ perception of emotions. When participants were asked to categorize the emotion expressed by the face, approach movements facilitate the identification of happy faces, while avoidance movements facilitate the identification of angry faces [81, 82]. However, to our knowledge, no study has examined whether approach and avoidance tendencies of the observer can modulate interpersonal distance. This gap in the literature highlights the need for further investigation, as it could provide insight into a potential bidirectional relationship between action tendencies and interpersonal distance, similar to the established relationship between action tendencies and emotions [80, 83].

This study aimed to investigate the influence of emotional facial expressions on postural action tendencies and on interpersonal distance. Specifically, we seek to answer the following research questions:

  1. Can emotional facial expressions, simulating approach or withdrawal movements, elicit observable postural changes?
  2. How do emotional facial expressions influence preferred interpersonal distances?
  3. To what extent do the action tendencies triggered by approaching or withdrawing emotional faces influence individuals’ preferred distance from others?

To achieve these goals, we conducted an experiment that involved studying the effects of emotional faces (happiness, sadness, fear, disgust, anger and neutral) simulating either an approaching or a withdrawing movements on two measures: (i) the spontaneous approach and avoidance behaviors through the postural parameter CoP-Y in a passive viewing task of the emotional faces, and (ii) the voluntary adjustment of preferred distances in response to emotional faces, using a computerized distance task.

In relation to our primary research question, we predicted that potentially affiliative or prosocial emotional expressions (e.g., happy, neutral, sad, fearful) would induce approach behaviors, whereas aversive emotional expressions (e.g., angry, disgusted) would induce avoidance behaviors. Furthermore, these tendencies are hypothesized to be modulated by the dynamic changes in faces movements. For example, an approaching happy face will amplify the approach tendency compared to a withdrawing happy face, and an approaching threatening face will amplify the avoidance tendency. Regarding our second research question, we hypothesized that participants would prefer greater interpersonal distance in response to aversive expressions like angry and disgusted faces, and shorter distances for affiliative expressions like happy, neutral, sad, and fearful faces. For our third research question, we aim to explore whether these action tendencies, as manifested through CoP-Y shifts, would influence the preferred interpersonal distances set by participants. We predict that approach or avoidance postural shifts will accentuate these preferred distances, making them either smaller or larger.

Materials and methods

Participants

The required number of participants was calculated using G*power 3.0 analysis [84]. Based on previous studies investigating the effects of emotion on posture (e.g., [55, 62, 85]), as well as on preferred interpersonal distance (e.g., [86, 87]), and considering the typical effect size of around 0.15 observed in this literature, we estimated the minimum number of participants required as 48 (f = 0.15, α = 0.05, and β” = 0.95). In all, seventy undergraduates completed the study from mars 2019 to february 2020, as a requirement for an introduction to psychology course. They were all native French speakers with normal or corrected vision. We also ensured that none of the participants had any neurological, psychiatric or significant depressive symptoms that can affect emotional processing [88, 89], or any postural issues such as scoliosis or recent surgery (see [90]). Based on individual data inspection, thirteen participants were not included in the following analysis: two for having stabilometric parameter values greater than three standard deviations beyond the group average and eleven for showing some loss of postural stability due to erratic movements such as self-touching or moving their lower limbs. The final analysis included data from fifty-seven participants (52 females; 5 males) who were analyzed (mean age = 20.2 ± 1.9 years old).

The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee from the Paris Descartes University (reference number n IRB: 20130500001072). All participants were informed about the procedure before the experiment and provided their written informed consent. Within 12 months of the data collection, the personal information gathered for this study has been pseudonymized and after 18 months it has been completely anonymized. True anonymization renders information non-personal.

Stimuli

Sixty emotional faces were used as stimuli, sourced from the study by Lebert et al. (2021) [91]. The faces were computer-generated using FaceGen Modeller software (color front faces, hair removed, and direct gaze), and comprised ten identities (5 women, 5 men), each displaying six different facial expressions, namely happiness, fear, anger, sadness, disgust, and neutrality.

Stimuli used for the computerized distance task

To create the computerized distance task and manipulate the perceived distance to each face, we followed the protocol used by Vieira et al. (2017) [44]. We generated faces of different sizes, ranging from the largest size (18.5 × 25 cm) simulating closer distance, to the smallest size (3.9 × 5 cm) simulating larger distance. The face sizes were incrementally reduced by 2.5 cm in height from the maximum size, resulting in a total of nine distinct face sizes. The adjustment in face size was calibrated to be noticeable yet subtle enough to avoid immediate detection by participants. In addition, we included a tenth size which corresponds to the dimension of a real face (14 × 18.5 cm), allowing for a more precise and realistic adjustment of distance in relation to others. This size was included to provide participants with an option for a typical distance of one meter from an interlocutor, as suggested by Hecht et al. (2019) [5].

To precisely control the perceived distance to each face in our computerized distance task, we calculated the visual angle for each face size using the formula below. Participants were placed at a fixed distance of 100 cm from the screen.

Using this visual angle, we were able to estimate the distance of a real face (18.5 cm) from the participant using the following formula:

This allowed us to use simulated distances instead of image size or visual angles as a more explicit measure of perceived distance. For example, a real 18.5 cm face perceived at a visual angle of 14.25° would correspond to a distance of 74 cm from the participant, while a visual angle of 2.86° would correspond to a distance of 369 cm.

Stimuli used for the postural task

To investigate postural responses to emotional faces that appear to approach or withdraw, participants underwent postural recording while passively viewing the emotional faces either increasing (approach) or decreasing (withdrawal) in size. Following the methodology used in previous postural studies [32, 91, 92], we generated 30-second image sequences, with the same facial stimuli used in the computerized interpersonal distance task. Each sequence included 10 distinct identities of the same emotion, presented for 3 seconds each. The identities were displayed either from smallest to largest (simulating an approaching movement) or from largest to smallest (simulating a withdrawing movement). There were 12 sequences of 30 seconds each, comprised of 2 stimulus movements (approach and withdrawal) for each of the 6 emotions.

Material

Faces were displayed on a Dell screen with a resolution of 1920*1200 pixels, placed at a distance of 1 meter from each participant, and positioned at eye height. The postural task was performed using a force platform (AMTI: AccuSway+®) that enabled the recording of the anteroposterior (AP) displacements of the CoP, allowing for the calculation of the mean position of the CoP on the anteroposterior axis (CoP-Y, in cm). The mean CoP-Y provided information about the displacement toward or away from the stimuli, and could therefore be considered as an index of action tendencies. Data were collected at a frequency of 100 Hz. In the computerized distance-task, the adjustment of the preferred distance required a Microsoft SideWinder Plug and Play GamePad (USB) joystick.

Procedure

The experimental task was performed in a quiet room with a constant luminosity and was divided into two parts: i) an initial computerized distance task (without postural recording), ii) passive viewing of approaching or withdrawing emotional faces with postural recording followed by a computerized distance task (see Fig 1). The task was programmed and implemented using Opensesame [93].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Schematic representation of the experimental tasks.

The top panel of the graph illustrates the experimental setup where the participant stands on a force platform facing emotional faces that are either approaching or withdrawing. Participants took part in a three-part experiment consisting of: (A) an initial computerized stop-distance task, (B) a postural task where participants’ approach or avoidance tendencies were quantified using center of pressure (CoP-Y) displacements, and (C) a final computerized stop-distance task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0298069.g001

Computerized distance task: Initial adjustment.

Participants were placed on a force platform with their feet hip-width apart and their arms positioned along the body to maintain a comfortable posture. Next, participants performed the initial computerized distance task, which involved adjusting the preferred distance from emotional faces by changing their size on a computer screen using a joystick. The task consisted of 72 approach (faces were initially small, simulating a greater distance) and 72 withdrawal trials (faces were bigger, simulating a closer distance). Each included 6 identities repeated twice * 6 emotions. After a fixation cross of 600 ms, the face was displayed on the screen and participants used the joystick to increase (by pressing the “triangle button”) or decrease (by pressing the “cross button”) the face size until they felt comfortable with the distance for face-to-face interaction. They then locked their answer before moving on to the next trial.

Postural task followed by a computerized distance task.

During the main experiment, participants completed twelve blocks, each comprising a postural task followed by a computerized distance task with the same emotion. Each block began with a fixation cross displayed on the screen for 2000 ms. Then, during the postural task, participants passively viewed a 30-sec sequence of emotional faces simulating an approach or withdrawal movement, while the displacements of their center of pressure (CoP-Y) were recorded. After each block, we ensured that participants had correctly identified the displayed emotions through verbal feedback, using a forced-choice task among the six basic emotions Next, participants performed the computerized distance task, which was similar to the initial adjustment task but only included the emotion presented in the previous passive viewing task. The stimuli were presented in a randomized order and the order of presentation of each block was counterbalanced between subjects.

Statistical analysis

The primary statistical analyses consisted of examining whether emotional facial expressions combined with an approach or a withdrawal movement triggered action tendencies visible on the posture. The CoP-Y postural parameter was analyzed using 2 (faces movements: approach and withdrawal movement) x 6 (emotions: happy, fear, anger, sadness, disgust, and neutral expression) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Planned comparisons were used for paired comparisons.

The secondary analyses entailed investigating whether the preferred distance was modulated by the expressed emotion of others and the action tendency induced by the perception of others’ movement. To do so, we compared the preferred distance before the triggering of action tendencies (initial adjustment) and after the approach or withdrawal postural blocks. We conducted a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the preferred distance assessed in the computerized distance-task using 3 (steps: initial adjustment, adjustment after approach postural blocks, adjustment after withdrawal postural blocks) x 6 (emotions: happy, fear, anger, sadness, disgust, and neutral expression) conditions. Planned comparisons were used for paired comparisons.

Finally, to deeper understand the association between spontaneous postural approach and avoidance tendencies and the preferred distance measured through the computerized distance task, we examined for each emotion the correlation between the CoP-Y and the subsequent preferred distance using Spearman correlation coefficients.

All the analyses were performed using R-statistical environment [94]. ANOVAs were computed using the “afex” package [95] followed by planned comparisons performed with the “emmeans” package [96]. Huynh-Feldt corrections were employed to adjust to the violation of the sphericity assumption in testing repeated measures effects. Bonferroni corrections were used to correct the p-values of multiple comparisons. A significance level of p = .05 was used for all statistical analyses. As a measure of the effect size, we calculated the partial eta-squared for the ANOVA and Cohen’s d for each planned comparison. Data are available on the OSF platform.

Results

Postural data were baseline-corrected since the participants were liable to move on both the mediolateral and anteroposterior axes during the presentation of the initial fixation cross. All trials started from the same (0.0) coordinate at the beginning of the emotional stimuli presentation. It should be noted that the order of blocks presentation did not have any effect on the mean CoP-Y position (F(2,54) = 0.35, p >0.05), nor did they interact significantly with the stimuli movements (F(2,54) = 0.07, p >0.05), and the emotions (F(9.71, 262.22) = 1.34, p >0.05), . Furthermore, the order of blocks presentation did not have any effect on the preferred distance (F(2,54) = 2.18, p >0.05, ) nor did they interact significantly with the stimuli movements (F(3.13, 84.41) = 1.93, p >0.05, ) and the emotions (F(3.08, 83.23) = 1.16, p >0.05, ).

First, we conducted an ANOVA on the mean CoP-Y position to examine the effect of Emotions and Faces movements on approach-avoidance action tendencies. The ANOVA revealed a main effect of Emotions (F(5, 280) = 4.39, p <0.001, ). Planned comparisons (see Fig 2) showed that the mean CoP-Y was located significantly further forward in response to neutral and happy faces (M = 0.18, SE = 0.07 and M = 0.18, SE = 0.07 respectively) compared to fearful and sad faces (M = 0.06, SE = 0.05 and M = 0.09, SE = 0.06 respectively, p <.05, Cohen’s d = 0.24), itself significantly further forward compared to disgusted and angry faces (M = —0.03, SE = 0.07 and M = —0.09, SE = 0.06 respectively, p <0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.33). For each of these pairwise emotions (i.e., neutral/happy, sad/fear, disgust/anger), no significant difference was observed (all p >0.3). Although Fig 2 shows that approaching disgusted faces elicit a backward mean COP Y whereas withdrawing disgust faces elicit a forward mean COP Y, the ANOVA did not reveal any effect of Faces movements (F<1) nor any Faces movements * Emotions interaction (F(5,280) = 1.00, p = 0.41) on the mean CoP-Y.

thumbnail
Fig 2. CoP-Y displacements.

Mean COPY as function of Faces movements (approach postural blocks and withdrawal postural blocks) and Emotions (neutral, happiness, sadness, fear, disgust and anger). Error bars depict standard error of the means.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0298069.g002

Second, we investigated if the preferred distance adjusted before the action tendencies triggering (initial adjustment) differed from the one established after the action tendencies triggering (after approach and avoidance postural blocks).

The ANOVA revealed a main effect of the Steps (F(1.55, 86.78) = 17.39, p <.001, ), a main effect of the Emotions (F(1.52, 85.28) = 48.31, p <.001, ), but no Steps * Emotions interaction (F(4.60, 257.44) = 1.61, p = 0.16). Planned comparisons revealed that participants set greater distance during the initial adjustment (M = 151.86, SE = 5.99) than after postural blocks (M = 142.48, SE = 1.42, p <.001, Cohen’s d = 0.27). The preferred distances did not differ after the approach (M = 143.36, SE = 5.61) or withdrawal (M = 141.60, SE = 5.28, p >0.05) postural blocks.

Interestingly (see Fig 3), the preferred distances set in response to neutral (M = 120.19, SE = 2.38) and happy faces (M = 115.76, SE = 2.73) were significantly shorter than with fearful (M = 139.25, SE = 1.91) and sad faces (M = 130.40, SE = 1.85) (p <.001, Cohen’s d = 0.80), itself significantly shorter than the preferred distances to angry (M = 189.72, SE = 4.17) and disgusted faces (M = 178.30, SE = 3.38) (p <.001, Cohen’s d = 0.96). For each of these pairwise emotions, no significant difference was observed (all p >0.08).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Preferred distance.

Mean preferred distance as function of Steps (initial adjustment, adjustment after approach or withdrawal postural blocks and Emotions (neutral, happiness, sadness, fear, disgust and anger). Error bars depict standard error of the means.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0298069.g003

Finally, we investigated whether there is an association between the mechanisms of spontaneous approach and avoidance tendencies, observe on posture, and the mechanisms of interpersonal distance regulation, voluntarily adjusted through a computerized distance task. For each emotion, we examined the correlation between the CoP-Y and the preferred distance. We did not observe any significant correlations between the CoP-Y and the preferred distance (see Fig 4) for emotional faces of neutral (r = 0.14, p = 0.28), happiness (r = -0.10, p = 0.44), fear (r = -0.20, p = 0.14), sadness (r = -0.06, p = 0.64), disgust (r = -0.06, p = 0.65) and anger (r = -0.15, p = 0.26).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Correlations between the CoP-Y (in cm) and the preferred distance (in cm) for emotional faces after postural blocks.

Spearman Rho and p value are provided for each emotional face.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0298069.g004

Discussion

This study aimed to deepen our understanding of how others’ emotions shape approach-avoidance postural behaviors and preferred interpersonal distance. Our primary objective was to investigate whether emotional faces, simulating approach, or withdrawal movements of others could elicit measurable postural shifts indicative of approach or avoidance tendencies. Consequently, we quantified postural changes through COP-Y displacements in response to approaching or withdrawing emotional faces. The secondary objective was to explore, for the first time, the extent to which action tendencies triggered by approaching or withdrawing emotional faces influence individuals’ preferred distance from others.

First, our study yielded significant postural results that contribute to the existing literature on the influence of emotional facial expressions on postural control. Our findings support the idea that approaching and withdrawing faces expressing neutrality and happiness lead to a stronger approach tendency than fearful and sad faces, while disgusted and angry faces elicit avoidance tendencies. These findings contrast with the motivational theory of emotions which predicts automatic approach and avoidance behaviors towards positive and negative stimuli respectively [23, 24]. Previous studies have already pointed out the importance of other factors than valence in the relationship between emotion and action tendencies, in particular the task-relevance emotional content of the stimuli [29, 30, 97], or their potential for future interaction [33, 35]. Extending this framework, our study combines both postural and interpersonal distance measures in a socially-relevant context. Indeed, participants were not only required to identify the emotion displayed but also to indicate their preferred distance for a face-to-face interaction. Our results indicate that the readiness for social interaction associated with a given emotion overcomes its valence in modulating action tendencies. Specifically, despite all being negatively valenced, fear and sadness elicited greater approach behavior, while anger triggered avoidance behavior.

In utilizing dynamic stimuli, our methodology yielded action tendencies observable in CoP-Y and also corroborated the findings of previous work [73], which found that dynamic emotional stimuli induced greater body sway amplitude in the anterior-posterior axis, thereby underscoring their efficacy in eliciting salient cues for action readiness. This observation stands in contrast with the weaker postural coupling reported in studies that employed static images or videos [32, 91, 98]. Additionally, contrary to our expectations, we observed that directionality of the face’s movement —whether approaching or withdrawing— did not yield differential postural responses. This absence of an effect could potentially be attributed to the complexity inherent in capturing the full spectrum of real-world social interactions within a single measure such as CoP-Y displacement. Notably, while our analysis did not indicate any significant interaction between facial movements and emotions, a trend emerged for faces displaying disgust, characterized by backward and forward CoP-Y displacements during approach and withdrawal, respectively. Consequently, our work underscores the imperative of incorporating dynamic emotional stimuli for a more accurate assessment of emotions’ impact on postural control, and provides a nuanced analysis of the observed effects associated with different emotional expressions. It’s worth noting that these action tendencies are relatively subtle and could be considered proxies for underlying motor intentions, rather than overt actions [83]. Consequently, by employing posturographic techniques, as opposed to traditional joystick tasks, we provide a more granular understanding of how such tendencies are manifested in the body. This methodological approach clarifies the complex relationship between emotional states and postural changes, thereby enhancing our understanding of how emotions shape social interactions.

Second, our results not only confirmed but also extended the findings of previous studies on interpersonal distance. Specifically, we found that distances chosen in response to neutral or happy faces were shorter than those chosen in response to fearful or sad expressions. This aligns with previous research, providing further support for the influence of emotional expression on interpersonal distance [18, 44]. Additionally, our study revealed that participants tend to choose larger distances in response to disgusted and angry facial expressions. This finding is consistent with observations from computerized and virtual distance tasks [18, 99], underscoring the perceived threats associated with these expressions. The choice of larger distances may be considered a protective mechanism, akin to maintaining a “safety buffer zone,” that has evolved to protect our integrity through defensive action [100, 101].

Furthermore, by incorporating blocks of approaching and withdrawing emotional faces, our investigation delved deeper into the mechanisms of interpersonal distance adjustment. Interestingly, we observed that the preferred distance were generally shorter following the approach or withdrawal postural blocks compared to the initial adjustment. However, the pattern of distance adjustment was similar across all three steps, suggesting that this difference may be due to habituation to the task or to facial expressions, and no difference was observed in the preferred distance adjustment after the approach and withdrawal postural blocks. Specifically, participants initially chose larger distances, reflecting a common tendency to maintain greater interpersonal space during initial encounters [53, 102]. However, as familiarity with the task and facial expressions increased, a significant shift in the preferred distance occurred, indicating a process of habituation that led to the recalibration of the preferred interpersonal distance. This habituation effect was observed consistently across both approach and withdrawal postural blocks, independent of the specific movements involved. These findings provide insights into the dynamic regulation of interpersonal distance and suggest the role of repeated exposure in shaping social interactions.

Our results indicate that emotional facial expressions and approach/withdrawal movements elicit visible action tendencies on posture, whereas subsequent voluntary adjustments of interpersonal distance are primarily influenced by emotional expressions rather than by facial movements. This emphasizes the intricate interplay between emotional facial expressions, approach/withdrawal movements, and visible action tendencies in posture. Furthermore, although we observed similar effects on both posture and preferred distance, such as forward lean and shorter preferred distance for neutral and happy expressions, and backward lean and larger preferred distance for disgusted and fearful expressions, we did not find significant correlations between these postural measures and the preferred distances chosen by participants. The lack of correlation between postural responses and preferred interpersonal distance could be interpreted as a dissociation between the underlying mechanisms of these two aspects of social interaction. Cognitive appraisal theories offer a plausible explanation for this dissociation (e.g., [103105]). Specifically, automatic reactions such as postural adjustments to emotional facial expressions may be governed by rapid primary appraisals that quickly assess the immediate significance of the emotional cue, such as its potential threat or benefit. On the other hand, the determination of preferred interpersonal distance might be influenced by a more complex set of secondary appraisals, incorporating considerations such as comfort level or personal preferences, thus requiring additional time for decision-making and planning. These findings highlight the complex nature of the interaction between emotions, posture, and interpersonal distance, highlighting the need for more nuanced research to elucidate the underlying mechanisms. A limitation of our study is that we did not measure arousal levels of the emotional stimuli. This leaves open the question of how arousal may influence the approach-avoidance behaviors we observed [67, 77].

While our findings do not yield a direct correlation between postural action tendencies and preferred interpersonal distance, they do offer insights into how emotional cues can influence each of these variables independently. Although the data are preliminary, they open the door for future research, particularly in understanding potential clinical applications for disorders affecting emotional functioning. Indeed, our work aligns with existing literature that investigates the reciprocal relationships between emotions and posture across diverse populations, including those with neurodevelopmental pathologies where emotional and postural difficulties often coexist (for review, see [106]). We hope that our work serves as a starting point for more extensive studies that could eventually contribute to improved diagnostic and therapeutic strategies.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this study sheds light on the intricate relationship between emotional cues, postural action tendencies, and the regulation of interpersonal distance, all of which are critical factors in successful social interactions. By examining a diverse range of emotional faces, we found that different emotional expressions significantly influenced postural action tendencies, with happy and neutral faces evoking a stronger approach tendency and disgusted and angry faces eliciting an avoidance tendency. Furthermore, our findings provide additional evidence of the modulation of interpersonal distance regulation in response to emotional cues, revealing distinct adjustments based on different emotional facial expressions, including shorter distances in response to neutral or happy faces, and larger distances in response to disgusted or angry expressions. However, the absence of a correlation between postural measures and preferred distance suggests a dissociation between the mechanisms underlying spontaneous approach and avoidance tendencies and those involved in the voluntary regulation of interpersonal distance. Overall, these findings provide a foundation for further research on the underlying mechanisms driving these processes and their implications for interpersonal communication and overall social well-being.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the participants for their cooperation.

References

  1. 1. Geers L, Coello Y. The relationship between action, social and multisensory spaces. Scientific Reports. 2023;13(1):202. pmid:36604525
  2. 2. Hayduk LA. Personal space: An evaluative and orienting overview. Psychological bulletin. 1978;85(1):117.
  3. 3. Welsch R, von Castell C, Hecht H. The anisotropy of personal space. PLoS One. 2019;14(6):e0217587. pmid:31163063
  4. 4. Ahs F Fredrik, Dunsmoor JE, Zielinski D, LaBar KS. Spatial proximity amplifies valence in emotional memory and defensive approach-avoidance. Neuropsychologia. 2015;70:476–485. pmid:25541499
  5. 5. Hecht H, Welsch R, Viehoff J, Longo MR. The shape of personal space. Acta psychologica. 2019;193:113–122. pmid:30622020
  6. 6. McBride G, King M, James J. Social proximity effects on galvanic skin responses in adult humans. The Journal of psychology. 1965;61(1):153–157. pmid:5833997
  7. 7. Sawada Y. Blood pressure and heart rate responses to an intrusion on personal space. Japanese Psychological Research. 2003;45(2):115–121.
  8. 8. Szpak A, Loetscher T, Churches O, Thomas NA, Spence CJ, Nicholls ME. Keeping your distance: attentional withdrawal in individuals who show physiological signs of social discomfort. Neuropsychologia. 2015;70:462–467. pmid:25447057
  9. 9. Cartaud A, Ott L, Iachini T, Honoré J, Coello Y. The influence of facial expression at perceptual threshold on electrodermal activity and social comfort distance. Psychophysiology. 2020;57(9):e13600. pmid:32437046
  10. 10. Elliot AJ. The hierarchical model of approach-avoidance motivation. Motivation and emotion. 2006;30:111–116.
  11. 11. McCall C. Mapping social interactions: the science of proxemics. Social Behavior from Rodents to Humans: Neural Foundations and Clinical Implications. 2017:295–308. pmid:26728171
  12. 12. Scheflen AE. Human communication: Behavioral programs and their integration in interaction. Behavioral Science. 1968;13(1):44–55.
  13. 13. Kennedy DP, Gläscher J, Tyszka JM, Adolphs R. Personal space regulation by the human amygdala. Nature neuroscience. 2009;12(10):1226–1227. pmid:19718035
  14. 14. Russo NF. Eye contact, interpersonal distance, and the equilibrium theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1975;31(3):497.
  15. 15. Wieser MJ, Pauli P, Grosseibl M, Molzow I, Mühlberger A. Virtual social interactions in social anxiety—the impact of sex, gaze, and interpersonal distance. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2010;13(5):547–554. pmid:20950179
  16. 16. Lockard JS, Mcvittie RI, Isaac LM. Functional significance of the affiliative smile. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 1977;9:367–370.
  17. 17. Kim NG, Son H. How facial expressions of emotion affect distance perception. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:1825. pmid:26635708
  18. 18. Ruggiero G, Frassinetti F, Coello Y, Rapuano M, di Cola AS, Iachini T. The effect of facial expressions on peripersonal and interpersonal spaces. Psychological Research. 2017;81(6):1232–1240. pmid:27785567
  19. 19. Cartaud A, Ruggiero G, Ott L, Iachini T, Coello Y. Physiological response to facial expressions in peripersonal space determines interpersonal distance in a social interaction context. Frontiers in psychology. 2018;9:657. pmid:29867639
  20. 20. Izard CE. Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on psychological science. 2007;2(3):260–280. pmid:26151969
  21. 21. Waller BM, Whitehouse J, Micheletta J. Rethinking primate facial expression: A predictive framework. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;82:13–21. pmid:27637495
  22. 22. Frijda N. Emotion experience. Cognition & Emotion. 2005;19(4):473–497.
  23. 23. Lang PJ, Bradley MM. Emotion and the motivational brain. Biological psychology. 2010;84(3):437–450. pmid:19879918
  24. 24. Bradley MM, Codispoti M, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ. Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion. 2001;1(3):276. pmid:12934687
  25. 25. Carver CS, Harmon-Jones E. Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135(2):183–204. pmid:19254075
  26. 26. Wilkowski BM, Meier BP. Bring it on: Angry facial expressions potentiate approach-motivated motor behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2010;98(2):201–210. pmid:20085395
  27. 27. Fawver B, Beatty GF, Naugle KM, Hass CJ, Janelle CM. Emotional State Impacts Center of Pressure Displacement Before Forward Gait Initiation. Journal of Applied Biomechanics. 2015;31(1):35–40. pmid:25322476
  28. 28. Phaf RH, Mohr SE, Rotteveel M, Wicherts JM. Approach, avoidance, and affect: a meta-analysis of approach-avoidance tendencies in manual reaction time tasks. Frontiers in psychology. 2014;5:378. pmid:24847292
  29. 29. Mirabella G. The weight of emotions in decision-making: how fearful and happy facial stimuli modulate action readiness of goal-directed actions. Frontiers in psychology. 2018;9:1334. pmid:30116211
  30. 30. Mirabella G, Grassi M, Mezzarobba S, Bernardis P. Angry and happy expressions affect forward gait initiation only when task relevant. Emotion. 2023;23(2):387. pmid:35588387
  31. 31. Cunningham WA, Arbuckle NL, Jahn A, Mowrer SM, Abduljalil AM. Aspects of neuroticism and the amygdala: chronic tuning from motivational styles. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(12):3399–3404. pmid:20600183
  32. 32. Lebert A, Chaby L, Garnot C, Vergilino-Perez D. Happy facial expressions impair inhibitory control with respect to fearful facial expressions but only when task-relevant. Experimental Brain Research. 2020;238(12):2877–2886.
  33. 33. Hess U, Blairy S, Kleck RE. The influence of facial emotion displays, gender, and ethnicity on judgments of dominance and affiliation. Journal of Nonverbal behavior. 2000;24:265–283.
  34. 34. Marsh AA, Ambady N, Kleck RE. The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach-and avoidance-related behaviors. Emotion. 2005;5(1):119. pmid:15755225
  35. 35. Geraci A, Franchin L. Is defensive behavior a subtype of prosocial behaviors? Frontiers in Psychology. 2021;12:678370. pmid:34248777
  36. 36. Hayduk LA. Personal space: Where we now stand. Psychological bulletin. 1983;94(2):293.
  37. 37. Hayduk LA. Personal space: The conceptual and measurement implications of structural equation models. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement. 1985;17(2):140.
  38. 38. Adams L, Zuckerman D. The effect of lighting conditions on personal space requirements. The journal of general psychology. 1991;118(4):335–340.
  39. 39. Pedersen DM. Prediction of behavioral personal space from simulated personal space. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1973;37(3):803–813.
  40. 40. Duke MP, Nowicki S. A new measure and social-learning model for interpersonal distance. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality. 1972;.
  41. 41. Sorokowska A, Sorokowski P, Hilpert P, Cantarero K, Frackowiak T, Ahmadi K, et al. Preferred interpersonal distances: A global comparison. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2017;48(4):577–592.
  42. 42. Bailenson JN, Blascovich J, Beall AC, Loomis JM. Interpersonal distance in immersive virtual environments. Personality and social psychology bulletin. 2003;29(7):819–833. pmid:15018671
  43. 43. Iachini T, Coello Y, Frassinetti F, Ruggiero G. Body space in social interactions: a comparison of reaching and comfort distance in immersive virtual reality. PloS one. 2014;9(11):e111511. pmid:25405344
  44. 44. Vieira JB, Tavares TP, Marsh AA, Mitchell DGV. Emotion and personal space: Neural correlates of approach-avoidance tendencies to different facial expressions as a function of coldhearted psychopathic traits. Human Brain Mapping. 2017;38(3):1492–1506. pmid:27859920
  45. 45. Lee YC, Yu X, Xiong W. A comparative evaluation of the four measurement methods for comfort and reachability distance perceptions. Behavior Research Methods. 2021:1–12. pmid:34664228
  46. 46. Frith C. Role of facial expressions in social interactions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2009;364(1535):3453–3458. pmid:19884140
  47. 47. Chaby L, Chetouani M, Plaza M, Cohen D. Exploring multimodal social-emotional behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: an interface between social signal processing and psychopathology. In: IEEE, International Conference on Social Computing, SocialCom/PASSAT. IEEE; 2012:950–954.
  48. 48. Xavier J, Magnat J, Sherman A, Gauthier S, Cohen D, Chaby L. A developmental and clinical perspective of rhythmic interpersonal coordination: from mimicry toward the interconnection of minds. Journal of Physiology-Paris. 2016;110(4):420–426. pmid:28625683
  49. 49. Anderson AK, Christoff K, Panitz D, De Rosa E, Gabrieli JDE. Neural Correlates of the Automatic Processing of Threat Facial Signals. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23(13):5627–5633. pmid:12843265
  50. 50. Cartaud A, Quesque F, Coello Y. Wearing a face mask against Covid-19 results in a reduction of social distancing Plos one. 2020;15(12):e0243023. pmid:33284812
  51. 51. Villani C, D’Ascenzo S, Scerrati E, Ricciardelli P, Nicoletti R, Lugli L. Wearing the face mask affects our social attention over space Frontiers in psychology. 2022;13:923558. pmid:35992481
  52. 52. Scerrati E, D’Ascenzo S, and Nicoletti R, Villani C, Lugli L. Assessing interpersonal proximity evaluation in the COVID-19 era: Evidence from the affective priming task. Frontiers in psychology. 2022;13:901730. pmid:35783734
  53. 53. Lloyd DM. The space between us: A neurophilosophical framework for the investigation of human interpersonal space. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009;33(3):297–304.
  54. 54. Fini C, Verbeke P, Sieber S, Moors A, Brass M, Genschow O. The influence of threat on perceived spatial distance to out-group members. Psychological research. 2020;84:757–764. pmid:30191315
  55. 55. Hagenaars MA, Roelofs K, Stins JF. Human freezing in response to affective films. Anxiety, Stress & Coping. 2014;27(1):27–37. pmid:23805855
  56. 56. Gladwin TE, Hashemi MM, van Ast V, Roelofs K. Ready and waiting: Freezing as active action preparation under threat. Neuroscience Letters. 2016;619:182–188. pmid:26994781
  57. 57. Livermore JJ, Klaassen FH, Bramson B, Hulsman AM, Meijer SW, Held L, et al. Approach-avoidance decisions under threat: the role of autonomic psychophysiological states. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2021;15:621517. pmid:33867915
  58. 58. Elliott AJ, Church MA. A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1997;72(1):218.
  59. 59. Frijda NH. Impulsive action and motivation. Biological psychology. 2010;84(3):570–579. pmid:20064583
  60. 60. Frijda NH, Kuipers P, Ter Schure E. Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1989;57(2):212.
  61. 61. Frijda NH. Emotion experience and its varieties. Emotion Review. 2009;1(3):264–271.
  62. 62. Horslen BC, Carpenter MG. Arousal, valence and their relative effects on postural control. Experimental brain research. 2011;215:27–34. pmid:21947171
  63. 63. Lebert A, Vergilino-Perez D, Chaby L. Pour une meilleure compréhension des liens réciproques entre émotion et posture. Revue de neuropsychologie. 2020;(1):11–18.
  64. 64. Chen M, Bargh JA. Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and social psychology bulletin. 1999;25(2):215–224.
  65. 65. Rinck M, Becker ES. Approach and avoidance in fear of spiders. Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry. 2007;38(2):105–120. pmid:17126289
  66. 66. Stins JF, Roelofs K, Villan J, Kooijman K, Hagenaars MA, Beek PJ. Walk to me when I smile, step back when I’m angry: emotional faces modulate whole-body approach–avoidance behaviors. Experimental brain research. 2011;212:603–611. pmid:21698468
  67. 67. Bouman D, Stins JF. Back off! The effect of emotion on backward step initiation. Human movement science. 2018;57:280–290. pmid:28919167
  68. 68. Hillman CH, Rosengren KS, Smith DP. Emotion and motivated behavior: postural adjustments to affective picture viewing. Biological psychology. 2004;66(1):51–62. pmid:15019170
  69. 69. Lelard T, Stins J, Mouras H. Postural responses to emotional visual stimuli. Neurophysiologie Clinique. 2019;49(2):109–114. pmid:30711434
  70. 70. Raymakers J, Samson M, Verhaar H. The assessment of body sway and the choice of the stability parameter (s). Gait & posture. 2005;21(1):48–58. pmid:15536033
  71. 71. Blaszczyk JW. The use of force-plate posturography in the assessment of postural instability. Gait & posture. 2016;44:1–6. pmid:27004624
  72. 72. Eerland A, Guadalupe TM, Franken IHA, Zwaan RA. Posture as Index for Approach-Avoidance Behavior. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(2):5. pmid:22355355
  73. 73. Gea J, Muñoz MA, Costa I, Ciria LF, Miranda JGV, Montoya P. Viewing Pain and Happy Faces Elicited Similar Changes in Postural Body Sway. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(8):e104381. pmid:25093727
  74. 74. Azevedo TM, Volchan E, Imbiriba LA, Rodrigues EC, Oliveira JM, Oliveira LF, et al. A freezing-like posture to pictures of mutilation. Psychophysiology. 2005;42(3):255–260. pmid:15943678
  75. 75. Roelofs K, Hagenaars MA, Stins J. Facing freeze: social threat induces bodily freeze in humans Psychological science. 2010;21(11)1575–1581. pmid:20876881
  76. 76. Gélat T, Chapus CF. Reaction time in gait initiation depends on the time available for affective processing. Neuroscience letters. 2015;609:69–73. pmid:26455865
  77. 77. Bouman D, Stins JF, Beek PJ. Arousal and exposure duration affect forward step initiation. Frontiers in psychology. 2015;6:1667. pmid:26579048
  78. 78. Stins J, Beek P. Organization of voluntary stepping in response to emotion-inducing pictures. Gait & Posture. 2011;34(2):164–168. pmid:21549605
  79. 79. Rougier M, Muller D, Ric F, Alexopoulos T, Batailler C, Smeding A, et al. A new look at sensorimotor aspects in approach/avoidance tendencies: The role of visual whole-body movement information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2018;76:42–53.
  80. 80. Neumann R, Strack F. Approach and avoidance: the influence of proprioceptive and exteroceptive cues on encoding of affective information. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2000;79(1):39. pmid:10909876
  81. 81. van Peer JM, Rotteveel M, Spinhoven P, Tollenaar MS, Roelofs K. Affect-congruent approach and withdrawal movements of happy and angry faces facilitate affective categorisation. Cognition and Emotion. 2010;24(5):863–875.
  82. 82. Nelson AJ, Adams RB Jr, Stevenson MT, Weisbuch M, Norton MI. Approach-avoidance movement influences the decoding of anger and fear expressions. Social Cognition. 2013;31(6):745–757.
  83. 83. Tcherkassof A, Frijda NH. Les émotions: une conception relationnelle. L’Année psychologique. 2014;114(3):501–535.
  84. 84. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences;39(2):175–191.
  85. 85. Facchinetti LD, Imbiriba LA, Azevedo TM, Vargas CD, Volchan E. Postural modulation induced by pictures depicting prosocial or dangerous contexts;410(1):52–56. pmid:17056179
  86. 86. Vieira JB, Marsh AA. Don’t stand so close to me: psychopathy and the regulation of interpersonal distance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;7. pmid:24454284
  87. 87. Cartaud A, Lenglin V, Coello Y. Contrast effect of emotional context on interpersonal distance with neutral social stimuli. Cognition. 2022;218:104913. pmid:34610511
  88. 88. Chaby L, Luherne-du Boullay V, Chetouani M, Plaza M. Compensating for age limits through emotional crossmodal integration. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6. pmid:26074845
  89. 89. Dalili MN, Penton-Voak IS, Harmer CJ, Munafò MR. Meta-analysis of emotion recognition deficits in major depressive disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2015;45(6):1135–1144. pmid:25395075
  90. 90. Beaulieu M, Toulotte C, Gatto L, Rivard CH, Teasdale N, Simoneau M, et al. Postural imbalance in non-treated adolescent idiopathic scoliosis at different periods of progression. European Spine Journal. 2009;18(1):38–44. pmid:19066989
  91. 91. Lebert A, Chaby L, Guillin A, Chekroun S, Vergilino-Perez D. Are You “Gazing” at Me? How Others’ Gaze Direction and Facial Expression Influence Gaze Perception and Postural Control. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;12:730953.
  92. 92. de Freitas Brandão A, Palluel E, Olivier I, Nougier V. Effects of emotional videos on postural control in children Gait & Posture. 2016;45:175–180.
  93. 93. Mathôt S, Schreij D, Theeuwes J. OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior research methods. 2012;44(2):314–324. pmid:22083660
  94. 94. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing; 2013.
  95. 95. Singmann H, Bolker B, Westfall J, Aust F. Package ‘afex’; 2015.
  96. 96. Lenth R. Package: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means; 2019.
  97. 97. Calbi M, Montalti M, Pederzani C, Arcuri E, Umiltà MA, Gallese V, et al. Emotional body postures affect inhibitory control only when task-relevant. Frontiers in Psychology. 2022:6857. pmid:36405118
  98. 98. Stins JF, Beek PJ. Effects of affective picture viewing on postural control. BMC neuroscience. 2007;8:1–7. pmid:17916245
  99. 99. Miller HC, Chabriac AS, Molet M. The impact of facial emotional expressions and sex on interpersonal distancing as evaluated in a computerized stop-distance task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale. 2013;67(3):188–194. pmid:23205511
  100. 100. Horowitz MJ, Duff DF, Stratton LO. Body-buffer zone: exploration of personal space. Archives of general psychiatry. 1964;11(6):651–656. pmid:14209746
  101. 101. Graziano MS, Cooke DF Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(6):845–859. pmid:16277998
  102. 102. Hall ET. The Hidden Dimension. Garden City, New York: Doubleday; 1966.
  103. 103. Smith CA, Lazarus RS. Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition & emotion. 1993;7(3-4):233–269.
  104. 104. Scherer KR. Appraisal considered as a process of multilevel sequential checking. Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research. 2001;92(120):57.
  105. 105. Moors Agnes, Ellsworth PC, Scherer KR, Frijda NH Appraisal theories of emotion: State of the art and future development Emotion Review. 2013;5(2):119–124.
  106. 106. Lebert A, Vergilino-Perez D, Cuny ML, Prodhomme J, Puget S, Beccaria K, et al. Kyste de la fosse postérieure chez l’enfant: intérêt de l’évaluation posturographique et socio-émotionnelle. Neurophysiologie Clinique. 2019;49(6):419.