Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sex differences in the immediate extinction deficit and renewal of extinguished fear in rats

  • Annalise N. Binette ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Annalise N. Binette, Michael S. Totty

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and Institute for Neuroscience, Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas, United States of America

  • Michael S. Totty ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Annalise N. Binette, Michael S. Totty

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and Institute for Neuroscience, Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas, United States of America

  • Stephen Maren

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    maren@tamu.edu

    Affiliation Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and Institute for Neuroscience, Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas, United States of America

Abstract

Extinction learning is central to exposure-based behavioral therapies for reducing fear and anxiety in humans. However, patients with fear and anxiety disorders are often resistant to extinction. Moreover, trauma and stress-related disorders are highly prone to relapse and are twice as likely to occur in females compared to males, suggesting that females may be more susceptible to extinction deficits and fear relapse phenomena. In this report, we tested this hypothesis by examining sex differences in a stress-induced extinction learning impairment, the immediate extinction deficit (IED), and renewal, a common form of fear relapse. In contrast to our hypothesis, there were no sex differences in the magnitude of the immediate extinction deficit in two different rat strains (Long-Evans and Wistar). However, we did observe a sex difference in the renewal of fear when the extinguished conditioned stimulus was presented outside the extinction context. Male Wistar rats exhibited significantly greater renewal than female rats, a sex difference that has previously been reported after appetitive extinction. Collectively, these data reveal that stress-induced extinction impairments are similar in male and female rats, though the context-dependence of extinction is more pronounced in males.

Introduction

Clinical disorders caused by trauma exposure (e.g., post-traumatic stress disorder, PTSD) afflict millions of men and women in the United States every year [1]. Importantly, women are twice as likely as men to develop PTSD, suggesting that biological sex may influence the neural and behavioral processes underlying the pathophysiology of stress- and trauma-related disorders [2]. Consistent with this, a substantial body of work has revealed sex differences in learning and memory processes that may contribute to the development and maintenance of PTSD. For example, there are robust sex differences in Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats [36]. In this form of learning, a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS), such as an acoustic tone, is arranged to precede and predict a noxious unconditioned stimulus (US), such as an electric footshock. After as little as a single conditioning trial, presentation of the CS (or placement in the conditioning context) elicits anticipatory, conditioned fear responses (CRs), including freezing behavior [710]. Several studies have revealed that male rats exhibit greater acquisition of conditioned freezing to the environmental stimuli associated with shock (i.e., the conditioning context) relative to females [5,1116], whereas freezing to discrete auditory CSs is similar in males and females [5,16,17]. Interestingly, there are also sex differences in hippocampal and amygdala synaptic plasticity mechanisms thought to underlie these forms of learning [5,18]. These results suggest that sex differences in the neural and behavioral mechanisms of aversive learning and memory may contribute to the pathophysiology of PTSD.

Another form of learning implicated in the maintenance of PTSD is extinction. Extinction is a form of learning in which repeated non-reinforced presentations of a conditioned stimulus (CS) reduce the magnitude reduction of the CRs acquired during Pavlovian conditioning [19,20]. Importantly, extinction is central to cognitive-behavioral therapies for PTSD, including prolonged exposure therapy [21]. In prolonged exposure therapy, trauma-related stimuli are repeatedly presented within a safe setting until those stimuli no longer elicit fear. Although extinction-based therapy can be highly effective at reducing pathological fear, the durability of extinction memory can be compromised by a variety of factors [22,23]. For instance, suppression of a CR after extinction is typically limited to the setting or context in which extinction learning occurred, resulting in renewal of the CR outside of the extinction context [24]. Furthermore, stress (acute or chronic) can considerably reduce extinction learning and long-term retention, thereby fear relapse [25]. For example, the “immediate extinction deficit” (IED) occurs when extinction training is conducted shortly after fear conditioning (a stressor), rendering impairments in long-term extinction memory and relapse of conditioned fear [26,27]. Interestingly, immediate extinction occurs against the background of high levels of generalized contextual fear, which has been proposed to play an important role in the IED [27] (though see [28]).

Although extinction learning plays a central role in behavioral therapies for trauma- and stressor-related disorders, the interaction of sex differences in contextual fear conditioning, on the one hand, with extinction learning and memory, on the other, is unclear. In addition to sex differences in contextual fear conditioning, there is considerable evidence that stress-related neural circuitry in females are more sensitive to those in males [2932]. These sex differences might make females more susceptible to stress-induced extinction learning impairments, such as the IED. Moreover, sex differences in contextual conditioning and stress responsivity might influence the relapse of extinguished fear, including renewal. Therefore, in this study, we characterized the IED in male and female rats using two common laboratory rat strains (Long Evans and Wistar) and standard Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction procedures. Surprisingly, we found that there was no sex difference in the IED in either rat strain; both male and female rats exhibited poor extinction retention relative to animals undergoing delayed extinction and exhibited similar levels of conditioned freezing compared with non-extinguished controls. In contrast, male Wistar rats showed greater renewal of fear to an extinguished CS in a novel context compared to females. Sex differences in renewal were not due to greater contextual fear in male rats. These results reveal that male and female rats are similarly susceptible to stress-induced extinction impairments, but that males may be more susceptible to fear renewal.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

This study was carried out in strict accordance with the recommendations of the National Institutes of Health and Texas A&M University. These experiments were approved by the Texas A&M University Animal Care and Use Committee (Animal Use Protocol Number: 2020–0305).

Subjects

A total of 64 adult female and male Long-Evans and Wistar rats were used in this study. Long-Evans Blue Spruce rats (male, n = 16; female, n = 16) were obtained from Envigo (Indianapolis, IN) and weighed 200–224 g upon arrival. Wistar rats (male, n = 16; female, n = 16) were bred in-house; they were 11–16 weeks of age upon behavioral testing. The Wistar rats used in this study were derived from Crh-Cre rats [33] obtained from the laboratory of Robert Messing at the University of Texas at Austin. These rats were bred in our laboratory with commercially supplied wild-type Wistar rats (Envigo, Indianapolis, IN). The Wistar rats were genotyped and randomly assigned to groups (extinction type) within each litter; both Cre+ and Crerats were included in the experiments and their behavior was identical. All rats were individually housed in a temperature- and humidity-controlled vivarium, with a 14:10 hour light/dark cycle and ad libitum access to food and water. Behavioral testing was conducted during the light phase. Rats were handled 1 minute per day for 5 days prior to testing to acclimate them to the experimenter.

Procedure

All behavioral procedures took place in a standard rodent conditioning chamber with two aluminum walls, two Plexiglas walls and a Plexiglas ceiling (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT). The chamber was outfitted with a speaker mounted to the upper corner of one wall for delivery of auditory stimuli. The grid floor was composed of stainless-steel rods for delivery of scrambled footshock. Load-cell force transducers located underneath each chamber measured displacement of the chamber in response to motor activity; these voltages (+/-10 V) were acquired at 5 Hz and transformed to absolute values (scale of 0–100). A freezing bout was defined as five consecutive values below 10 (freezing threshold, corresponding to one second of freezing).

To assess the IED, we conducted separate experiments in Long-Evans and Wistar rats. In the first experiment, male and female Long-Evans rats underwent either immediate or delayed extinction after auditory fear conditioning. Extinction was followed by a retention test conducted 24 hours later. All the behavioral procedures were conducted in the same context (Context A). Animals were transported from the vivarium to the laboratory in black plastic boxes. A metal pan beneath the grid floor of each conditioning chamber was cleaned with a 1% ammonium solution. The room housing the conditioning chambers was illuminated with red light and fans affixed to each chamber were turned on. Doors of the outer sound attenuating cabinets were closed. Conditioning consisted of a 3-min baseline period followed by 5 CS-US pairings. The CS was a 10-s white noise (80 dB) paired with a 2-s, 1-mA footshock unconditioned stimulus (US). The intertrial interval (ITI) was 70 seconds. After conditioning, animals were placed in the transport boxes and returned to the vivarium unless undergoing immediate extinction, in which case they remained in the transport boxes until the immediate extinction session began. Either 15-min or 24-hr later, the animals were returned to the conditioning chambers for immediate or delayed extinction, respectively. Extinction consisted of a 3-min baseline period followed by 45 CS-alone presentations (40-s ITI). All animals were returned to the chamber again 48-hr after conditioning for a retrieval test consisting of a second extinction session.

In the second experiment, we examined the IED in Wistar rats. Because the IED has not previously been studied in Wistar rats, we used a no-extinction control similar to our previously published work in Long-Evans rats [26]. The conditioning procedures were identical to those used for the Long-Evans rats, except for the use of a stronger footshock US (2 mA). After conditioning, animals received either an immediate or delayed extinction procedure or were simply placed in the conditioning chambers (no-extinction control); extinction retrieval testing was conducted 48 hours after. Conditioning and extinction procedures were conducted in Context A. After the retrieval test, all animals underwent a second extinction session (re-extinction) then were randomly assigned to a retrieval test (5 CS-only presentations) in either Context A (SAME, the extinction context) or Context B (DIFF, a novel context). Animals tested in Context B were transported from the vivarium to the laboratory in white transport boxes. For Context B, the conditioning chambers had a metal pan beneath the grid floor that was cleaned with a 3% acetic acid solution, and the room was illuminated with standard white light. Fans affixed to the sound attenuating cabinets were turned off and doors to the cabinets were left open.

Statistics

Data were analyzed with StatView software (SAS Institute). One female Long-Evans rat failed to acquire fear during conditioning due to technical difficulties and was thus excluded from statistical analyses. Results are displayed as mean ± standard error the mean (SEM). Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to assess percentage of time freezing with repeated measures of trial (⍺ = 0.05).

Results

Both male and female rats display the immediate extinction deficit

We first sought to determine if both male and female rats are similarly susceptible to the IED, a stress-induced extinction impairment (Fig 1A). To this end, Long-Evans rats underwent standard auditory fear conditioning which consisted of 5 CS-footshock (US) pairings. All rats acquired fear to the CS [main effect of Trials: F5, 135 = 99.8, p < .0001] and there were no differences between males and females [no main effect of Sex: F1, 27 = 0.60, p = 0.45; Trials x Sex interaction: F5, 135 = 0.46, p = .81] (Fig 1B; Conditioning). Rats next underwent fear extinction either 15 minutes (Immediate) or 24 hours (Delayed) later in the same context, which consisted of 45 CS-alone trials. We found that rats extinguished immediately following fear conditioning showed impaired within-session extinction compared to animals extinguished 24 hours later [main effect of Ext Type: F1, 27 = 6.21, p = 0.02; Block x Ext Type interaction: F9, 243 = 2.87, p = 0.003] (Fig 1B; Extinction). Forty-eight hours after conditioning, all animals were brought back to the extinction context and tested for extinction memory with another 45 CS-alone trials. During retrieval, both groups froze at similar levels to the first five trials, however, following this the Delayed animals reduced their freezing, demonstrating good extinction memory, while high freezing in the Immediate group persisted [Block x Ext Type interaction: F8, 216 = 2.90, p = .004] (Fig 1B; Retrieval). Hence, the IED was manifest as greater re-extinction (savings) in the animals that underwent delayed extinction compared to those undergoing immediate extinction as we have previously reported [34]. Importantly, although females showed a slightly faster reduction in freezing compared to males [F8, 216 = 1.72, p = 0.09], the immediate extinction procedure produced a similar deficit in extinction retention in both male and female rats [Block x Ext Type x Sex interaction: F8, 216 = 0.274, p = 0.97]. These data suggest that male and female Long-Evans rats are similarly susceptible to the immediate extinction deficit.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Male () and female () Long-Evans rats exhibit the IED.

(A) Behavioral timeline for the different experimental groups. (B) Freezing data show that Long-Evans rats acquired equivalent levels of conditional fear to the auditory CS (Conditioning). Both Immediate and Delayed groups showed a marked reduction in fear to the CS throughout fear extinction, with Delayed animals showing lower levels of fear at the end of extinction training (Extinction). Although all groups of animals showed equivalent levels of fear early in the retrieval test (Block 1), immediately extinguished animals demonstrated a reduced rate of re-extinction compared to Delayed animals, indicative of impaired extinction memory (Retrieval). This IED was similar between male and females, though, females showed faster rate of re-extinction compared to males in both groups. All error bars represent ± SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264797.g001

There are some inconsistencies in the reported findings of sex differences in learning and memory paradigms and it has been suggested that some of these findings may be strain specific [35]. We thus sought to replicate the above findings using a different strain of rats, while adding control groups that do not undergo fear extinction (Fig 2A). Equal numbers of Cre+ and Cremale and female Wistar rats (see the Materials and Methods section for more detail) were used for the following experiments. Throughout all statistical comparisons, we observed no significant effects of genotype and thus chose to collapse data across genotype to improve statistical power. All rats were first conditioned as previously described and all rats developed conditional responding to the CS [main effect of Trial: F5, 140 = 50.24, p < 0.0001] (Fig 2B). Female rats showed slightly higher freezing compared to males [main effect of Sex: F1, 28 = 8.65, p < 0.0065] and, although Immediate and Delayed groups displayed slightly different learning curves [Trial x Ext Type interaction: F5, 140 = 5.01, p = 0.0003], they displayed equivalent total amounts of freezing [no main effect of Ext Type: F1, 28 = 0.02, p = 0.90] (Fig 2B). Following conditioning, rats either underwent extinction 15 minutes (Immediate-Ext) or 24 hours (Delayed-Ext) after conditioning, or they were merely re-exposed to the context 15 minutes (Immediate-NoExt) or 24 later hours (Delayed-NoExt) (Fig 2A). Although female rats showed more freezing during conditioning, we did not observe any sex differences during extinction [no main effect of Sex: F1, 24 = 0.03, p = 0.86; Block x Sex interaction: F9, 216 = 0.61, p = 0.79; Ext Type x Sex interaction: F1, 24 = 0.61, p = 0.44; or Block x Ext Type x Sex interaction: F9, 216 = 0.76, p = 0.66].

thumbnail
Fig 2. Male () and female () Wistar rats exhibit an IED.

(A) Behavioral timeline for the different experimental groups. (B) All groups acquired similar levels of conditioned freezing prior to extinction. (C) Wistar rats that underwent extinction training showed a reduction in fear across extinction trials with immediately extinguished rats reaching lower levels of freezing than delayed animals. Despite this, immediate extinction animals still showed impaired extinction during retrieval testing as indicated by reduced rate of re-extinction. This effect was similar in both males and females. (D) Comparatively, No-Extinction animals exhibited high levels of conditioned freezing during retrieval testing that was similar among all groups. All error bars represent ± SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264797.g002

It is of note that Wistar rats displayed remarkably low baseline freezing during both the immediate and delayed extinction procedures (< 20%; Fig 2C) compared to Long-Evans rats in the previous experiment (~80%; Fig 1B). This appears to be a strain difference as similarly low levels of contextual fear have previously been reported in both Wistar [36] and Sprague-Dawley rats [37]. Nonetheless, Wistar rats exhibited high levels of CS-evoked freezing at the outset of extinction training compared to control animals that were merely exposed to the context [main effect of Ext: F1, 24 = 68.38, p < 0.0001; Block x Ext interaction: F9, 216 = 17.21, p < 0.0001] (Fig 2C and 2D). We additionally observed higher levels of contextual fear in male rats that were exposed to the conditioning context 24 hours after conditioning [Block x Sex x Ext-type interaction: F9, 108 = 2.562, p = 0.0104; Fischer’s PLSD: p = .036].

Like the previous experiment, all groups showed equally high freezing during the first 5 trials of retrieval testing; however, delayed extinction animals exhibited a clear decrease in freezing in the second block of 5 trials whereas high freezing persisted in all other groups [Block x Ext x Ext Type interaction: F8,192 = 3.50, p = 0.0009] (Fig 2C and 2D). This effectively demonstrates that undergoing extinction training immediately following conditioning impairs extinction retention to that of animals that never underwent extinction at all. Importantly, we once again show no sex differences in the IED when comparing across extinction groups [no Block x Ext x Ext Type x Sex interaction: F8,192 = 1.88, p = 0.065] or in any other comparison during retrieval testing (all p-values > .09). Collectively, we show that male and female rats, across strains, are equally susceptible to stress-induced extinction impairments.

Male, but not female, rats exhibit renewal of extinguished fear

Similar to stress-induced impairments in extinction learning, contextual processing is thought to be central to trauma-related disorders such as PTSD [38,39]. Previous work has demonstrated sex differences in the renewal of extinguished CRs after appetitive conditioning [40], however, it is currently unknown if this is true for aversively conditioned CSs. To test this, Wistar rats from the previous experiment underwent an additional extinction session to completely extinguish any remaining fear (Fig 3). The animals were then reassigned to new groups for renewal testing and were counterbalanced for previous group assignments. After the second extinction session, male and female rats were either placed back into the extinction context (A; retrieval context) or they were placed into a novel context (B; renewal context) where they were presented 5 CS-alone trials. During the second extinction session all rats showed moderate levels of fear during the first five-trial block but quickly reduced freezing to baseline levels [main effect of trials: F9,252 = 34.09, p < 0.0001], and there were no sex or group differences during extinction [Trials x Sex interaction: F9,252 = 1.579, p = 0.12; Trials x Context interaction: F9,252 = 1.169, p = 0.32; Trials x Sex x Context interaction: F9,252 = 0.654, p = 0.75] (Fig 3; Extinction II). During testing, both male and female rats placed back into the extinction context showed very low levels of freezing. However, males, but not females, placed into the novel context showed a renewal of conditioned freezing [Trials x Sex x Context interaction: F5,140 = 2.60, p = 0.028], particularly during the first two trials (Fig 3; Renewal Test). Importantly, these results cannot be explained by prior extinction group assignments as an analysis of the renewal data including this variable did not reveal significant Sex x Ext (all p > 0.4) or Sex by Ext-type interactions (all p > 0.3). These data demonstrate that male rats are more susceptible to the renewal of extinguished fear.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Male () but not female () Wistar rats exhibit renewal of extinguished fear.

Freezing data showing that male and female Wistar rats displayed similar levels of fear during a second extinction session after the prior extinction retrieval test (left). However, only male rats displayed fear renewal the following day when presented the extinguished CS in a novel context (DIFF, right) relative to animals tested in the extinction context (SAME). This was particularly evident during the first two trials of testing. All error bars represent ± SEM.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264797.g003

Discussion

In this study we investigated sex differences in failures of extinction learning and retrieval. We found that male and female rats are equally susceptible to the IED—a stress-induced extinction impairment—in two different strains of animals. Interestingly, we also show that females, but not males, exhibit impairments in the renewal of extinguished fear, suggestive of a sex difference in contextual processing. This finding is in line with previous work showing sex differences in the renewal of appetitive conditioning [40]. Our work extends previous literature and suggests that differences in contextual processing may be a critical factor accounting for the disparity in susceptibility for stress and trauma-related disorders across sexes.

Although both male and female rats show comparable deficits in immediate extinction, both male and female rats showed a more rapid (but non-significant) reduction in conditioned freezing during retrieval testing, regardless of the extinction procedure (immediate or delayed). It is unlikely that this rate difference is a consequence of sex differences in conditioning insofar as Long-Evans rats showed no sex differences in conditioning, whereas Wistar females showed greater levels of freezing during conditioning. A potential explanation for sex differences in the (re)extinction of fear is estrous cycle phase. For example, extinction is impaired in females that undergo extinction training during metestrus but enhanced in females trained in estrus or proestrus [6,41]. Ovarian steroids underlie the more rapid reduction in freezing during extinction retrieval in females [42,43], which was observed under both low- and high-stress conditions. This suggests that stress does not block the adaptive effects of high estrogen and progesterone on retrieval facilitation. From another perspective, however, changes in hormonal state across extinction training and retrieval testing in females might be expected to impair extinction retrieval and facilitate renewal because extinction retrieval is highly context-dependent [24]. Yet the opposite was true: female rats showed superior extinction retrieval and did not exhibit renewal (at least in Wistar rats). It would be important in future work to examine the role that estrous cycle and gonadal steroids may play in these processes.

Previous work has found that female rats display lower levels of contextual fear after auditory or contextual fear conditioning [5,1116]. In the present study, we did not observe sex differences in contextual freezing during the baseline periods of extinction sessions, which typically serves as a measure of contextual fear. However, in the immediate extinction procedure this baseline period is confounded by non-associative sensitization that summates with associative fear to the context [44]. In addition, contextual fear during the baseline period of the extinction retrieval test is confounded by context extinction (or lack thereof) experienced during the extinction training session. In Experiment 2, however, rats in the Delay-No-Extinction group were placed into the conditioning context (24 h after conditioning) for a ~35 min test with no CSs. In this test of contextual freezing, which was not confounded by sensitization, male Wistar rats exhibited reliably more freezing than female rats.

Given that the IED is a stress-induced deficit driven by the locus coeruleus (LC) [27,45,46], a sexually-dimorphic structure [29], it is perhaps surprising that we did not observe sex differences in the IED. Recent work has revealed that the IED is driven by LC-derived norepinephrine (LC-NE) which excites neurons in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) [45,47,48] and, in turn, impairs the infralimbic (IL) division of the medial prefrontal cortex [4951]. The IL is critical to the formation of long-term extinction memories [5254]. Although LC-NE release within the prefrontal cortex enhances arousal and is necessary for successful extinction learning [55], high levels of LC-NE may act to impair prefrontal function [45,56]. Substantial work has demonstrated that LC neurons in females compared to males are genetically distinct [57], have greater dendritic morphology [58], and are 10-30x more sensitive to activation by corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF), irrespective of cycling hormones [59]. Moreover, the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) is a large source of CRF input that is directly excitatory to LC-NE neurons [6064], and CeA CRF+ cells have recently been shown to be necessary and sufficient to drive the IED [65]. All of this would suggest that females should be highly susceptible to stress-induced extinction impairments. Nonetheless, it has also been found that prior stress sensitizes LC neurons in male rats [66,67], but not females, which abolishes sex differences in LC sensitivity [59]. Thus, we suggest that high levels of physiological stress (such as that from fear conditioning) may result in a ceiling effect such that male and female rats are equally susceptible to stress-induced impairments in extinction learning. Given this, it would be worthwhile to know if female rats are susceptible to the IED under conditions that are typically not stressful enough to cause extinction impairments in male rats, such as weaker shocks or fewer conditioning trials [45,68]. Although acute stress from fear conditioning in rodents does not appear to result in obvious sex differences in the IED procedure, it is still possible that the wide-range of sex differences in the LC-NE system plays a central role in the development of chronic stress and/or psychiatric disorder, such as PTSD, in humans.

Our finding that male, but not female, rats exhibit renewal of extinguished fear mirrors work in both appetitive and aversive conditioning [40,69]. Anderson and Petrovich (2015) investigated the renewal of food seeking using an ABA design in Long-Evans rats and found that males, but not females, renewed previously extinguished conditional responding to the food cup when tested outside of the extinction context [40]. They additionally found that renewal returned in ovariectomized female rats that received estradiol replacement throughout behavioral training and testing, but not in ovariectomized females without estradiol replacement. This suggests that levels of estradiol may play a critical role in renewal in female rats [40]. Contrary to this, Hilz and colleagues have shown that renewal in females only occurs when they are extinguished in the proestrous phase (high levels of estrogen) and tested in metestrous/diestrous phase (low levels of estrogen), suggesting that renewal in intact females relies on a shift in interoceptive hormonal state [70,71]. Further adding to this theory, Park and colleagues show that female rats display renewal at a juvenile age (P18) before hormone cycling begins [72,73]. In aversive conditioning, estrous cycle phase has been shown to modulate fear renewal in female rats. Female rats that undergo auditory fear extinction during metestrus and diestrus, but not in proestrus and estrus, show fear renewal to the CS in a novel context [69]. Taken together, we suggest that renewal in female rats depends heavily on both interoceptive and exteroceptive context.

Although we failed to observe renewal in female rats, previous reports have observed robust renewal in females [24,7476]. Indeed, Bouton and Bolles’ seminal findings found renewal in female Wistar rats [24]. Notably, however, they measured fear using a conditioned suppression procedure in which rats show a reduction in lever pressing for food during an aversive CS. Response competition in this test situation may influence sex difference in renewal. Nonetheless, others have shown that female rats exhibit renewal using freezing as the dependent variable [75,76], suggesting that this discrepancy is unlikely to be due to procedural differences. The higher levels of contextual freezing observed in the present experiment in male rats suggests that exteroceptive stimuli may have stronger control of their behavior, including the context-dependence of extinction. Collectively, there is still much work to be done to determine the factors that regulate sex differences in fear relapse after extinction.

Indeed, sex differences in brain areas critical for the context-dependence of extinction may underlie differences in renewal. The hippocampal formation encodes and transmits spatial information to various limbic regions, including the mPFC and amygdala, primarily through its ventral subregion (vHPC) that is critical for guiding contextually appropriate behaviors. The vHPC sends monosynaptic projections to the lateral and central nuclei of the amygdala [77,78], and a dense feedforward inhibitory circuit to the mPFC [79,80], each of which have been implicated in context-dependent fear renewal. Appetitive renewal similarly involves the activation of mPFC-projecting vHPC neurons [81], modulation of the mPFC [82,83], and amygdala activation [70,71,83]. Interestingly, sex differences in the recruitment of these neural circuits mirrored the behavioral sex differences seen in appetitive renewal [70,71,8183]. It seems likely that the failure to recruit these circuits when confronted with an extinguished CS outside of the extinction context underlies impaired renewal often observed in female rats.

In summary, we first show that both male and female rats are equally susceptible to the IED despite well-known sex differences in the neural circuits underlying the IED. We speculate that enhanced basal excitability of the LC-NE system in females may result in increased susceptibility to mild stressors compared to male rats, whereas strong acute stressors result in the sensitization of the male LC-NE system, abolishing potential sex differences in extinction learning. We next show that female rats do not display context-mediated fear renewal, similar to reports in appetitive literature. We argue that renewal of conditional responding in female rats may depend heavily on interoceptive, in addition to exteroceptive, contexts. Changes in interoceptive context are monitored by the insular cortex [84], and it’s possible that insular projections to the amygdala and para-hippocampal regions may gate the renewal of extinguished fear. Alternatively, cycling hormones such as estradiol may modulate the excitability of limbic circuits underlying renewal.

References

  1. 1. Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Wittchen H-U. Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2012;21: 169–184. pmid:22865617
  2. 2. Steel Z, Marnane C, Iranpour C, Chey T, Jackson JW, Patel V, et al. The global prevalence of common mental disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis 1980–2013. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43: 476–493. pmid:24648481
  3. 3. Dalla C, Shors TJ. Sex differences in learning processes of classical and operant conditioning. Physiol Behav. 2009;97: 229–238. pmid:19272397
  4. 4. Day HLL, Stevenson CW. The neurobiological basis of sex differences in learned fear and its inhibition. Eur J Neurosci. 2020;52: 2466–2486. pmid:31631413
  5. 5. Maren S, De Oca B, Fanselow MS. Sex differences in hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats: positive correlation between LTP and contextual learning. Brain Res. 1994;661: 25–34. pmid:7834376
  6. 6. Velasco ER, Florido A, Milad MR, Andero R. Sex differences in fear extinction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;103: 81–108. pmid:31129235
  7. 7. Pavlov PI. Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Ann Neurosci. 1927;17: 136–141. pmid:25205891
  8. 8. Maren S. Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24: 897–931. pmid:11520922
  9. 9. Fendt M, Fanselow MS. The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23: 743–760. pmid:10392663
  10. 10. LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23: 155–184. pmid:10845062
  11. 11. Wiltgen BJ, Sanders MJ, Behne NS, Fanselow MS. Sex differences, context preexposure, and the immediate shock deficit in Pavlovian context conditioning with mice. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115: 26–32. pmid:11256449
  12. 12. Gresack JE, Schafe GE, Orr PT, Frick KM. Sex differences in contextual fear conditioning are associated with differential ventral hippocampal extracellular signal-regulated kinase activation. Neuroscience. 2009;159: 451–467. pmid:19171181
  13. 13. Ribeiro AM, Barbosa FF, Godinho MR, Fernandes VS, Munguba H, Melo TG, et al. Sex differences in aversive memory in rats: possible role of extinction and reactive emotional factors. Brain Cogn. 2010;74: 145–151. pmid:20727653
  14. 14. Daviu N, Andero R, Armario A, Nadal R. Sex differences in the behavioural and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal response to contextual fear conditioning in rats. Horm Behav. 2014;66: 713–723. pmid:25311689
  15. 15. Colon L, Odynocki N, Santarelli A, Poulos AM. Sexual differentiation of contextual fear responses. Learn Mem. 2018;25: 230–240. pmid:29661835
  16. 16. Cossio R, Carreira MB, Vásquez CE, Britton GB. Sex differences and estrous cycle effects on foreground contextual fear conditioning. Physiol Behav. 2016;163: 305–311. pmid:27195460
  17. 17. Totty MS, Warren N, Huddleston I, Ramanathan KR, Ressler RL, Oleksiak CR, et al. Behavioral and brain mechanisms mediating conditioned flight behavior in rats. Sci Rep. 2021;11: 8215. pmid:33859260
  18. 18. Blume SR, Freedberg M, Vantrease JE, Chan R, Padival M, Record MJ, et al. Sex- and Estrus-Dependent Differences in Rat Basolateral Amygdala. J Neurosci. 2017;37: 10567–10586. pmid:28954870
  19. 19. Bouton ME. Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn Mem. 2004;11: 485–494. pmid:15466298
  20. 20. Bouton ME, Maren S, McNally GP. Behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of pavlovian and instrumental extinction learning. Physiol Rev. 2021;101: 611–681. pmid:32970967
  21. 21. VanElzakker MB, Dahlgren MK, Davis FC, Dubois S, Shin LM. From Pavlov to PTSD: the extinction of conditioned fear in rodents, humans, and anxiety disorders. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;113: 3–18. pmid:24321650
  22. 22. Goode TD, Maren S. Animal models of fear relapse. ILAR J. 2014;55: 246–258. pmid:25225304
  23. 23. Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52: 976–986. pmid:12437938
  24. 24. Bouton ME, Bolles RC. Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learn Motiv. 1979;10: 445–466.
  25. 25. Maren S, Holmes A. Stress and fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41: 58–79. pmid:26105142
  26. 26. Maren S, Chang C. Recent fear is resistant to extinction. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103: 18020–18025. pmid:17090669
  27. 27. Maren S. Nature and causes of the immediate extinction deficit: a brief review. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;113: 19–24. pmid:24176924
  28. 28. Merz CJ, Wolf OT. The immediate extinction deficit occurs in a nonemotional learning paradigm. Learn Mem. 2019;26: 39–45. pmid:30651376
  29. 29. Bangasser DA, Cuarenta A. Sex differences in anxiety and depression: circuits and mechanisms. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2021;22: 674–684. pmid:34545241
  30. 30. Bangasser DA, Wiersielis KR, Khantsis S. Sex differences in the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system and its regulation by stress. Brain Res. 2016;1641: 177–188. pmid:26607253
  31. 31. Bangasser DA, Wiersielis KR. Sex differences in stress responses: a critical role for corticotropin-releasing factor. Hormones (Athens). 2018;17: 5–13. pmid:29858858
  32. 32. Bale TL, Epperson CN. Sex differences and stress across the lifespan. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18: 1413–1420. pmid:26404716
  33. 33. Pomrenze MB, Millan EZ, Hopf FW, Keiflin R, Maiya R, Blasio A, et al. A transgenic rat for investigating the anatomy and function of corticotrophin releasing factor circuits. Front Neurosci. 2015;9: 487. pmid:26733798
  34. 34. Chang C, Maren S. Early extinction after fear conditioning yields a context-independent and short-term suppression of conditional freezing in rats. Learn Mem. 2009;16: 62–68. pmid:19141467
  35. 35. Colom-Lapetina J, Begley SL, Johnson ME, Bean KJ, Kuwamoto WN, Shansky RM. Strain-dependent sex differences in a long-term forced swim paradigm. Behav Neurosci. 2017;131: 428–436. pmid:28805432
  36. 36. Pomrenze MB, Giovanetti SM, Maiya R, Gordon AG, Kreeger LJ, Messing RO. Dissecting the Roles of GABA and Neuropeptides from Rat Central Amygdala CRF Neurons in Anxiety and Fear Learning. Cell Rep. 2019;29: 13–21.e4. pmid:31577943
  37. 37. Chang C, Maren S. Strain difference in the effect of infralimbic cortex lesions on fear extinction in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124: 391–397. pmid:20528083
  38. 38. Liberzon I, Abelson JL. Context Processing and the Neurobiology of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Neuron. 2016;92: 14–30. pmid:27710783
  39. 39. Maren S, Phan KL, Liberzon I. The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14: 417–428. pmid:23635870
  40. 40. Anderson LC, Petrovich GD. Renewal of conditioned responding to food cues in rats: Sex differences and relevance of estradiol. Physiol Behav. 2015;151: 338–344. pmid:26253218
  41. 41. Milad MR, Igoe SA, Lebron-Milad K, Novales JE. Estrous cycle phase and gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience. 2009;164: 887–895. pmid:19761818
  42. 42. Gupta RR, Sen S, Diepenhorst LL, Rudick CN, Maren S. Estrogen modulates sexually dimorphic contextual fear conditioning and hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) in rats. Brain Res. 2001;888: 356–365. pmid:11150498
  43. 43. Nagaya N, Acca GM, Maren S. Allopregnanolone in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis modulates contextual fear in rats. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9: 205. pmid:26300750
  44. 44. Kamprath K, Wotjak CT. Nonassociative learning processes determine expression and extinction of conditioned fear in mice. Learn Mem. 2004;11: 770–786. pmid:15537742
  45. 45. Giustino TF, Ramanathan KR, Totty MS, Miles OW, Maren S. Locus Coeruleus Norepinephrine Drives Stress-Induced Increases in Basolateral Amygdala Firing and Impairs Extinction Learning. J Neurosci. 2020;40: 907–916. pmid:31801809
  46. 46. Totty MS, Payne MR, Maren S. Event boundaries do not cause the immediate extinction deficit after Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Sci Rep. 2019;9: 9459. pmid:31263140
  47. 47. Giustino TF, Seemann JR, Acca GM, Goode TD, Fitzgerald PJ, Maren S. β-Adrenoceptor Blockade in the Basolateral Amygdala, But Not the Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Rescues the Immediate Extinction Deficit. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42: 2537–2544. pmid:28462941
  48. 48. McCall JG, Siuda ER, Bhatti DL, Lawson LA, McElligott ZA, Stuber GD, et al. Locus coeruleus to basolateral amygdala noradrenergic projections promote anxiety-like behavior. Elife. 2017;6. pmid:28708061
  49. 49. Fitzgerald PJ, Giustino TF, Seemann JR, Maren S. Noradrenergic blockade stabilizes prefrontal activity and enables fear extinction under stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112: E3729–37. pmid:26124100
  50. 50. Kim SC, Jo YS, Kim IH, Kim H, Choi J-S. Lack of medial prefrontal cortex activation underlies the immediate extinction deficit. J Neurosci. 2010;30: 832–837. pmid:20089891
  51. 51. Hong J, Kim D. Freezing response-independent facilitation of fear extinction memory in the prefrontal cortex. Sci Rep. 2017;7: 5363. pmid:28706238
  52. 52. Do-Monte FH, Manzano-Nieves G, Quiñones-Laracuente K, Ramos-Medina L, Quirk GJ. Revisiting the role of infralimbic cortex in fear extinction with optogenetics. J Neurosci. 2015;35: 3607–3615. pmid:25716859
  53. 53. Quirk GJ, Mueller D. Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33: 56–72. pmid:17882236
  54. 54. Maren S, Quirk GJ. Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5: 844–852. pmid:15496862
  55. 55. Uematsu A, Tan BZ, Ycu EA, Cuevas JS, Koivumaa J, Junyent F, et al. Modular organization of the brainstem noradrenaline system coordinates opposing learning states. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20: 1602–1611. pmid:28920933
  56. 56. Snyder K, Wang W-W, Han R, McFadden K, Valentino RJ. Corticotropin-releasing factor in the norepinephrine nucleus, locus coeruleus, facilitates behavioral flexibility. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37: 520–530. pmid:21993205
  57. 57. Mulvey B, Bhatti DL, Gyawali S, Lake AM, Kriaucionis S, Ford CP, et al. Molecular and functional sex differences of noradrenergic neurons in the mouse locus coeruleus. Cell Rep. 2018;23: 2225–2235. pmid:29791834
  58. 58. Bangasser DA, Zhang X, Garachh V, Hanhauser E, Valentino RJ. Sexual dimorphism in locus coeruleus dendritic morphology: a structural basis for sex differences in emotional arousal. Physiol Behav. 2011;103: 342–351. pmid:21362438
  59. 59. Curtis AL, Bethea T, Valentino RJ. Sexually dimorphic responses of the brain norepinephrine system to stress and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31: 544–554. pmid:16123744
  60. 60. McCall JG, Al-Hasani R, Siuda ER, Hong DY, Norris AJ, Ford CP, et al. CRH Engagement of the Locus Coeruleus Noradrenergic System Mediates Stress-Induced Anxiety. Neuron. 2015;87: 605–620. pmid:26212712
  61. 61. Van Bockstaele EJ, Colago EE, Valentino RJ. Amygdaloid corticotropin-releasing factor targets locus coeruleus dendrites: substrate for the co-ordination of emotional and cognitive limbs of the stress response. J Neuroendocrinol. 1998;10: 743–757. pmid:9792326
  62. 62. Valentino RJ, Rudoy C, Saunders A, Liu XB, Van Bockstaele EJ. Corticotropin-releasing factor is preferentially colocalized with excitatory rather than inhibitory amino acids in axon terminals in the peri-locus coeruleus region. Neuroscience. 2001;106: 375–384. pmid:11566507
  63. 63. Reyes BAS, Drolet G, Van Bockstaele EJ. Dynorphin and stress-related peptides in rat locus coeruleus: contribution of amygdalar efferents. J Comp Neurol. 2008;508: 663–675. pmid:18381633
  64. 64. Bouret S, Duvel A, Onat S, Sara SJ. Phasic activation of locus ceruleus neurons by the central nucleus of the amygdala. J Neurosci. 2003;23: 3491–3497. pmid:12716958
  65. 65. Jo YS, Namboodiri VMK, Stuber GD, Zweifel LS. Persistent activation of central amygdala CRF neurons helps drive the immediate fear extinction deficit. Nat Commun. 2020;11: 422. pmid:31969571
  66. 66. Curtis AL, Pavcovich LA, Grigoriadis DE, Valentino RJ. Previous stress alters corticotropin-releasing factor neurotransmission in the locus coeruleus. Neuroscience. 1995;65: 541–550. pmid:7777167
  67. 67. Curtis AL, Pavcovich LA, Valentino RJ. Long-term regulation of locus ceruleus sensitivity to corticotropin-releasing factor by swim stress. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1999;289: 1211–1219. pmid:10336508
  68. 68. Myers KM, Ressler KJ, Davis M. Different mechanisms of fear extinction dependent on length of time since fear acquisition. Learn Mem. 2006;13: 216–223. pmid:16585797
  69. 69. Bouchet CA, Lloyd BA, Loetz EC, Farmer CE, Ostrovskyy M, Haddad N, et al. Acute exercise enhances the consolidation of fear extinction memory and reduces conditioned fear relapse in a sex-dependent manner. Learn Mem. 2017;24: 358–368. pmid:28716955
  70. 70. Hilz EN, Smith RW, Hong YJ, Monfils MH, Lee HJ. Mapping the estrous cycle to context-specific extinction memory. Behav Neurosci. 2019;133: 614–623. pmid:31599608
  71. 71. Hilz EN, Agee LA, Jun D, Monfils MH, Lee HJ. Expression patterns of Arc mRNA after renewal of appetitive behavior in female rats. BioRxiv. 2021;
  72. 72. Park CHJ, Ganella DE, Kim JH. Context fear learning and renewal of extinguished fear are dissociated in juvenile female rats. Dev Psychobiol. 2020;62: 123–129. pmid:31267536
  73. 73. Park CHJ, Ganella DE, Kim JH. Juvenile female rats, but not male rats, show renewal, reinstatement, and spontaneous recovery following extinction of conditioned fear. Learn Mem. 2017;24: 630–636. pmid:29142058
  74. 74. Adkins JM, Lynch J, Gray M, Jasnow AM. Presynaptic GABAB receptor inhibition sex dependently enhances fear extinction and attenuates fear renewal. Psychopharmacology. 2021; pmid:33855580
  75. 75. Morris RW, Bouton ME. The effect of yohimbine on the extinction of conditioned fear: a role for context. Behav Neurosci. 2007;121: 501–514. pmid:17592941
  76. 76. Harris JA, Westbrook RF. Evidence that GABA transmission mediates context-specific extinction of learned fear. Psychopharmacology. 1998;140: 105–115. pmid:9862409
  77. 77. Li W-G, Wu Y-J, Gu X, Fan H-R, Wang Q, Zhu J-J, et al. Input associativity underlies fear memory renewal. Natl Sci Rev. 2021;8: nwab004. pmid:34691732
  78. 78. Xu C, Krabbe S, Gründemann J, Botta P, Fadok JP, Osakada F, et al. Distinct hippocampal pathways mediate dissociable roles of context in memory retrieval. Cell. 2016;167: 961–972.e16. pmid:27773481
  79. 79. Marek R, Jin J, Goode TD, Giustino TF, Wang Q, Acca GM, et al. Hippocampus-driven feed-forward inhibition of the prefrontal cortex mediates relapse of extinguished fear. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21: 384–392. pmid:29403033
  80. 80. Jin J, Maren S. Fear renewal preferentially activates ventral hippocampal neurons projecting to both amygdala and prefrontal cortex in rats. Sci Rep. 2015;5: 8388. pmid:25669753
  81. 81. Anderson LC, Petrovich GD. Distinct recruitment of the hippocampal, thalamic, and amygdalar neurons projecting to the prelimbic cortex in male and female rats during context-mediated renewal of responding to food cues. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2018;150: 25–35. pmid:29496643
  82. 82. Anderson LC, Petrovich GD. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates sex differences in persistent cognitive drive for food. Sci Rep. 2018;8: 2230. pmid:29396448
  83. 83. Anderson LC, Petrovich GD. Sex specific recruitment of a medial prefrontal cortex-hippocampal-thalamic system during context-dependent renewal of responding to food cues in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2017;139: 11–21. pmid:27940080
  84. 84. Namkung H, Kim S-H, Sawa A. The insula: an underestimated brain area in clinical neuroscience, psychiatry, and neurology. Trends Neurosci. 2017;40: 200–207. pmid:28314446