Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Uric acid levels and risk of cognitive impairment: Dose-response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies

  • Qianqian Liu,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    Affiliations School of Traditional Chinese Medicine, Shandong University of Traditional Chinese Medicine, Jinan, China, Department of Chinese and Western Medicine, Shandong Provincial Hospital Affiliated to Shandong First Medical University, Jinan, China

  • Min Peng,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Chinese and Western Medicine, Shandong Provincial Hospital Affiliated to Shandong First Medical University, Jinan, China

  • Tiantian Yang,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Chinese and Western Medicine, Shandong Provincial Hospital Affiliated to Shandong First Medical University, Jinan, China

  • Guomin Si

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    sgm977@126.com

    Affiliations School of Traditional Chinese Medicine, Shandong University of Traditional Chinese Medicine, Jinan, China, Department of Chinese and Western Medicine, Shandong Provincial Hospital Affiliated to Shandong First Medical University, Jinan, China

Abstract

Purpose

Studying the effects of uric acid levels on cognitive function and quantifying the dose-response relationship.

Methods

Based on PubMed and Embase search terms, we identified prospective cohort studies that included blood uric acid as a risk factor and cognitive impairment as a result up to September 2022. We extracted pooled relative risks (RRs) and corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs).

Results

Nine reports (including 488,915 participants and 5516 cognitive impairment cases) with median follow-up of 8.8–22 years were eligible for analyses. Compared with lowest category of blood uric acid concentration, the combined RR of cognitive impairment events in the highest classification was 0.81 (95% CI: 0.70–0.92, P < 0.001). Dose-response analysis of eight reports (including 484,297 participants and 5059 cognitive impairment cases) showed that there was no evidence of a curvilinear relationship between blood uric acid levels and cognitive impairment (P = 0.51 for nonlinear relationship). The summary RR of cognitive impairment for an increase of 1 mg/dL blood uric acid level was 0.98 (95% CI: 0.95–1.00; linear trend P = 0.07, I2 = 67.1%, heterogeneity P < 0.05). There was also a linear negative association between blood uric acid levels and cognitive impairment risk in the male subgroup analysis (RR = 0.97, 95% CI: 0.95–0.99, P < 0.05).

Conclusion

Levels of blood uric acid are not related to risk of cognitive impairment. A subgroup analysis shows that the rise in blood uric acid levels in the male population is related to a decreased risk of cognitive impairment. These results need to be confirmed by further studies.

1 Introduction

Cognitive impairment is an umbrella term for impairment of one or more cognitive domains which may be influenced by various causes and may range from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to different stages of dementia [1, 2]. With the increase in population aging, cognitive impairment has become a global public health concern, causing serious social and economic losses [3, 4]. MCI is estimated to affect approximately 25% of people aged 80–84 years, with a cumulative incidence of dementia of 14.9% in people aged 65 years and older at 2 years of follow-up [5]. An estimated 55 million people were living with dementia in 2019, it is predicted that by 2050, this number will reach 139 million, according to the updated data from the World Health Organization (WHO) [6]. With the full implementation of the seven priority action areas of the WHO Global Action Plan on the Public Health Response to Dementia, more attention has been paid to prevent disease progression and delay its progression by controlling modifiable risk factors [1, 7, 8]. Among the known risk factors, blood uric acid (UA) has attracted particular attention.

The body produces UA through purine metabolism and it has both oxidation-promoting and anti-oxidant properties. It has been found that UA may be involved in mechanisms such as oxidative stress and inflammatory responses associated with cognitive impairment and acts as a double-edged sword for cognitive function in the brain [9]. Some observational studies suggest that high UA levels may have deleterious effects on brain health, including the development of cerebral small vessel disease and cognitive decline [10, 11]. However, studies have also indicated that serum UA plays a neuroprotective role in the diseases of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s and that low blood UA levels are not only associated with faster disease progression, but are also indicative of malnutrition [1215]. Appropriate increases in UA levels within the normal range can instead delay the onset and progression of cognitive impairment [16]. Notably, many vascular risk factors and diseases have also been associated with UA, which may predispose people to cognitive impairment [2]. This may also, to some extent, alter the relations between UA levels and poor cognitive outcomes. In addition, recent studies suggest that the impact of UA levels on cognitive function may vary according to gender and dementia subtype, but with variable results [17, 18].

The above cumulative findings develop new approaches to identifying and treating cognitive impairment risk factors and highlight the potential for blood UA management in the prevention of cognitive impairment. However, the correlation between blood UA and cognitive impairment remains controversial, although extensive research had been utilized to explore the above relationship. Furthermore, studies have examined whether blood UA is associated with cognitive impairment in published systematic reviews [19, 20], but not specifically within prospective cohort studies, and no relevant dose-response analyses have been evaluated. Therefore, we sought to quantify the relationship between blood UA levels and cognitive impairment by conducting a dose-response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies.

2 Material and methods

2.1 Search strategy

From inception until September 2022, we searched PubMed and Embase for prospective cohort studies investigating UA levels and cognitive impairment (including MCI and dementia of all types). The meta-analysis was prospectively registered in INPLASY (2022100111). Both MESH subject headings and free terms were employed in the search (S1 and S2 Tables). Furthermore, a thorough review of the references of the relevant original articles was also undertaken to uncover further related studies. It was not restricted in any way in terms of language. Two researchers independently scrutinized all the retrieved articles and preliminary eligibility was carefully reviewed in accordance with titles, abstracts, and text in full when necessary. PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) statement was followed in our study [21].

2.2 Study selection

The included studies must meet the following criteria: 1) Prospective cohort studies; 2) Cognitive impairment (including all types of dementia and MCI) as a specific outcome, based on certain standardized diagnostic criteria (Table 1); 3) The investigators classified UA concentrations into at least three categories and provided relative risks (RRs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for each. Furthermore, we excluded studies that lacked sufficient data, reviews, editorials, and non-human studies. Studies that were reported more than once were compiled based on the results with the longest follow-up.

thumbnail
Table 1. Assessment of cognitive function in included studies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.t001

2.3 Data extraction and quality evaluation

Two authors (QQL and TTY) independently extracted data from each study using standard extraction formats. Each study was analyzed for the following factors: author, publication year, study name, study location, gender and age of participants, diagnostic criteria, size of the sample (number of participants and incidents), UA levels, cognitive impairment (endpoint), follow-up year and RRs (95% CIs) for different UA levels.

Studies were evaluated according to Newcastle-Ottawa quality criteria [22]. Studies with quality scores of 0 to 3, 4 to 6, and 7 to 9 were considered low quality, moderate quality, and high quality, respectively. When the study had multiple adjusted models, we extracted the model that reflected the most adjustment for potential confounders. To resolve differences by consensus, the review process was guided by group consensus and a third reviewer was consulted (MP).

2.4 Statistical analysis

In this study, the hazard ratios were considered equivalent to the RRs, and the RRs with 95% CI were considered to be the effect sizes of all studies. Any outcomes stratified by gender were considered to be two separate reports[23]. A dose-response relationship was investigated by using the generalized least squares regression method of Greenland and Longnecker and Orsini and colleagues [24, 25]. UA, number of reported cases, total participants, and RRs with 95% CIs were extracted for each stratum according to this method.

The RR corresponded to the mean/median of the UA level categories for each stratum in this meta-analysis. If they were not accounted for, the mean of the lower and upper limits per category was used as the dose [26]. If the highest/lowest UA level category was open, in order to calculate the midpoint, half the width of the adjacent categories were added (highest category) and subtracted (lowest category) [26]. If no numbers are available for cases or non-cases per category, the method of Chêne et al was used to supply an approximation based on cases in total and RR for per category [27, 28]. In addition, we assessed potential curvilinear relationships between UA levels and cognitive impairment using four-node restricted cubic splines at the 5%, 35%, 65% and 95% percentile distributions.

Statistical heterogeneity between studies was assessed by the I2 test. A low value of 25%, a medium value of 50%, and a high value of 75% were considered [29]. When heterogeneity was negligible, we used a fixed-effect model (Mantel–Haenszel method) and when it was significant, we used a random-effect model (DerSimonian and Laird method). Overall effects were examined using forest plots. Publication bias was assessed using Egger’s regression asymmetry test and Begg’s test. Stratified analyses were also conducted based on gender, age, location of the study, the length of follow-up, the number of participants, dementia type, and diabetes status. Stata version 13.0 of the software (Stata Corp, College Station, TX, USA) was used to perform all statistical analyses. Statistical significance was determined by P < 0.05.

3 Results

3.1 Literature search

The results of the literature search and selection are shown in Fig 1. We retrieved 606 articles in PubMed and 486 articles in Embase before September 30th, 2022. After excluding duplicate papers (n = 53) and studies that failed to satisfy the inclusion criteria (n = 984), the remaining 55 articles appeared relevant to this meta-analysis. Twenty-two articles published were excluded due to less than three UA categories after reviewing these publications in their entirety. We also excluded 22 studies because they were interventional or retrospective studies. Due to insufficient data, four articles were excluded. Another two articles were excluded because they were meeting abstracts. Three of the six articles examined men and women separately in the final meta-analysis. Overall, this meta-analysis included six articles and nine independent reports. We compared the blood UA levels of the highest and lowest categories based on nine studies, and in eight studies, the impact of every 1 mg/dL of blood UA level was analyzed.

3.2 Study characteristics

Tables 14 show extracted information from the included studies. A median follow-up of 8.8 to 22 years was provided for 488,915 participants with 5516 cognitive impairments. Of the total cohort, four studies were conducted in Europe and the others were from China and the United States [3035]. The research quality evaluation results (scores 0–9) of all studies were ≥7 (high quality), with an average score of 7.2 (Table 5).

thumbnail
Table 2. Characteristics of participants in included studies of UA in relation to risk of cognitive impairment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.t002

thumbnail
Table 4. Outcomes and covariates of included studies of UA in relation to risk of cognitive impairment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Study quality of included studies on serum UA and risk of cognitive decline.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.t005

3.3 Meta-analysis

3.3.1 Relationship between blood UA levels and cognitive impairment.

Six articles with nine reports were considered to provide sufficient data for the analysis of blood UA levels and cognitive impairment relationships [3035]. Compared with the lowest category of blood UA level, the combined RR of cognitive impairment events in the highest group was 0.81 (95% CI: 0.70–0.92, P < 0.001) (Fig 2). Results of the research showed no significant heterogeneity (P = 0.051, I2 = 48.2%).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Forest plot of association between highest versus lowest categories of UA and risk of cognitive impairment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g002

3.3.2 Dose-response analysis.

Five articles with eight reports were considered in this dose-response analysis of blood UA levels and cognitive impairment events [3135]. UA levels did not appear to be associated with the risk of cognitive impairment in a curvilinear manner (P = 0.51 for nonlinear relationship; Fig 3). The summary RR of cognitive impairment for an increase of 1 mg/dL blood UA level was 0.98 (95% CI: 0.95–1.00; linear trend P = 0.07, I2 = 67.1%, heterogeneity P < 0.05) (Fig 4). The regression tests of Begg’s and Egger’s did not demonstrate substantial publication bias (P = 0.902 and P = 0.962). In addition, we conducted a sensitivity analysis to assess whether outcomes were affected by the excluded studies, and found no substantial changes in the results (Fig 5).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Dose-response analyses of UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Forest plot of UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. The sensitive plot on the association of UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g005

3.3.3 Subgroup analysis.

Subgroup analyses were conducted based on gender, age, location of the study, the length of follow-up, dementia type, diabetes status, and number of participants, to explore the potential source of heterogeneity (Table 6). Fig 6 shows that in the male cohort, blood UA levels and cognitive impairment risk exhibited a linear negative relationship (RR = 0.97, 95% CI: 0.95–0.99, P < 0.05). This meta-analysis of blood UA levels found a negatively linear relationship between UA levels and cognitive impairment in individuals who were followed for fewer than ten years (RR = 0.96, 95% CI: 0.95–0.98, P < 0.001) (Fig 7). When stratified according to participants, if the number of participants in total was more than 2000, the incidence risk had a linear negative correlation (RR = 0.98, 95% CI: 0.97–1.00, P < 0.05) (Fig 8). In any of the three subgroup analyses, there was no evidence of significant heterogeneity (P > 0.05) (Figs 911).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Dose-response plot on the association of UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment in male.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g006

thumbnail
Fig 7. Dose-response plot on the association of UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment with less than 10 years of follow-up.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g007

thumbnail
Fig 8. Dose-response plot on the association of UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment with more than 2,000 participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g008

thumbnail
Fig 9. Forest plot of UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment in male.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g009

thumbnail
Fig 10. Forest plot of UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment with less than 10 years of follow-up.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g010

thumbnail
Fig 11. Forest plot of UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment with more than 2,000 participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.g011

thumbnail
Table 6. An analysis of the relative risk of cognitive impairment based on stratification.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293832.t006

4 Discussion

4.1 Principal findings

To the best of our knowledge, the first dose-response meta-analysis conducted on prospective cohort studies investigating UA concentrations and cognitive impairment was presented in our study. Based on the nine prospective cohort studies, a significant correlation was identified between blood UA levels and risk of cognitive impairment in the comparison of the highest versus lowest group. The remarkable relationships disappeared when we conducted a dose-response analysis. However, studies including only male participants, with more than 2000 volunteers and followed up for less than 10 years, demonstrated that a reduction in cognitive impairment was associated with higher blood UA levels. Nevertheless, this should be evaluated further.

4.2 Results in relation to other studies

Our findings identified that the risk of cognitive impairment is not significantly correlated with blood UA levels. A meta-analysis of 5575 participants pointed out that there is a potential risk of Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s dementia associated with low concentrations of blood UA, but this does not apply to vascular dementia [19]. Another meta-analysis of 1128 cases and 2498 controls found that high UA level in blood was a protective element for Alzheimer’s disease [20]. Different study types, study sample size, cognitive impairment diagnosis, and not well-controlled confounders may have contributed to this discrepancy.

In clinical practice, the normal levels of serum UA range from 2 mg/dL to 7 mg/dL. In hyperuricemia, the serum urate concentration is above 7mg/dL, while in hypouricemia, it is below 2mg/dL [36, 37]. To date, most cohort studies have examined the relationship between UA levels and cognitive trajectories. A national retrospective cohort study of older adults conducted in Korea found that there was a lower risk of dementia among patients with gout compared to the general population, regardless of the type of dementia [38]. This discovery provides lateral support for the beneficial neuroprotective effects of hyperuricemia, which are similar to some of our conclusions. Further study showed that both serum UA baseline levels and dynamic changes may affect cognitive trajectories. Although stable or moderate increases of serum UA levels may benefit cognition, hyperuricemia persisting for a long time can negatively impact cognition [39]. However, the observations of Liu et al. find that both high and low blood UA levels are correlated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment, supporting a U-shaped relationship between these associations [40]. Our study includes the populations with hyperuricemia, normal UA levels and hypouricemia. The included studies lack stratification data of the three populations mentioned above, so we cannot consider them as subgroup variables for further analysis. Future studies may be specifically designed to increase the diversity of the study population to solve this problem.

4.3 Interpretation of the results

We further considered the reasons for lack of an overall relationship between blood UA levels and cognitive impairment. First, it is well known that blood UA levels are associated with obesity, alcohol consumption, smoking, physical activity, hypertension, diabetes and other elements [10, 41, 42], that are also risk factors for cognitive impairment. Despite our selection of the most adjusted models, the influence of residual confounding factors on the basis of the results could not be excluded. We lacked an assessment of nutritional status, which might play a partial role in blood UA levels and cognitive function [43]. Studies have also demonstrated that people with low blood UA levels tend to have poorer nutritional status [44, 45], which increases the risk of cognitive impairment [46]. Furthermore, although "one-third" of the total amount of urate in the body comes from dietary purines [47], we cannot ignore the influence of dietary patterns on UA levels. Dietary interventions are considered important for gout prevention [48]. A meta-analysis showed that plant-based dietary patterns were associated with lower UA levels, while animal-based dietary patterns were associated with higher UA levels [49]. Further studies have shown that in populations with normal UA levels, UA concentrations in short- and long-term dieters remain within the normal range even when only plant-based dietary patterns are used [50]. However, due to the wide variation in dietary habits among people born in different countries, geographic regions, and generations, as well as changes in dietary habits during long-term follow-up, most of the included studies lacked data on diet and UA levels, which could be further analyzed in the future by refining the study protocols.

Second, deteriorating health and death are the main hallmarks of the aging process [51]. A potential explanation might be that survival bias reduced the likelihood of people with different levels of blood UA surviving into later life, both inhibiting the development of cognitive impairments and affecting the true assessment of cognitive characteristics in later life. Most of our studies did not consider the character of survival bias in the development of cognitive impairment, which brought bias to the research results.

Third, blood UA levels might modulate cognitive function in a different way. UA has been shown to be a catalyst for human intellectual development has been proven [52], which is due to its anti-oxidant properties and potential neuroprotection [9, 53]. Previous studies have indicated that UA has both oxidation and reduction properties based on differences in the physicochemical environment [11, 54]. UA not only plays a role in oxidative stress as a bioactive pro-inflammatory factor, but also protects neurons under certain conditions and exhibits antioxidant properties [5557]. Pathological changes in neurodegeneration and cerebrovascular injuries play a synergistic or additive role in the course of cognitive impairment [58, 59]. Several studies, including observational studies and animal experiments, have illustrated that the progression of cognitive impairment is closely linked to oxidative stress and inflammatory responses [5963], and that blood UA levels are associated with these mechanisms [10, 6466]. Redox homeostasis is important to maintain normal cellular physiological function [67, 68]. One possible interpretation is that physiological levels of blood UA plays a neuroprotective role in the development of cognitive disorders; however, when UA values are elevated to a certain range and remain high for a long time, the neuroprotective effect might be obscured by the increased vascular risk associated with high blood UA levels.

Fourth, we assessed baseline UA levels and cognitive impairment correlations. Most studies have only measured baseline UA levels, and the role of UA might depend on the duration of its levels. Changes in UA levels occur over the course of the prolonged follow-up [69], and chronically higher UA levels could be associated with cognitive decline [39]. Recently, a community-based cohort study suggested that the interaction between genotype of apolipoprotein E4 and additional genetic risk factors modified the risks of dementia [70]. Similarly, Lee and colleagues found that higher serum UA may interact with apolipoprotein E4 to alleviate cognitive decline in female patients with MCI [71]. The influence of genetic factors in the incidence of cognitive impairment should be further explored.

Furthermore, various cognitive domains may be affected by alterations in UA levels [16, 72]. Huang et al found that low levels (within normal range) of plasma UA had a potentially detrimental effect on cognitive function in the executive domain in adults without hyperuricemia [15]. Yuan et al showed that lower UA levels were associated with poorer performance in situational memory and mental status [73]. A recent cross-sectional study showed that higher UA was associated with poorer performance in the domain of visual memory, but not in the domain of reaction speed [74]. However, some of the studies we included indicated that increased UA levels were correlated with enhanced memory function [30, 35]. We did not explore this further as the included studies were not subdivided regarding the association of UA with different cognitive domains.

Studies in men have shown a significant negative association between blood UA levels and cognitive impairment. Similar to our results, another prospective study suggests that the effect of blood UA levels on spontaneous brain activity and cognitive function varies by gender [75]. Sex hormones may play a vital role in this connection [76]. It is believed that the loss of estrogen plays a role in age-related cognitive changes in neurobiology [77, 78]. Cognitive function of women in a certain age interval may benefit from estrogen supplementation [79]. Higher plasma estrogen levels in women may lead to increased clearance of serum UA by the kidneys, weakening the protective effect of UA on cognition [78, 80]. The specific mechanism needs to be explored further. Our study also shows that the notable relationship vanished when restricted to studies with longer follow-up times. Due to the lack of survival data, a competing risk analysis could not be conducted, which may have biased the results. The power to detect the latent relationship could be increased with a larger sample size. Therefore, an analysis of more than 2000 participants may exhibit remarkable results.

4.4 Strengths and limitations

The strengths of the study should be acknowledged. First, a meta-analysis involving prospective cohort studies with long follow-up periods and large sample sizes was conducted, which enabled us to quantitatively evaluate the relationship between blood UA levels and cognitive impairment. Second, we included studies with fully adjusted models to decrease confounding bias. Third, a dose-response analysis was conducted to explore the potential nonlinear relations.

Our findings also need to be interpreted by considering several limitations. First, causality cannot be established, owing to the natural defection of observational studies. Second, although we brought the fully adjusted models into the analysis, the residual confounders cannot be completely ignored. Third, most of the studies provided baseline blood UA levels. However, the concentration of blood UA may change during the long follow-up time. Further reports with repeated blood UA levels are warranted. Lastly, the significant relationship observed in the subgroup analyses should be explained carefully and verified in future studies.

5 Conclusion

We found that cognitive impairment did not appear to be correlated with blood UA levels. Higher blood UA levels are related to a decline in cognitive impairment in males. A larger sample size, longer follow-up time and dynamic observation of blood UA levels are needed to confirm these findings. Future directions: 1) Studies should be conducted in hyperuricemia, normal UA levels and hypouricemia populations separately to better quantify the effect of UA levels on cognitive function and to provide guidelines for the management of UA in different populations; 2) To Investigate the effects of UA on different cognitive domains; 3) Large prospective cohort studies with long-term follow-up should be conducted in order to eliminate as much as possible the effects of residual confounders such as dietary patterns, nutritional status and survival data.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the researchers who conducted the original studies included in this meta-analysis.

References

  1. 1. Arvanitakis Z, Shah RC, Bennett DA. Diagnosis and Management of Dementia: Review. JAMA. 2019;322(16):1589–99. pmid:31638686
  2. 2. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. The Lancet. 2020;396(10248):413–46. pmid:32738937
  3. 3. Risk Reduction of Cognitive Decline and Dementia: WHO Guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2019.
  4. 4. Therriault J, Zimmer ER, Benedet AL, Pascoal TA, Gauthier S, Rosa-Neto P. Staging of Alzheimer’s disease: past, present, and future perspectives. Trends Mol Med. 2022;28(9):726–41. pmid:35717526
  5. 5. Petersen RC, Lopez O, Armstrong MJ, Getchius TSD, Ganguli M, Gloss D, et al. Practice guideline update summary: Mild cognitive impairment: Report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination, and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2018;90(3):126–35. pmid:29282327
  6. 6. Gauthier S WC, Servaes S, Morais JA, Rosa-Neto P. World Alzheimer Report 2022: Life after diagnosis: Navigating treatment, care and support. Alzheimer’s Disease International. London, England: 2022. p. 25.
  7. 7. Chowdhary N, Barbui C, Anstey KJ, Kivipelto M, Barbera M, Peters R, et al. Reducing the Risk of Cognitive Decline and Dementia: WHO Recommendations. Front Neurol. 2021;12:765584. pmid:35082745
  8. 8. Zlokovic BV, Gottesman RF, Bernstein KE, Seshadri S, McKee A, Snyder H, et al. Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia (VCID): A report from the 2018 National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Workshop. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(12):1714–33. pmid:33030307
  9. 9. Mijailovic NR, Vesic K, Borovcanin MM. The Influence of Serum Uric Acid on the Brain and Cognitive Dysfunction. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2022;13. pmid:35530021
  10. 10. Tang X, Song Z-H, Cardoso MA, Zhou J-B, Simó R. The relationship between uric acid and brain health from observational studies. Metabolic Brain Disease. 2022. pmid:35670991
  11. 11. Verhaaren BFJ, Vernooij MW, Dehghan A, Vrooman HA, de Boer R, Hofman A, et al. The relation of uric acid to brain atrophy and cognition: the Rotterdam Scan Study. Neuroepidemiology. 2013;41(1):29–34. pmid:23548762
  12. 12. Boccardi V, Carino S, Marinelli E, Lapenna M, Caironi G, Bianco AR, et al. Uric acid and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease: results from the ReGAl 2.0 project. Aging clinical and experimental research. 2021;33(2):361–6. pmid:32277437
  13. 13. Fatima T, Jacobsson LTH, Kern S, Zettergren A, Blennow K, Zetterberg H, et al. Association between serum urate and CSF markers of Alzheimer’s disease pathology in a population-based sample of 70-year-olds. Alzheimer’s & dementia (Amsterdam, Netherlands). 2021;13(1):e12241. pmid:34934798
  14. 14. Tana C, Ticinesi A, Prati B, Nouvenne A, Meschi T. Uric Acid and Cognitive Function in Older Individuals. Nutrients. 2018;10(8):975. pmid:30060474
  15. 15. Huang Y, Zhang S, Shen J, Yang J, Chen X, Li W, et al. Association of plasma uric acid levels with cognitive function among non-hyperuricemia adults: A prospective study. Clinical Nutrition. 2022;41(3):645–52. pmid:35131717
  16. 16. Geng R, Zhang Y, Liu M, Deng S, Ding J, Zhong H, et al. Elevated serum uric acid is associated with cognitive improvement in older American adults: A large, population-based-analysis of the NHANES database. Frontiers in aging neuroscience. 2022;14:1024415. pmid:36570535
  17. 17. Li Q, Cen K, Cui Y, Feng X, Hou X. Uric acid levels and their association with vascular dementia and Parkinson’s disease dementia: a meta-analysis. Neurological sciences: official journal of the Italian Neurological Society and of the Italian Society of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2023. pmid:36690824
  18. 18. Au B, Dale-McGrath S, Tierney MC. Sex differences in the prevalence and incidence of mild cognitive impairment: A meta-analysis. Ageing research reviews. 2017;35:176–99. pmid:27771474
  19. 19. Zhou Z, Zhong S, Liang Y, Zhang X, Zhang R, Kang K, et al. Serum Uric Acid and the Risk of Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2021;13. pmid:33716713
  20. 20. Du N, Xu D, Hou X, Song X, Liu C, Chen Y, et al. Inverse Association Between Serum Uric Acid Levels and Alzheimer’s Disease Risk. Molecular neurobiology. 2016;53(4):2594–9. pmid:26084440
  21. 21. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 2021;372:n71. pmid:33782057
  22. 22. Wells G, editor The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Non-Randomised Studies in Meta-Analyses. Symposium on Systematic Reviews: Beyond the Basics; 2014.
  23. 23. Rong Y, Chen L, Zhu T, Song Y, Yu M, Shan Z, et al. Egg consumption and risk of coronary heart disease and stroke: dose-response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ. 2013;346(jan07 2):e8539–e. pmid:23295181
  24. 24. Greenland S, Longnecker MP. Methods for trend estimation from summarized dose-response data, with applications to meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;135(11):1301–9. pmid:1626547
  25. 25. Orsini N, Bellocco R, Greenland S. Generalized least squares for trend estimation of summarized dose–response data. Stata Journal. 2006;6(6):40–57.
  26. 26. Yuan H, Yu C, Li X, Sun L, Zhu X, Zhao C, et al. Serum Uric Acid Levels and Risk of Metabolic Syndrome: A Dose-Response Meta-Analysis of Prospective Studies. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2015;100(11):4198–207. pmid:26308292
  27. 27. Bekkering GE, Harris RJ, Thomas S, Mayer A-MB, Beynon R, Ness AR, et al. How Much of the Data Published in Observational Studies of the Association between Diet and Prostate or Bladder Cancer Is Usable for Meta-Analysis? American Journal of Epidemiology. 2008;167(9):1017–26. pmid:18403406
  28. 28. Chêne G, Thompson SG. Methods for summarizing the risk associations of quantitative variables in epidemiologic studies in a consistent form. American journal of epidemiology. 1996;144(6):610–21. pmid:8797521
  29. 29. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557–60. pmid:12958120
  30. 30. Euser SM, Hofman A, Westendorp RGJ, Breteler MMB. Serum uric acid and cognitive function and dementia. Brain: a journal of neurology. 2009;132(Pt 2):377–82. pmid:19036766
  31. 31. Cao Z, Xu C, Yang H, Li S, Xu F, Zhang Y, et al. Associations of BMI and Serum Urate with Developing Dementia: A Prospective Cohort Study. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2020;105(12):e4688–e98. pmid:32918088
  32. 32. Scheepers LEJM, Jacobsson LTH, Kern S, Johansson L, Dehlin M, Skoog I. Urate and risk of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: A population‐based study. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2019;15(6):754–63. pmid:31056343
  33. 33. Latourte A, Soumaré A, Bardin T, Perez-Ruiz F, Debette S, Richette P. Uric acid and incident dementia over 12 years of follow-up: a population-based cohort study. Annals of the Rheumatic Diseases. 2018;77(3):328–35. pmid:28754803
  34. 34. Chen C, Li X, Lv Y, Yin Z, Zhao F, Liu Y, et al. High Blood Uric Acid Is Associated With Reduced Risks of Mild Cognitive Impairment Among Older Adults in China: A 9-Year Prospective Cohort Study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2021;13. pmid:34720995
  35. 35. Alam AB, Wu A, Power MC, West NA, Alonso A. Associations of serum uric acid with incident dementia and cognitive decline in the ARIC-NCS cohort. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2020;414:116866. pmid:32387846
  36. 36. Dalbeth N, Gosling AL, Gaffo A, Abhishek A. Gout. Lancet (London, England). 2021;397(10287):1843–55. pmid:33798500
  37. 37. Otani N, Ouchi M, Misawa K, Hisatome I, Anzai N. Hypouricemia and Urate Transporters. Biomedicines. 2022;10(3). pmid:35327453
  38. 38. Min KH, Kang SO, Oh SJ, Han JM, Lee KE. Association Between Gout and Dementia in the Elderly: A Nationwide Population-Based Cohort Study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2021;29(12):1177–85. pmid:33593591
  39. 39. Wang J, Jin R, Wu Z, Liu Y, Jin X, Han Z, et al. Moderate increase of serum uric acid within a normal range is associated with improved cognitive function in a non-normotensive population: A nationally representative cohort study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:944341. pmid:36118682
  40. 40. Liu Q, Liao X, Pan Y, Jin A, Zhang Y. Association Between Serum Uric Acid Levels and Cognitive Function in Patients with Ischemic Stroke and Transient Ischemic Attack (TIA): A 3-Month Follow-Up Study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2021;17:991–9. pmid:33833515
  41. 41. Li Z, Meng L, Huang Z, Cui L, Li W, Gao J, et al. Ideal Cardiovascular Health Metrics and Incident Hyperuricemia. Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken). 2016;68(5):660–6. pmid:26714267
  42. 42. Leiros M, Amenedo E, Rodriguez M, Pazo-Alvarez P, Franco L, Leis R, et al. Cognitive Status and Nutritional Markers in a Sample of Institutionalized Elderly People. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:880405. pmid:35686024
  43. 43. Rullier L, Lagarde A, Bouisson J, Bergua V, Barberger-Gateau P. Nutritional status of community-dwelling older people with dementia: associations with individual and family caregivers’ characteristics. International journal of geriatric psychiatry. 2013;28(6):580–8. pmid:22821728
  44. 44. D’Silva KM, Yokose C, Lu N, McCormick N, Lee H, Zhang Y, et al. Hypouricemia and Mortality Risk in the US General Population. Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken). 2021;73(8):1171–9. pmid:33026684
  45. 45. Cho SK, Chang Y, Kim I, Ryu S. U-Shaped Association Between Serum Uric Acid Level and Risk of Mortality: A Cohort Study. Arthritis Rheumatol. 2018;70(7):1122–32. pmid:29694719
  46. 46. Zamroziewicz MK, Barbey AK. Nutritional Cognitive Neuroscience: Innovations for Healthy Brain Aging. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2016;10:240. pmid:27375409
  47. 47. Schlesinger N. Dietary factors and hyperuricaemia. Curr Pharm Des. 2005;11(32):4133–8. pmid:16375734
  48. 48. Nie J, Deng M-G, Wang K, Liu F, Xu H, Feng Q, et al. Higher HEI-2015 scores are associated with lower risk of gout and hyperuricemia: Results from the national health and nutrition examination survey 2007–2016. Frontiers in Nutrition. 2022;9. pmid:35990332
  49. 49. Cheng S, Shan L, You Z, Xia Y, Zhao Y, Zhang H, et al. Dietary patterns, uric acid levels, and hyperuricemia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Food & Function. 2023;14(17):7853–68. pmid:37599588
  50. 50. Jakse B, Jakse B, Pajek M, Pajek J. Uric Acid and Plant-Based Nutrition. Nutrients. 2019;11(8). pmid:31357560
  51. 51. Zhang H, Cherian R, Jin K. Systemic milieu and age-related deterioration. GeroScience. 2019;41(3):275–84. pmid:31152364
  52. 52. Orowan E. The origin of man. Nature. 1955;175(4459):683–4. pmid:14370192
  53. 53. Pellecchia MT, Savastano R, Moccia M, Picillo M, Siano P, Erro R, et al. Lower serum uric acid is associated with mild cognitive impairment in early. Journal of neural transmission (Vienna, Austria: 1996). 2016;123(12):1399–402. pmid:27682634
  54. 54. So A, Thorens B. Uric acid transport and disease. J Clin Invest. 2010;120(6):1791–9. pmid:20516647
  55. 55. Tamagno E, Guglielmotto M, Vasciaveo V, Tabaton M. Oxidative Stress and Beta Amyloid in Alzheimer’s Disease. Which Comes First: The Chicken or the Egg? Antioxidants (Basel). 2021;10(9). pmid:34573112
  56. 56. Sautin YY, Johnson RJ. Uric acid: the oxidant-antioxidant paradox. Nucleosides Nucleotides Nucleic Acids. 2008;27(6):608–19. pmid:18600514
  57. 57. Chouraki V, Preis SR, Yang Q, Beiser A, Li S, Larson MG, et al. Association of amine biomarkers with incident dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in the Framingham Study. Alzheimer’s & dementia: the journal of the Alzheimer’s Association. 2017;13(12):1327–36. pmid:28602601
  58. 58. Rundek T, Tolea M, Ariko T, Fagerli EA, Camargo CJ. Vascular Cognitive Impairment (VCI). Neurotherapeutics. 2021. pmid:34939171
  59. 59. Iadecola C. The pathobiology of vascular dementia. Neuron. 2013;80(4):844–66. pmid:24267647
  60. 60. Butterfield DA, Halliwell B. Oxidative stress, dysfunctional glucose metabolism and Alzheimer disease. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2019;20(3):148–60. pmid:30737462
  61. 61. Scheltens P, De Strooper B, Kivipelto M, Holstege H, Chetelat G, Teunissen CE, et al. Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet. 2021;397(10284):1577–90. pmid:33667416
  62. 62. Cecerska-Heryc E, Polikowska A, Serwin N, Roszak M, Grygorcewicz B, Heryc R, et al. Importance of oxidative stress in the pathogenesis, diagnosis, and monitoring of patients with neuropsychiatric disorders, a review. Neurochem Int. 2022;153:105269. pmid:34971747
  63. 63. Costantino I, Marco D, Vladimir H, Anne J, Sarah TP, Julie AS, et al. Vascular Cognitive Impairment and Dementia. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2019;73(25).
  64. 64. Latourte A, Dumurgier J, Paquet C, Richette P. Hyperuricemia, Gout, and the Brain-an Update. Curr Rheumatol Rep. 2021;23(12):82. pmid:34971414
  65. 65. Molshatzki N, Weinstein G, Streifler JY, Goldbourt U, Tanne D. Serum uric acid and subsequent cognitive performance in patients with pre-existing. PloS one. 2015;10(3):e0120862. pmid:25794156
  66. 66. Tian T, Liu XR, Li TT, Nie ZC, Li SJ, Tang Y, et al. Detrimental effects of long-term elevated serum uric acid on cognitive function in rats. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):6732. pmid:33762656
  67. 67. Jones DP, Sies H. The Redox Code. Antioxidants & redox signaling. 2015;23(9). pmid:25891126
  68. 68. Huang JH, Co HK, Lee YC, Wu CC, Chen SH. Multistability maintains redox homeostasis in human cells. Mol Syst Biol. 2021;17(10):e10480. pmid:34612597
  69. 69. Zhang S, Ma Z, Li Q, Liu J, Tao L, Han Y, et al. Dose-response relationship between distinct serum uric acid trajectories and metabolic syndrome risk: A 5-year prospective cohort study. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis. 2021;31(4):1189–99. Epub 2021/02/08. pmid:33549428.
  70. 70. van der Lee SJ, Wolters FJ, Ikram MK, Hofman A, Ikram MA, Amin N, et al. The effect of APOE and other common genetic variants on the onset of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia: a community-based cohort study. The Lancet Neurology. 2018;17(5):434–44. pmid:29555425
  71. 71. Lee Y-g, Park M, Jeong SH, Kang SW, Baik K, Jung JH, et al. Effects of baseline serum uric acid and apolipoprotein E4 on longitudinal cognition and cerebral metabolism. Neurobiology of Aging. 2021;106:223–31. pmid:34311431
  72. 72. Beydoun MA, Canas J-A, Dore GA, Beydoun HA, Rostant OS, Fanelli-Kuczmarski MT, et al. Serum Uric Acid and Its Association with Longitudinal Cognitive Change Among Urban Adults. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2016;52(4):1415–30. pmid:27104899
  73. 73. Yuan Z, Liu H, Zhou R, Gu S, Wu K, Huang Z, et al. Association of serum uric acid and fasting plasma glucose with cognitive function: a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatrics. 2023;23(1). pmid:37142950
  74. 74. Khaled Y, Abdelhamid AA, Al-Mazroey H, Almannai AK, Fetais S, Al-Srami AS, et al. Higher serum uric acid is associated with poorer cognitive performance in healthy middle-aged people: a cross-sectional study. Internal and Emergency Medicine. 2023;18(6):1701–9. pmid:37330420
  75. 75. Li L, Li JZ, U JS, U JS, Akos V-S, Rock HS, et al. Uric Acid Has Different Effects on Spontaneous Brain Activities of Males and Females: A Cross-Sectional Resting-State Functional MR Imaging Study. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2019;13.
  76. 76. Newhouse P, Dumas J. Estrogen-cholinergic interactions: Implications for cognitive aging. Hormones and behavior. 2015;74:173–85. pmid:26187712
  77. 77. Albert KM, Newhouse PA. Estrogen, Stress, and Depression: Cognitive and Biological Interactions. Annual review of clinical psychology. 2019;15:399–423. pmid:30786242
  78. 78. Dumas JA, Kutz AM, Naylor MR, Johnson JV, Newhouse PA. Estradiol treatment altered anticholinergic-related brain activation during working memory in postmenopausal women. NeuroImage. 2012;60(2):1394–403. pmid:22266175
  79. 79. Dumas J, Hancur-Bucci C, Naylor M, Sites C, Newhouse P. Estradiol interacts with the cholinergic system to affect verbal memory in postmenopausal women: evidence for the critical period hypothesis. Hormones & Behavior. 2008;53(1):159–69. pmid:17964576
  80. 80. Sumino H, Ichikawa S, Kanda T, Nakamura T, Sakamaki T. Reduction of serum uric acid by hormone replacement therapy in postmenopausal women with hyperuricaemia (letter). The Lancet. 1999;(9179):354.