Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

A content analysis of 32 years of Shark Week documentaries

  • Lisa B. Whitenack ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    lwhitena@allegheny.edu

    Affiliation Departments of Biology & Geology, Allegheny College, Meadville, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Brady L. Mickley ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Brady L. Mickley, Julia Saltzman

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Environmental Science, Allegheny College, Meadville, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Julia Saltzman ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Brady L. Mickley, Julia Saltzman

    Roles Investigation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Biological Sciences, University of New Hampshire, Durham, New Hampshire, United States of America, Field School, Coconut Grove, Florida, United States of America, University of Miami Rosenstiel School of Marine and Atmospheric Science, Miami, Florida, United States of America

  • Stephen M. Kajiura,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, Florida, United States of America

  • Catherine C. Macdonald,

    Roles Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Field School, Coconut Grove, Florida, United States of America, University of Miami Rosenstiel School of Marine and Atmospheric Science, Miami, Florida, United States of America

  • David S. Shiffman

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Arizona State University New College of Interdisciplinary Arts and Science, Glendale, Arizona, United States of America

Abstract

Despite evidence of their importance to marine ecosystems, at least 32% of all chondrichthyan species are estimated or assessed as threatened with extinction. In addition to the logistical difficulties of effectively conserving wide-ranging marine species, shark conservation is believed to have been hindered in the past by public perceptions of sharks as dangerous to humans. Shark Week is a high-profile, international programming event that has potentially enormous influence on public perceptions of sharks, shark research, shark researchers, and shark conservation. However, Shark Week has received regular criticism for poor factual accuracy, fearmongering, bias, and inaccurate representations of science and scientists. This research analyzes the content and titles of Shark Week episodes across its entire 32 years of programming to determine if there are trends in species covered, research techniques featured, expert identity, conservation messaging, type of programming, and portrayal of sharks. We analyzed titles from 272 episodes (100%) of Shark Week programming and the content of all available (201; 73.9%) episodes. Our data demonstrate that the majority of episodes are not focused on shark bites, although such shows are common and many Shark Week programs frame sharks around fear, risk, and adrenaline. While criticisms of disproportionate attention to particular charismatic species (e.g. great whites, bull sharks, and tiger sharks) are accurate and supported by data, 79 shark species have been featured briefly at least once. Shark Week’s depictions of research and of experts are biased towards a small set of (typically visual and expensive) research methodologies and (mostly white, mostly male) experts, including presentation of many white male non-scientists as scientific experts. While sharks are more often portrayed negatively than positively, limited conservation messaging does appear in 53% of episodes analyzed. Results suggest that as a whole, while Shark Week is likely contributing to the collective public perception of sharks as bad, even relatively small alterations to programming decisions could substantially improve the presentation of sharks and shark science and conservation issues.

Introduction

Shark species can make important contributions to the resilience, structure, and function of marine ecosystems [1,2]. At least 32% of all chondrichthyan species (sharks, rays, skates, and chimeras) are assessed as or estimated to be Threatened with extinction [36], primarily due to overfishing [7,8]. Conservation of shark populations is challenging given that many species share life history characteristics that contribute to vulnerability to intensive exploitation, including slow growth, late age at maturity, and relatively low fecundity [9]. Management is further complicated by the reality that shark fisheries globally are often under-managed, enforcement resources are typically limited, many species are highly mobile through multiple management jurisdictions, and sharks can represent an important food source, especially in subsistence fishing communities [3].

In addition to the biological, ecological, and practical difficulties of effectively conserving sharks, shark conservation is believed to have been hindered in the past by public perceptions of sharks as dangerous to humans [10], including portrayals suggesting that they are evil or vicious [11]. One of the primary ways that the public (defined here as people who are not shark scientists, marine scientists, or aquarists) obtains knowledge about sharks is through the media, including news stories, social media, and television programs [12,13]. Although negative images of sharks predate the modern era, with sharks featured as villains in many famous works of art and literature (e.g. Copley’s Watson and the Shark (1778) or Hemingway’s The Old Man and The Sea (1952)), the movie Jaws (1975) marked a shift towards modern presentations of sharks with visceral visual immediacy. These negative portrayals have been reinforced over decades by media reporting focused overwhelmingly on shark bite incidents and by “attack” focused movies and television programs [11,1417]. This media landscape contributes to a collective public conceptualization of sharks as bad, dangerous man-eaters [1823]. Public perceptions of sharks may affect shark conservation efforts directly or indirectly, by altering public attitudes or by shaping public knowledge and support for particular policies [10,24].

Shark Week, an annual event from Discovery Communications, has played a large role in shaping public perceptions of sharks since 1988. Shark Week is the highest-profile coverage of marine biology or ocean conservation on U.S. television, and represents the greatest temporary increase over baseline in U.S. residents paying attention to any ocean science topic [25,26]. In 2020, over 21 million viewers tuned in and 37% of those viewers did not watch the Discovery Channel in the month prior to Shark Week [27]. Social media mentions occur in the hundreds of thousands each year during Shark Week, as do notable spikes in Wikipedia searches about sharks [25]. Therefore, this long-running programming event has potentially enormous influence on public perceptions of sharks, shark research, shark researchers, and shark conservation; it may be the only time that many people think about these topics at all during a typical year.

On one hand, one study has shown that survey respondents who are Shark Week viewers tend to display significantly higher levels of knowledge about sharks and tendency to believe shark conservation is urgent compared to survey respondents who have not watched Shark Week [12]. However, Shark Week has received regular criticism for poor factual accuracy, fearmongering, bias, and inaccurate representations of science and scientists. Criticism includes concerns that Shark Week only features white male scientists despite the existence of many scientists from a variety of diverse backgrounds and concerns that the same geographic locations and species are repeatedly featured despite interesting and conservation-relevant research taking place around the world (e.g. [2832]). Multiple scientific experts have reported that their words were selectively edited to make it appear that they were responding to questions they were not asked, or that they found the way they were presented in Shark Week programs profoundly professionally embarrassing [33,34]. In 2010, in response to criticism, Discovery Communications agreed to reduce entertainment programming during Shark Week and present more scientifically-oriented episodes, though many of these more scientific programs continued to depict sharks negatively [35].

Fictional programming from Shark Week has also generated scientific criticism, with shows like “Megalodon: The Monster Shark Lives” (2013), “Megalodon: The New Evidence” (2014), and “Shark of Darkness: Wrath of Submarine” (2014) presenting fictional storylines featuring computer-generated imagery (CGI) and actors pretending to be scientists and government officials, without clear communication to the viewer that these programs are not factual (e.g. [36,37]. Otodus megalodon (often referred to just as “Megalodon”), was a shark that lived 15.9–3.5 million years ago and has been estimated to reach a maximum size of 14-18m long, making it the largest shark species that has ever lived [3843]. According to a post-show social media poll following “Megalodon: The Monster Shark Lives”, 79% of respondents reported believing that O. megalodon was still alive [36], despite the fact that all scientific evidence suggests that O. megalodon went extinct well over 2 million years ago [40,43]. These types of programs not only present inaccurate information in ways that may reduce trust in science and scientists (e.g. [44]), but through their depiction of scientific and government collusion to hide the truth, join broader sociological trends towards belief in conspiracy theories, which can undermine confidence in previously undisputed facts and limit the potential for social consensus-building around a wide range of important issues [45].

Defenses of Shark Week have typically fallen into two primary categories. Firstly, defenders of current programming have argued that alternative programming choices do not exist. Both Discovery Communications and National Geographic have stated that it is impossible to include more diverse representations of shark scientists because available shark scientists are predominantly white men [46], and that demand will not allow for more conservation-focused or science-focused programs [47]. These arguments are undermined by the existence of organizations like Minorities in Shark Science (MISS; [48]) which to date has over 300 members (including a portion who are employed as shark scientists) who identify as women of color (Graham, pers. comm.), and the popularity of factual, documentary television nature programming like that found on the BBC (as opposed to the more “hybrid” programming combining factual and non-fiction or fictional genres [49] often seen on Shark Week). For example, while viewership for the Blue Planet II series was notably lower in the U.S. than in the U.K., each episode had approximately 3 million U.S. viewers [50]. For reference, this is on par with both the viewership of the final game of the 2021 Stanley Cup and the 2018 Shark Week episode “Shaq Does Shark Week” [51,52].

Secondly, defenders argue that the most problematic elements of Shark Week are also essentially harmless (e.g., that no one expects Shark Week to offer accurate information or believes what they see there (Shiffman, pers. obsv.)). It is well-established that reality television and info- or edu-tainment programming can have real-world effects on public understanding of issues. Evidence suggests that exposure to pseudoscientific television programming is associated with viewers holding pseudoscientific beliefs (e.g. [53,54]). For example, seventy-two percent of women who were pregnant for the first time and watched reality shows related to birth and pregnancy said that reality television could help them understand what it was like to give birth, although these shows disproportionately focus on medically- and technologically-assisted birth, present birth as dangerous, and frame women’s bodies as incapable [55,56]. Reality TV filming locations have been reported to have substantial effects on tourist destination choice and demand for particular locations [49]. The power of media has become especially apparent during the COVID-19 pandemic, as 2020 was marked by the propagation of medical disinformation and conspiracy theories across social media, with an estimated 30% of the U.K. and U.S. populations subscribing to COVID scientific conspiracy narratives [57]. These and many other similar examples refute arguments that Shark Week programming can have no meaningful real-world impact on sharks, shark science, or shark conservation. Shark Week represents a significant opportunity for public engagement with shark science and conservation for a large and enthusiastic audience. Documentary programming can shape public opinion about conservation-relevant issues, particularly in concert with other reinforcing information or events, sometimes resulting in pro-environmental policy and management changes [58].

In sum, the narrative around Shark Week programming is complex. Shark Week clearly resonates with a large viewership, however, viewers and scholars have also perceived a lack of diversity (both in featured experts and featured sharks), a focus on shark “attacks”, negative and fear-mongering messaging, and factual inaccuracies. Given the different narratives around Shark Week and the event’s potential importance for generating public support for shark conservation, our goal was to quantify some of the reported trends in Shark Week programming. Previous studies have examined select time periods in Shark Week’s history [35,59], but a comprehensive survey of the entire history of Shark Week has not been performed previously. Specifically, we tested the following hypotheses to better understand the content of Shark Week episodes. These hypotheses were constructed based on both the current literature, and our own experiences and those of others in our field of shark science:

  1. Titles of Shark Week episodes use negative language evoking fear, and the use of negative language has increased over time (see [21]);
  2. Shark Week episodes focus on the three species of sharks most associated with shark “attacks” on humans (the white shark Carcharodon carcharias, tiger shark Galeocerdo cuvier, and bull shark Carcharias leucas) and do not focus on species threatened most with extinction (see [31,32,59]);
  3. Shark Week episodes focus on shark “attacks” on humans and swimming with sharks with no broader goal, rather than scientific research or conservation (see [29,31,35]);
  4. Shark Week episodes focus on a small number of research methodologies, primarily tagging (see [30]);
  5. Experts/hosts featured on Shark Week episodes tend to be white and male, which include many with no or little scientific expertise and a small number of people that are on many episodes (see [30,31,35,46]);
  6. Shark Week episodes are largely filmed in a few locations repeatedly each year (see the “Air Jaws” series of episodes; Shiffman, pers. observ.);
  7. Shark Week episodes tend to portray sharks negatively while also presenting statements about conservation that are not actionable (see [31,35,59]).

This quantitative analysis of the key features of Shark Week’s 30+ year run will provide better data from which to discuss current practices and recommend improvements.

Methods

Title analysis

Titles from all Shark Week episodes from 1988 to 2020 were collected from two online sources: thetvdb.com (2021) and the Washington Post (1994) (N = 272). Each title was analyzed to determine whether it contained words evoking negative connotations or if the title could be taken as negative within context. Individual title words were searched within the list of Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW) by author SMK [60]. Title words that scored below the mean for valence (ranging from pleasant to unpleasant) or above the mean for arousal (ranging from calm to excited) were considered as eliciting a negative effect. These titles often included words associated with violence (wrath, fury), fear (terror, fear, scream), death (deadly, death, killer), danger (danger, dangerous), or attacks (shark attack, shark bite).

In addition to analyzing individual words within titles, whole titles were analyzed in context, as pairing a negative word with a non-negative word may render an episode title as no longer negative. For example, the word “monster” is recognized as negative in the analysis described above, but a program entitled “Monster Garage: Shark Boat” used the word “monster” to describe a garage, not sharks. In this case, the title would not be classified as negative. Analyzing titles as described above also allowed some titles that contained no negative words from the ANEW list to be categorized as negative based upon the construction of the title, such as “Rogue Shark” or “Lair of the Mega Shark”. Several titles that contained the word “Jaws” were treated as negative since the word was deliberately chosen based on its association with the eponymous movie. However, episodes from the “Air Jaws” series were not treated as negative unless other parts of the title had a negative connotation, as this series already has a reputation for not being negative. Whole titles were analyzed first by author SMK, followed by all authors to reach consensus. The list of titles was then independently analyzed by a public relations professional in the same manner to independently confirm the validity of our approach.

The total number of titles and number of negative titles were counted for each year and the proportion of negative titles was calculated. Each title was stripped of punctuation and decomposed into individual words. Singular and plural words were grouped together (e.g. Shark, Sharks) and root words with various suffixes were also grouped together (e.g. Kill, Killer, Killing). The frequency of occurrence of words and word groups was sorted from most to least frequent for all years combined. Lines of best fit were calculated in Excel.

Episode analysis

For the purpose of this study, obtainable episodes (N = 201, 73.9% of all aired Shark Week episodes; S1 Table) from 1988–2020 were watched by one of four trained coders (LBW, BLM, JS, and DSS). Analyzed episodes were obtained from a variety of online sources (i.e., Hulu®, Amazon Prime®, Discovery+®, YouTube®, Vimeo®), library loans, and private holdings. Those which were not coded were not obtainable via any of the aforementioned platforms, despite extensive online searches between 2019 and 2021.

A code book was developed by all coauthors, which is described in detail in S1 File. The coding process included the following areas of interest: A) documentary title, B) documentary year, C) production company, D) locations of filming, E) species of chondrichthyans (sharks, rays, skates, chimeras) featured, F) featured experts/hosts of the show, G) general type of documentary, H) research methods featured, I) purpose/goal of documentary, J) accomplishment of goal/purpose, K) mention of shark conservation, L) mention of shark finning, M) mention of shark meat for consumption, N) mention of ways to help sharks, O) negative portrayal of sharks, P) positive portrayal of sharks, Q) portrayal of sharks other than negative or positive, R) mention of specific misconceptions (e.g., “bull sharks are the only sharks that can enter freshwater” and “sharks can smell a drop of blood from a mile away”), S) anything else about a given episode that coders found noteworthy including space for coders to note variables that they were unsure about. Most variables were binary (presence/absence), which minimized the chance of coding error or coding agreement problems resulting from interpretation. Coding variables were explicitly chosen to be as objective as possible to eliminate the opportunity to insert personal bias.

Prior to data collection, all coders were briefed by author DSS on what each of the variables meant and were shown several brief examples. All coders then completed analysis of the same three episodes to check for intercoder reliability and assess whether the coding tool was working as intended. There were no disagreement in quantitative coding or results among these three episodes watched by all reviewers; all coders reported identical results.

Coders filled out the same structured coding form in Google Forms for each episode, filling in applicable information throughout the duration of the show. Given the large amount of media to watch and code, each episode was coded by one coder (see [32,61]) for the purposes of data analysis (but see below regarding reliability analysis). Coders were generally haphazardly assigned to episodes by year by author LBW, with JS coding 83 episodes, BLM 82 episodes, DSS 20 episodes, and LBW 16 episodes (S1 Table). There were two purposeful exceptions to the haphazard assignment of episodes. The first was for episodes only available on DVD, as DVDs were shipped to author LBW; therefore LBW and BLM analyzed DVD episodes. The second was for an episode that a coder (LBW) appeared in; this episode was assigned to a different coder (BLM). A single coder (DSS) acted as arbitrator to minimize error resulting from coder uncertainty and to make decisions on any variables that were coded unclearly. This was necessary for four episodes: one for research method, two for species, and one for geographic location.

Following data collection, we performed a post-hoc intercoder reliability analysis [62]. Because authors JS and BLM were unavailable at the time of the post-hoc analysis, we only considered episodes that one of those authors watched. From that condensed list, we used a random number generator to choose the initial list of episodes to include. To ensure coverage across the 30 year spectrum, we haphazardly removed episodes from years with more than three episodes in the initial list. 21 episodes (10.4% of total available episodes) were included the post-hoc reliability analysis (S1 Table). Authors DSS and LBW each independently watched the 21 episodes and coded them as described above. Thus, each episode in this analysis was watched by three of the four coding authors. For each coded variable, percent agreement was calculated directly (number of observations in agreement/total number of observations). Cohen’s Kappa was the calculated for each variable using GraphPad.

Host/Expert analysis

To better understand the sources of expert perspectives offered by Shark Week, we assessed the academic research productivity of those identified onscreen as “experts”, “scientists”, and “researchers” as a rough proxy for recognized scientific activity [32]. While we recognize that there are other types of expertise outside of scientific expertise that are important and real (e.g. direct experience with sharks but not scientific method), we observed through our own casual viewing and this study that the activities shown in Shark Week episodes are often framed as scientific experiments, even if they were sometimes poorly designed, not actual experiments, pseudoscience, or attempted to answer a question that was answered decades earlier [30,63]. Given that this may lead to viewers assuming that all presented experts are scientists and that scientists are often trusted types of experts (e.g. [64]), and that we it can be difficult for the public to sort through misinformation and true information (e.g. [65,66], we were interested in this specific type of expertise.

We used Google Scholar to determine the number of peer-reviewed publications (as of July 9, 2021) authored by each Shark Week expert/host. Because scientific journal publications are not the only evidence of scientific expertise, we also counted authorship of peer-reviewed book chapters and edited books, abstracts from conferences, and government reports. This allowed us to capture evidence of scientific expertise from populations such as early career researchers that may have not yet published in a journal, as well as government scientists who may not necessarily report their findings in scientific journals, but rather government reports. Popular press books and magazine articles were not included. We sorted expert scientific productivity into bins based on number of publications (0, 1–5, 6–15, 16–25, 26–50, 50+ publications). Fictional experts were removed from the analysis (i.e. fictional scientist “Colin Drake” from the Megalodon episodes).

In order to assess the gender and racial diversity of non-fictional hosts/experts, we conducted Google searches for biographies, news articles, or social media profiles of each host/expert [67]. We successfully located sources for all 204 non-fictional hosts/experts. The pronouns used in these sources were used to determine their gender for this analysis, with the caveat that this allows us to speak only to broad trends in gender representation on Shark Week, not to the individual gender identities of particular experts. We recognize that people may be misgendered in sources that they did not write themselves.

We coded host/expert race based on how they would could be visually perceived by U.S. audiences, with the exception of the small number of hosts/experts who mention their race in public biographies or platforms (i.e., on Twitter #BlackinMarineScience or #LatinxinSTEM). Because names are a poor predictor of racial identity, we did not consider names in this analysis. Therefore we relied on visual assessment of hosts/experts via Google searches for photos and videos, following a similar method as Cukier et al. [67]. Authors DSS, JS, and LBW independently coded whether they perceived that hosts/experts were white or white-perceived, or not, and then discussed their coding to reach consensus. Race is a social construct and is defined either by self-identification or by how one is perceived by others. Our method allowed us to develop an approximate estimate of the proportion of hosts/experts who can be assessed as white or white-perceived to examine broad trends in representation on Shark Week without speaking to individual racial identities of particular experts. We recognize that the authors may have misidentified some hosts’/experts’ self-identified race in this study.

Species analysis

After all episodes were coded, we collected the following additional information on each shark species appearing in the episode: A) maximum recorded size [68], B) IUCN Red List Status ([6], data retrieved in June 2021), C) taxonomic order. All species appearing on screen and also mentioned by name in narration were recorded, regardless of total screen time and regardless of whether that species was discussed meaningfully during the episode. Results thus over-represent the coverage of less commonly featured species (e.g. a blacknose shark that appeared for ten seconds in an episode largely about tiger sharks would yield the same results as an episode that featured both blacknose and tiger sharks equally).

Results and discussion

Title analysis and programming trends

Between 1988 and 2020, there were 272 unique Shark Week program titles (S1 Table). For the first 15 years, the number of original programs each year did not exceed seven. The number of programs began to increase dramatically after 2010, with the greatest number of unique programs (24) produced in 2018 and 2020 (Fig 1). Over half (51.8%) of all Shark Week programming was produced in the past eight years (2013–2020). The increase in the number of titles each year is best fit with an exponential function (y = 2.290e0.0622x where x = year 1–33, r2 = 0.882). Overall, 59 titles (21.7%) used words with negative connotations, based on the ANEW. Although the number of titles produced increased exponentially, the number of titles with negative words each year ranged between zero and five and increased less dramatically (y = 1.204e0.039x where x = year 1–33, r2 = 0.449).When titles were analyzed within context (considering phrases rather than individual words), 42.6% of all titles were categorized as negative. The number of negative titles increased proportionally with the total number of titles as an exponential function (y = 0.938e0.074x where x = year 1–33, r2 = 0.850).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Trends in Shark Week episode titles.

(A) Frequency of negative words used in episode titles; (B) Percent of episodes with negative titles per year. Orange circles indicate episode titles coded as negative based solely on the Affective Norms for English Words list (ANEW), blue squares indicate episode titles coded as negative by authors based on context (phrases rather than individual words); (C) Number of episodes with negative titles per year. Black circles indicate total number of episodes aired each year, orange circles indicate episode titles coded as negative based solely on the ANEW, blue squares indicate episode titles coded as negative by authors based on context.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.g001

The proportion of titles that were assessed as having negative connotations based on the ANEW ranged from 0–75% in any given year (Fig 1). During the first five years of programming (1988–1992) only two titles were considered negative. In total, six non-consecutive years, representing 18.2% of the time examined, had no negative titles (Fig 1B). The last year in which there were no negative titles was 2000. In contrast, at least half of the program titles each year were considered negative for twelve years, which represents 36.4% of the time examined (Fig 1B). The greatest number of negative titles was found in 2018 (14) and 2020 (12). These two years both produced the greatest number of programs (24 each year) and combined they represent 17.6% of all Shark Week programming since inception.

One hundred seventy-three titles (64.6%) included the root word “shark”. Many of the 99 titles that did not include “shark” referred to a specific species, such as “Great White Encounters”. Sixteen titles (5.9%) were merely descriptive and used the format, “Sharks of xxx”, where the xxx denotes a location.

The 272 program titles were composed of 1047 total words. There were 354 unique words, including root words plus their derivatives. The words “shark(s)” occurred the most frequently (161 occurrences) and accounted for 15.4% of all title words. This was followed by the prepositions “of” (6.5%) and “the” (5.6%), then “jaws” (3.2%), “great” (2.8%), “white(s)” (2.6%), and “attack(s)” (1.4%). “Attack(s)” was the most frequently occurring word that has a negative connotation based on the ANEW list, and “jaws” was the most frequently occurring word that has a negative connotation in context.

The number of negative titles for Shark Week shows increased at a lower rate (for words from the ANEW list) or at a similar rate (for titles taken in context) to the total number of titles. The number of negative titles based on the ANEW list did not exceed five per year, despite a dramatic increase in the total number of programs produced. For the first 15 years, all programs with negative titles were derived from words within the ANEW list. However, more recently, the number of negative titles in context has increased at a greater rate than the number of titles with negative words from the ANEW list. It is possible that titles are being constructed to avoid negative words, but the titles still depict sharks in a sensationalized, potentially negative light when taken in context.

Some words that have a negative connotation in isolation can be rendered neutral in context. For example, an existing franchise entitled, “Naked and Afraid” produced episodes for Shark Week in 2018 and 2020 with the title, “Naked and Afraid of Sharks”. In this case, the franchise title already included the word “afraid” so these titles were not included in the list of negative titles. However, taken in isolation, the title “Naked and Afraid of Sharks” would be considered to be a negative title. Returning to the example of “monster” outlined in the methods section, “Monster Garage: Shark Boat” was not classified as negative. In some cases, “monster” might have been chosen for its negative connotations. However, the word “monster” can also refer to something that is very large, so programs like “Monster Mako” may refer to a particularly large mako shark. Titles which used words like “monster” to describe sharks themselves were classified as negative despite some ambiguity around their meaning.

The word “Jaws’’ is unique in the context of Shark Week titles. Nearly all vertebrates possess jaws, so the word is not considered negative in itself. However, the 1975 movie “Jaws” caused people to associate the word with a killer shark. The word may evoke a primal fear of being attacked, bitten, or eaten, and has been associated with negative connotations in other studies (e.g. [14,21,69]). Titles that include the word “Jaws” may indirectly suggest that the subject is dangerous or fear-inducing, without having to use words that are explicitly negative. Therefore, titles that included the word “jaws”, other than “Air Jaws”, were classified as negative within context.

There has been a recent trend to amalgamate “shark” with root words that have a negative connotation. This creates chimeras such as Sharkpocalypse (2013), Sharkageddon (2014), Sharksanity (2014,- 2016), and Sharkwrecked (2018, 2019) (S1 Table). None of these fabricated words would appear in the ANEW list so they were analyzed within context. While the root words “apocalypse”, “armageddon”, and “shipwreck” are not included in the ANEW list, the root word “insane” is listed and does have a negative connotation [60]. The implication of combining “shark” with high valence words is to create an association between them and thus infer that sharks are dangerous, scary, or otherwise unpleasant.

While our analysis examines episode titles with a simple categorization scheme (negative or not), it is important to note that not all negative words carry equal weight. In other words, some titles might be perceived by audiences to be more negative than others. For example, “Great Hammerhead Invasion” (aired in 2017) might be perceived as less negative or frightening than “Great White Serial Killer Lives” (2017) or “I Was Prey: Terror of the Deep” (2020). While more negative titles may benefit the network by increasing viewership [70], words and phrases that invoke vivid imagery can also cause people to be overly concerned or fearful about the risk of events—such as shark bites—with very small probabilities of occurring [7173]. Experiments examining psychological responses to Shark Week titles and content, such as studies on risk perception (e.g. [73]) and psychological responses to horror films (e.g. as reviewed in [74]), could elucidate the relationship between specific words and affect.

Episode analysis

General content: research is the most common category overall, but not every year.

201 episodes were watched, coded, and scored, though not every variable was present in every episode. A plurality of episodes were broadly categorized as being about “Research” (37%) or “Natural History” (16%) (Fig 2). Of 21 episodes included in the reliability analysis, the coders agreed on the documentary class for 17, agreement of 80.1%. This resulted in a Cohen’s Kappa of 0.618, Substantial Agreement. Of the four episodes with disagreement, two concerned the distinction between “Research” and “Natural History.” Our definition of research was very broad and essentially included any attempt to obtain the answer to any question about shark biology or behavior via observation or experimentation, which could overlap with the “Natural History” category (S1 File).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Number and percent of episodes by documentary class.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.g002

Many of the episodes categorized as research include atypical or unscientific methods, attempts to answer questions long considered resolved in the peer-reviewed scientific literature, or experimental design that would likely not be considered scientifically valid if presented in an academic journal or conference. Episodes that focused on reenactments of sharks biting people (“Shark bites”) and episodes with no purpose beyond people diving with sharks (“Diving with sharks”) each represented about 15% of all episodes. Episodes about mythical/legendary sharks represented about 7% of all episodes. When analyzed by year, we found no trends in programming; episodes have not become more or less focused on science or shark bites over time (Fig 3). However, we note that “research” themed programming was 25% or less in several years, including several consecutive years leading up to the fictional Megalodon episodes (2009–2012) and the most recent year analyzed in this study (2020).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Percent of episodes categorized by “Research” (orange line), “Shark Bites” (green line), or “Diving with Sharks” (blue line).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.g003

A focus on shark bites, shark-related danger, and mythical, legendary, or fictional monster sharks reflects, at least in part, the use of violence or fear as a marketing tool. Violent programming is a market differentiator known to attract advertiser-desired demographics, particularly 18-34-year-old males [70]. Hamilton [70] describes this tendency as an economically rational and self-interested act by networks, despite creating negative externalities for society that are not borne by the producers or programmers who are making decisions about content. It is also possible that the creation of frustrated and vocal constituencies opposed to inaccurate and fear-mongering programming, including opposition from scientists and conservationists, is part of an overall marketing strategy in which critique drives further public attention and viewership to even highly problematic content. Controversy and social media discussion are strongly positively correlated with sales performance, although strong and consistent negative word of mouth feedback may harm perceptions of a brand [75]. This argument is supported in the context of Shark Week in particular by O’Donnell’s observation that the year that aired the fake Megalodon documentary (2013) was also the year that generated the greatest volume of Twitter discussion about Shark Week [76].

While we can categorize episodes by theme, it does not reflect the likely unequal viewership across individual episodes. Unequal viewership may come from unequal marketing efforts or scheduling (e.g. the 8 PM Sunday show may receive more viewers than the 10 PM Tuesday show). It is also possible that some episodes are more highly watched simply because of their titles; phrases such as “shark attack” have been shown to trigger intuitive emotional responses related to survival and fear, which in turn can drive viewership [14]. Unfortunately, the data needed to analyze episodes by viewership or marketing effort are not available across the 32-year history of Shark Week.

Research methods: No research and high-tech methods dominate.

Among episodes that included research methods, methods featured tended to be simple, declarative, and visual, such as satellite tagging large charismatic animals (Fig 4). The most common methods were satellite telemetry tagging, acoustic telemetry tagging, or the use of high-tech camera equipment including drones, ROVs, BRUVs, or ultra-high-speed cameras; when added together these techniques were featured in approximately 40% of episodes. Some of this high-tech camera equipment is non-standard in published scientific research and was used in these episodes to obtain high-quality imagery for television, rather than scientific research purposes. While we note that examples of published studies using high-tech camera equipment do exist (as reviewed in [77]), the definition of “research method” used here was inclusive of activities that would not meet the scientific threshold for “research”. 18.3% of Shark Week episodes featured no research that met this criterion.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Number and percentage of occurrences of particular research methods in Shark Week episodes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.g004

Research with high-tech equipment such as ROVs and satellite telemetry tagging is also expensive, which may translate into featuring well-funded and prominent (often white and male) researchers. These research methodologies typically require more grant, donor, or personal funding, which is easier to access for scientists with some degree of seniority and influence and institutional support. Other methodologies producers may consider less visually appealing may be more likely to be performed by scientists working with limited resources, including early career researchers, scientists at less wealthy institutions, or those from less wealthy countries, making them less likely to be featured in Shark Week episodes. Compensation for Shark Week appearances may also exacerbate these inequities; for example, scientists requesting industry-minimum pay for their appearances have been passed over, whereas those who have more financial resources and may not need the additional funds can appear for less pay or just for exposure (Jewell, pers.comm.). Appearing on Shark Week programming can have positive benefits for researchers, including increased visibility at home institutions and in the media, increased professional opportunities, and additional research funds or resources [31,78].

The research methods featured in Shark Week are also notably distinct from the most common methods used in scientific investigations [79], which are dominated by age and growth, life history, and reproductive biology work. While perhaps not as camera friendly, this kind of work is vital to generate data for the sustainable management of shark species, and the fact that commonly conducted, management-relevant science is rarely featured could potentially impact public understanding of the purpose, function, and social relevance of marine science and the scientific process.

In addition to focusing on a very limited range of existing research techniques, television programming often presents science and scientific discovery as reporting unquestionably true facts rather than as generating knowledge through human-led iterative processes [80]. For example, the episode “Shark Storm” featured an experiment with one lemon shark that was trained to tap a target to obtain a food reward, and a second lemon shark that learned to do the same by observing the first shark. This was then presented as unequivocal proof that sharks are capable of social learning, although a sample size of one is not iterative. A significant disconnect has been found between scientists (who generally described science on television as failing to reflect the practices and methods of science), and those working to produce science programs for television, who believed reflecting uncertainty and methodological processes in science television programs would undermine confidence in science, and negatively affect ratings and audience interest [80]. In general, media producers have reported wanting to feature experts who are authoritative, confident, and willing to court controversy—all characteristics which do not necessarily align with effectively conveying scientific knowledge or nuance [81]. These are also masculine-coded characteristics and women may receive gendered hostility for displaying them or be more likely to be professionally penalized by senior male colleagues for them, perhaps explaining why women experts are generally more hesitant to appear on television than their male colleagues [81]; for more discussion of misogyny in shaping perceptions of female leadership and expertise, see [82,83].

For the 21 episodes included in the post-hoc reliability analysis, 43 methods were identified, with agreement on 30 of them (69.7% agreement) and a Cohen’s Kappa of 0.39 (Fair Agreement). Notably nearly all disagreement occurred with respect to methods that were only briefly mentioned in the relevant episodes or rarely-used methods that

were classified as “other” in Fig 4, and none concerned methods that were a major focus of the show.

Featured experts: Overwhelmingly white males with widely variable levels of scientific training and scientific expertise.

229 host/experts were featured in Shark Week episodes. In the post-hoc reliability analysis, 100 experts were identified in the 21 episodes, with agreement across 87 of them (87% agreement and a Cohen’s Kappa of 0.74 (Substantial Agreement.). All disagreements came from episodes that featured many experts for brief periods of time.

Shark Week episodes often repeatedly rely on a subset of hosts/experts; 102 of the 229 hosts/experts were featured in more than one episode. Of those, 80 were featured 2–5 times, 13 were featured 6–10 times and nine were featured more than 10 times. Of the latter, eight of the nine host/experts were featured in between 10 and 19 episodes, with one person featured in 43 different episodes; all were associated with male pronouns and white or white-perceived by our coders.

22.7% of the 204 people billed as an expert, scientist, or researcher by Shark Week have no peer-reviewed publications (Fig 5). 14.4% of featured experts have between one and five scientific publications, and although some of these individuals are early career researchers, many are people who are not working professionally in science. For example, one cinematographer is a coauthor on two scientific publications but is primarily not a scientist. However, just over 41% of experts featured have more than 26 peer-reviewed publications. Although the metric of publications is an imperfect measure of scientific and research contributions, it does provide a general sense of whether someone is actively engaged in publishable scientific research. Publication metrics and credentials may not be central to television representations of expertise. On U.S. talk shows, experts—particularly “intellectual experts”—are subject to “leveling” or being treated in ways that present them as equivalently knowledgeable as non-experts. They are often brought on late in an episode, featured alongside non-experts, given little time to speak, frequently interrupted, and may be challenged or disagreed with [84]. In some sense, Shark Week undertakes a similar leveling process, treating activists, divers, camera-people, and others as having equivalent scientific expertise to credentialed scientists. Of the nine most frequently featured host/experts, three have no peer-reviewed publications, including the host with the most Shark Week episodes (43 episodes). Papson [59] distinguishes between two types of Shark Week experts: experiential experts, who have direct experience with sharks but are not scientists, and scientists. While there are multiple kinds of useful and relevant knowledge, it may be helpful for Shark Week to more clearly distinguish between scientists and non-scientists (who may well possess other forms of valuable expertise but should not be presented as scientific authorities). It is also noteworthy that many of the most egregious and harmful factual errors or misrepresentations highlighted in criticisms of Shark Week came from non-scientists who Shark Week presents as scientific experts.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Frequency of number of scientific publications authored by Shark Week experts.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.g005

Shark Week programming has previously been criticized for overwhelmingly featuring white men as experts in their programming [46] and we were left with the same impression after viewing 201 episodes. 93.9% of experts were white or white-perceived by our coders, with only 6.1% of experts perceived as non-white. 24 out of 201 episodes included at least one host/expert perceived by coders as non-white; only one episode included more than one host/expert perceived as non-white. Based on our search of biographies, interviews, and social media posts, no experts used non-binary pronouns or publicly mentioned being trans*. 78.6% of hosts/experts were associated with male pronouns (an online biography for two hosts/experts was not readily available via Google search), whereas the remaining hosts/experts (20.1%) were associated with female pronouns (Fig 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Percent of episodes including any appearance by an expert/host referred to by female pronouns, by year.

The red dashed line indicates 50% for a given year. Overall, 20.1% of hosts/experts were associated with female pronouns.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.g006

60 of the 201 episodes included at least one host/expert associated with female pronouns. Only 11 episodes included more than one host/expert associated with female pronouns; of these nine aired between 2016 and 2018 and one each in 2003 and 2004. Of the 35 experts referred to as “Dr.”, three were associated with female pronouns. We note that two of the male experts referred to as “Dr.” do not have a Ph.D., D.V.M., or similar degree, and that some experts/hosts known to have Ph.D.’s were not referred to as “Dr.” Our results over-represent the coverage of experts associated with female pronouns and experts perceived by coders as non-white; if a female and/or non-white-perceived expert was featured on screen in an episode in which the vast majority of speaking was performed by male and/or white-perceived experts, the episode was counted as featuring a female expert.

Based on data from the U.S. National Science Foundation and U.S. census, people identifying as Hispanic or Latino, American Indian or Alaska Native, and Black or African American are underrepresented in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) [85,86]. Women are also underrepresented in science, filling approximately 26% of jobs, with underrepresentation even more pronounced among women of color) [87]. Systemic racism in STEM begins in the K-12 classroom and continues beyond higher education, as do issues such as gender essentialism and gender stereotypes [8891]. STEM fields, and shark science in particular, are known to suffer from problems with misogyny, harassment, and discrimination (e.g. [78,9295]). These issues contribute to the historical and current exclusion of people from all STEM fields. Inclusion of all identities is critical for the future of STEM and shark science specifically.

Including more diverse experts in Shark Week episodes will not, of course, address all of the barriers and issues in STEM or shark science. However, Shark Week is the highest-profile media coverage that the field of shark science receives, and overwhelmingly featuring white men signals to viewers (including students who may want to be professional scientists) that one has to be a white man to be a successful scientist [9597]. Indeed, media portrayals of scientists, which often are white and male [97], contribute to how children perceive scientists [9799] and draw scientists [96,99101]. Gendered preconceptions of science being male-dominated can push girls away from science [102]. This becomes a cycle of excluding people from the field, which in turn means that role models with the same identities that are being excluded are few and far between [96]. Counter-stereotyping and access to same-race and same-sex role models can play an important role in making historically excluded groups feel a greater sense of belonging in science, so the availability of role models, including in media, is significant [103105].

The selection methods for experts appearing on Shark Week have an important influence on content. Experts may be selected for media appearances based on a prior existing relationship with the producer or documentary team, or may be asked to vet or recommend other potential experts being considered (e.g., [106]). As people are most likely to have social networks structured around homophily (i.e., primarily composed of people similar to themselves [93]); these recruitment methods can perpetuate a lack of diversity among featured experts. Host/experts are also found through production teams researching social media or published works, such as research papers. This is more likely to favor established, senior researchers with a larger publication record or a higher public profile. It could also result in people with a particularly active social media presence being featured, whether or not they are scientific experts. Even if female experts are found, women tend to be under-recognized for their scientific achievements compared to their male collaborators [107]. The limitations created by this recruitment process are not necessarily insurmountable; Shark Week’s chief competitor, National Geographic’s “Shark Fest,” has partnered with the non-profit Minorities in Shark Sciences to improve diversity among their own hosts [108], while Shark Week has made no such moves publically as of this writing.

Featured species: White sharks, bull sharks, tiger sharks, and hammerheads highlighted.

Including species that were not the focus of an episode but were briefly introduced by name on screen, at least 79 extant (living) species of shark or species groups (e.g. “hammerhead”, “mako”, “sevengill”, “sixgill”, “thresher”, “wobbegong”) were featured in at least one Shark Week episode (Fig 7, S2 Table). Additionally, eight extinct species and 13 species of extant non-shark chondrichthyans were also featured (10 batoids (rays), 3 holocephalans (chimera and ratfish)). 46 extant species were featured in more than one episode, 30 appeared in more than five episodes, and 16 appeared in more than ten episodes. Across all episodes, an average of 4.9 species appeared. 39 episodes showed just one species, and 36 of these single-species episodes featured only white sharks (Carcharodon carcharias). 124 Species were noted in the 21 episodes included in the post-hoc reliability analysis; there was agreement on 110 species (88.7%) and a Cohen’s Kappa of 0.774 (Substantial Agreement). All disagreements except for two came in episodes that featured more than ten species, many just briefly, and none concerned a species that was a primary focus of an episode.

thumbnail
Fig 7. Species appearing in at least 5 Shark Week episodes.

Hammerheads were most commonly great hammerheads S. mokarran, though scalloped hammerheads S. lewini and smooth hammerheads S. zygaena were sometimes mentioned. Conservation status reflects that of great and scalloped hammerheads (Critically Endangered); smooth hammerheads are currently assessed as Vulnerable. Mako sharks were almost always shortfin makos Isurus oxyrinchus, but one longfin mako I. paucus was mentioned (both species are Endangered).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.g007

Across the 201 coded episodes, the most common species featured were white sharks C. carcharias (18.4% of all episodes), tiger sharks Galeocerdo cuvier (12.2% of all episodes), bull sharks Carcharhinus leucas (9.6% of all episodes), and hammerhead sharks Sphyrnidae (8.4% of all episodes) (Fig 7). Often the specific species of hammerhead was not mentioned, so all hammerheads were grouped together for analysis; when species were specified, great hammerheads Sphyrna mokarran were featured most often (62.3%) with the occasional scalloped (21.7%) and smooth (2.9%) hammerheads (S. lewini and S.zygaena, respectively).

The species highlighted show some interesting contrasts with similar analyses of shark species of interest in scientific publications [79] and popular press [32] coverage. While white sharks appear in the top five featured species in Shark Week, scientific publications, and popular press coverage, some of the most-studied species (bonnethead shark Sphyrna tiburo, sandbar shark Carcharhinus plumbeus, and spiny dogfish Squalus acanthias) are rarely featured in any Shark Week episodes (5, 7, and 1 episode, respectively) (S2 Table). Similarly, some of the species that received the most media attention in popular press articles (32) such as the porbeagle Lamna nasus and basking shark Cetorhinus maximus were rarely featured in any Shark Week episodes (2 and 4 episodes, respectively) (S2 Table). As in other forms of popular media, more highly threatened species were not more likely to be featured, with an overall tendency to feature large, charismatic, well-known species.

Featured localities: U.S., Bahamas, and South Africa take the lead.

Though dozens of countries were featured in at least one episode each, a handful of filming locations dominated. The United States was the most common filming location (24.2% of all episodes), followed by the Bahamas and South Africa with 15% each, and New Zealand, Australia, and Mexico with approximately 10% each (Fig 8). Within the United States, 31.5% of episodes took place in California, followed by Florida (26.7% of episodes), Hawaii (17.8% of episodes), and Massachusetts (9.6% of episodes). At least one episode took place in nearly every coastal state’s waters (except for Delaware, and noting that most shows featured in Georgia were filmed at the Georgia Aquarium). However, most states other than Florida, California, Hawaii, and Massachusetts were not featured often, and some states were featured only once. In the post-hoc reliability analysis, 66 locations were identified by coders, who agreed on 63 of those locations, for an agreement of 95.4% and a Kohen’s Kappa of 0.91 (Almost Perfect Agreement). For each location of disagreement, two occurred in episodes briefly featuring many locations and one was missed by one coder.

thumbnail
Fig 8. Shark Week filming locations.

(A) Locations by country; (B) Locations within the United States.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.g008

This geographic focus on just a few countries (and on a relatively small number of locations within those countries) in part reflects a focus on particular species and researchers, though Shark Week episodes regularly feature experts who have no particular experience with a location, but fly in to the location just for filming the episode. Although sharks are circumglobal, familiar and logistically simple sites in which filmmakers have prior experience or existing relationships may be favored for filming [109], potentially acting as a factor that contributes to reducing the diversity of species, locations, narratives, and experts featured. For example, two of the top three filming localities have majority Black populations; 91% of the Bahamian population is Black [110] and approximately 80% of the South African population is Black African [111]. Despite 30% of filming localities being located in these two countries, non-white experts are rarely featured in Shark Week (this study; [46]).

Messaging: Many episodes are contradictory.

174 (86.6%) of the coded episodes had a stated goal at the beginning of the episode. Of these episodes, 64 (36.8%) did not address their stated goals during the course of the episode, and 110 (63.2%) claimed to have accomplished their stated goal. The stated goals varied from specific research goals to answering general questions about shark behavior (see S1 File for examples). If the overarching goal of Shark Week episodes is to educate viewers, it is important that they have a clearly stated purpose and that this purpose is addressed. The fact that this often did not happen suggests that many episodes serve no purpose beyond imagery of sharks. This aligns with Papson’s analysis of the 1990 season of Shark Week, where episodes’ stated goals were to decrease fear of sharks, yet the footage and narration used were designed to promote fear [59].

148 (73.6%) of the coded episodes included some sort of fear-mongering language or negative portrayal of sharks as perceived by the coders. These comments mostly focused on shark bites on humans (Table 1). On the other hand, 127 (63.2%) of the episodes had at least one mention of sharks as awe-inspiring, beautiful, misunderstood, or ecologically important (Table 2). Notably, this was often a brief mention that played over the ending credits, while fear-mongering-type narratives often occurred throughout the episode. Papson identified a similar trend in contradictory messaging in the 1990 season of Shark Week [59,112]. For example, for most episodes of that season, characterization of sharks as evil to simply provide drama for the viewers or served to set up the episode for experts to show that sharks are not evil, although contradictory messaging often reinforces the misconception [112,113]. Papson also notes one episode (“Shark Hunter”) where the shark was left characterized as evil without the positive messaging for the purpose of justifying the fisher’s targeting of sharks [59].

thumbnail
Table 1. Representative example dialogue and narration showing sharks in a negative light.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Representative example dialogue and narration showing sharks in a positive light.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.t002

Of the 21 episodes included in the post-hoc reliability analysis, there were ten that mentioned shark conservation, and coders agreed on nine of them (90% agreement). Three mentioned shark fins, one mentioned shark meat, and no episodes mentioned how viewers can help; coders agreed on 100% of these variables. Nine episodes contained identified “good” statements about sharks, and there was intercoder agreement on eight of those (88.9% agreement). Fifteen episodes had a mention of sharks as bad/scary/harmful, and there was intercoder agreement on 13 of those (86.7% agreement). These sample sizes were too low to calculate Cohen’s Kappa.

As discussed previously, the language used to describe sharks does matter, as studies have shown that negatively valenced words like “attack” can contribute to negative public sentiment towards sharks [14,76,114]. Public acceptance of predators is related to the frequency and intensity of interactions (especially negative interactions) with humans, so support for shark conservation is likely to be related to perceived frequency of bites or human injuries (i.e., “attacks”) [115]. Media portrayal of these issues has indeed been shown to play a role in public support for shark conservation [12]. Muter et al. [17], and Neff and Hueter [114] also found that news stories about sharks largely focus on fear-mongering and exaggerated stories of sharks biting people rather than on shark research or conservation. Exposure to violent Shark Week programming has been shown to induce greater levels of fear of sharks [116], and fear correlates with support for policies, like shark culls or beach netting, that are harmful to shark conservation [117]. However, we note that we cannot assign direct causality between Shark Week programming specifically and viewers supporting harmful policies; this would require experiments and interview with viewers [69,118].

One area in which Shark Week programming may be effective at reducing fear is through episodes including neutral and positive interactions with sharks, which have been shown to improve public perceptions [112]. Episodes of Shark Week in the last several years typically include at least one well-known celebrity interacting with sharks (e.g., Shaquille O’Neal, Will Smith, Ronda Rousey). Celebrity actions, opinions, and endorsements are known to influence the attitudes we adopt and the decisions we make [119], and in conservation specifically, celebrity endorsement of a cause yields higher willingness-to-engage amongst the public [120]. However, it should be noted that Shark Week’s celebrity episodes often feature unnecessary artificial danger or inappropriate interactions with animals such as chasing, riding, or harassing them, which could undermine any positive messaging and potentially endanger people’s safety [121].

In terms of specific threats to sharks and shark conservation, 28 episodes (13.9%) mentioned shark finning or the shark fin trade, and eight (4.0%) mentioned that people eat shark meat. However, Shark Week episodes are generally lacking in actionable educational content about shark conservation. 107 episodes (53.2%) at least briefly mention something related to conservation, often vague statements about shark population decline, the ecological importance of sharks, or extinction risks. Just six episodes (3.0%) mentioned anything specific about shark conservation policy or specific ways that Shark Week’s large audience could help; these statements were mostly about individuals choosing to not eat shark fin soup or releasing sharks they catch. There was no content encouraging viewers to speak to government officials about specific ongoing policy discussions, advising them to avoid specific seafoods with shark bycatch, requesting donations to nonprofits that have a track record of success, or incorporating any other common advice given by experts to those who want to help conserve sharks. When suggestions are provided during programming, many of them are not actionable in any way that could actually be useful to shark conservation efforts, an enormous missed opportunity given Shark Week’s massive audience and the general lack of public pro-sustainability engagement in US shark fisheries discussions [122]. Past attempts to leverage their audience included a 2014 social media ad with five ways that people could help sharks, which included “report shark attacks” and “avoid shark fishing in marinas” as suggestions without explanation. The most specific was “lobby for shark protection,” but no information was provided on who to lobby or what to ask them to do. Exposing a large audience to vague platitudes is of questionable value for conservation and may even undermine existing campaigns [122].

Recommendations

The scale of Shark Week’s platform to communicate with the public about sharks means that even minor adjustments to programming could have a meaningful effect. Some of the simplest improvements involve reducing harmful sensationalism (including perceptions of the dangerousness of many activities), enhancing factual accuracy and raising editorial standards, and clearly distinguishing between fact-based and fictional programming. Similarly, explicit differentiation between credentialed scientific experts and non-scientist experts would be helpful in avoiding inadvertently legitimizing incorrect information.

In portrayals of science and scientists, it would be helpful to feature real science and more realistic scientific methods (even if recreated or dramatized), a wider range of shark species, and a more diverse range of scientists. These changes would likely help with factual accuracy while also benefiting the diversification of shark science, recruitment in STEM, and public recognition of the work of scientists from historically excluded groups. While not all science may be especially camera-friendly, there is plenty of real scientific research that is important and interesting to watch.

In terms of the effects these changes might have on the public, some studies of students of varying ages have shown that increasing knowledge about animals increases positive attitudes towards those animals [123,124], including for sharks in particular [125,126]. Television has the potential to drive conservation action or intention—for example, an increase in internet searches for conservation charities and sustainable practices were seen during and after the airing of Blue Planet II episodes [126,127]. This does not mean that providing the public with positive representations of sharks, or accurate information about them, represents a solution to conservation problems or will necessarily generate interest or concern about them in itself [128,129]. However, evidence suggests playing on existing negative stereotypes—even with an intent to challenge them—can actually serve to reinforce them (e.g. [113]). Best practices for improving the public image of sharks include shifting away from negative stereotypes and providing detailed information about how conservation problems connect to people’s lives and what actions they can take to help [112].

The majority of Shark Week episodes do contain at least some educational content about sharks (often vague or brief mentions), with most episodes falling into the (broad) categories of Research or Natural History (Fig 2). Even episodes focused on bites or attacks can offer some educational value when they include scientifically accurate information, though this is often undermined by conflicting messages and sensationalism, as in programs which terrorize viewers and then briefly mention shark conservation as the credits roll. While conservation content may not be appropriate for every episode, providing actionable steps for viewers is necessary in order for them to move from positive attitudes towards behavior that supports shark conservation; viewers who are misinformed or under-informed about key issues related to conservation are unlikely to support expert-backed policy solutions in useful ways [32]. For example, a small number of episodes correctly connected shark fishing for meat and fins to the current population decline of sharks [3,4,8]. However, no episodes linked these facts to specific actions the audience could take to make a difference, and only six episodes included anything arguably specific and detailed about conservation.

Conclusion

Shark Week has a complicated history over the course of its 30+ years, and has received substantial criticism for scientific inaccuracy, while also unquestionably increasing the public attention paid to sharks. This analysis attempted to quantitatively assess some of the trends and practices seen in Shark Week programming that have been anecdotally discussed for decades.

Our analyses demonstrate that the majority of episodes are not focused on shark bites, although such episodes are common and many titles and episodes are framed around fear, risk, and adrenaline. Shark Week episodes contain many of the same cinematic structures as fictional films featuring sharks, which serve to build suspense and keep people watching [47,59]. Including tangential mentions, a surprising number and diversity of shark species have been featured, although anecdotal descriptions of disproportionate attention to particular large charismatic species are supported by our data. Shark Week’s depictions of research and of scientists are biased towards particular research methodologies and (mostly white, mostly male) scientists, including non-scientists being presented as scientific experts even as they share incorrect information. Our results, coupled with the scholarly literature, suggest that as a whole, Shark Week is likely contributing to collective perceptions of sharks as monsters, and that even relatively small alterations to programming decisions could substantially improve the presentation of sharks and shark science and conservation issues.

This requires a complex balance of Shark Week’s potentially competing goals to educate and entertain audiences and contribute to conservation. If Shark Week does not retain viewers, any efforts to improve programs’ educational and conservation impact will not be meaningful. These competing imperatives have been recognized since at least the 1940s with radio shows such as Great Moments in Science and television’s The Nature of Things [130] successfully combining entertainment and accurate educational content. Successful, scientifically accurate programming featuring Don Herbert (“Mr. Wizard”), Carl Sagan, and Bill Nye, among others, succeeded because hosts displayed excellent and entertaining communication skills [130,131]. Programming featuring stunning visuals and music such as Blue Planet and Planet Earth effectively entices viewers with ‘visual and aural pleasure’ (sensu [132]) while also delivering accurate educational information (but see [133] for how background music can influence perceptions of sharks).

Given its popularity and global viewership, Shark Week has the potential to generate interest in both sharks and scientific careers among viewers. However, Shark Week fails to feature the full range of shark research topics and methods and the diversity of people performing research on sharks. Cultivating a positive attitude toward sharks through Shark Week has the potential to drive enhanced support of shark and ocean conservation efforts. Currently, Shark Week can be seen as a missed opportunity to benefit sharks, shark science, and shark conservation.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Shark Week episodes by year.

Blue column indicates whether the episode was included in the content analysis and the initials of the coder that analyzed the episode. Yellow columns indicate titles deemed negative using the Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW), using context (phrases instead of individual words), and with the ANEW and context combined.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.s001

(PDF)

S2 Table. Chondrichthyan species featured in Shark Week episodes and number of episodes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.s002

(PDF)

S1 File. Coding guidelines for the content analysis of Shark Week episodes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0256842.s003

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We thank J. Carrier and R. Hueter for providing personal copies of episodes for our use. We also thank R. Kajiura for providing his professional independent analysis of episode titles and A. Knupsky for pointing us toward the ANEW. C. Bailey and B. Davis gave invaluable advice on demographic analysis. We appreciate the feedback and suggestions from C. Pepin-Neff, P. Klimley, and our anonymous reviewers. This study was inspired by conversations held in Allegheny College’s 2019 FS101 course “Sharks and Recreation”, taught by author LBW—thank you to the Sharks and Rec students for their conversations and insights.

References

  1. 1. Heithaus MR, Frid A, Wirsing AJ, Worm B. Predicting ecological consequences of marine top predator declines. Trends Ecol Evol. 2008;23: 202–10. pmid:18308421
  2. 2. Heithaus M, Frid A, Vaudo J, Worm B, Wirsing A. Unraveling the ecological importance of elasmobranchs. In: Carrier JC, Musick JA, Heithaus M, editors. Sharks and their relatives II: Biodiversity, adaptive physiology, and conservation. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2010. pp. 611–38.
  3. 3. Dulvy NK, Fowler SL, Musick JA, Cavanagh RD, Kyne PM, Harrison LR, et al. Extinction risk and conservation of the world’s sharks and rays. eLife. 2014;3:e00590. pmid:24448405
  4. 4. Dulvy NK, Pacoureau N, Rigby CL, Pollom RA, Jabado RW, Ebert DA, et al. Overfishing drives over one-third of all sharks and rays toward a global extinction crisis. Current Biology. 2021;31: 4773,4787.e8. pmid:34492229
  5. 5. Stein RW, Mull CG, Kuhn TS, Aschliman NC, Davidson LNK, Joy JB, et al. Global priorities for conserving the evolutionary history of sharks, rays and chimaeras. Nat Ecol Evol. 2018;2: 288–98. pmid:29348644
  6. 6. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species [Internet].; 2021 [cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://www.iucnredlist.org/en.
  7. 7. MacNeil MA, Chapman DD, Heupel M, Simpfendorfer CA, Heithaus M, Meekan M, et al. Global status and conservation potential of reef sharks. Nature. 2020;583: 801–6. pmid:32699418
  8. 8. Pacoureau N, Rigby CL, Kyne PM, Sherley RB, Winker H, Carlson JK, et al. Half a century of global decline in oceanic sharks and rays. Nature. 2021;589: 567–71. pmid:33505035
  9. 9. Musick JA, Bonfil R. Management techniques for elasmobranch fisheries. Food & Agriculture Org.; 2005.
  10. 10. Jacques PJ. The social oceanography of top oceanic predators and the decline of sharks: A call for a new field. Prog Oceanogr. 2010,;86: 192–203.
  11. 11. Ferguson K. Submerged realities: shark documentaries at depth. Atenea. 2006;26: 115–29.
  12. 12. O’Bryhim JR, Parsons ECM. Increased knowledge about sharks increases public concern about their conservation. Mar Policy. 2015;56: 43–7.
  13. 13. Glithero L, Zandvliet D. Canadian ocean literacy survey highlights. 2020.
  14. 14. Pepin-Neff CL. Flaws: shark bites and emotional public policymaking. Cham: Springer; 2019.
  15. 15. Peschak T. Sharksand shark bite in the media. In: Nel DC, Peschak TP, editors. Finding balance: white shark conservation and recreational safety in in shore waters of Cape Town, South Africa: Proceedings of a specialist workshop. World Wildlife Fund; 2006. pp. 159–63.
  16. 16. Philpott R. Why sharks may have nothing to fear more than fear itself: an analysis of the effect of human attitudes on conservation of the great white shark. Colo J Int Environ Law Policy. 2002;13: 445–72.
  17. 17. Muter BA, Gore ML, Gledhill KS, Lamont C, Huveneers C. Australian and U.S. news media portrayal of sharks and their conservation. Conserv Biol. 2013;27: 187–96. pmid:23110588
  18. 18. Asen R. Imagining the public sphere. Philos Rheto. 2002;35: 345–67.
  19. 19. Jarvis JL. Shark fin soup: Collective imagination in the transnational public sphere. Glob Media J Can ed. 2019;11: 49–64.
  20. 20. Busque BL, Roetman P, Dorrian J, Litchfield C. An analysis of Australian news and current affair program coverage of sharks on Facebook. Conserv Sci Pract. 2019;1: e111.
  21. 21. Neff C. The Jaws Effect: How movie narratives are used to influence policy responses to shark bites in Western Australia. Austr J Pol Sci 2015;50(1): 114–127.
  22. 22. Sabatier E, Huveneers C. Changes in media portrayal of human-wildlife conflict during duccessive fatal shark bites. Conserv Soc. 2018;16: 338–350.
  23. 23. Boissonneault MF, Gladstone W, Scott P, Cushing N. Grey nurse shark human interactions and portrayals: a study of newspaper portrayals of the grey nurse shark from 1969–2003‬. Electr Green J. 2005;1: 1–21.‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬
  24. 24. Simpfendorfer CA, Heupel MR, White WT, Dulvy NK, Simpfendorfer CA, Heupel MR, et al. The importance of research and public opinion to conservation management of sharks and rays: a synthesis. Mar Freshw Res. 2011;62: 518–27.
  25. 25. Mittermeier JC, Roll U, Matthews TJ, Grenyer R. A season for all things: Phenological imprints in Wikipedia usage and their relevance to conservation. PLOS Biol. 2019;17(3): e3000146. pmid:30835729
  26. 26. Upwell Tide Report: Son of Sharkinar, Shark Conversation. And more sharks. [Internet]. [cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://us5.campaign-archive.com/?u=c880da0c24096213459a64f11&id=03d6ce99cc.
  27. 27. Morfoot A. Inside the Shark Week Vs. SharkFest Battle for Cable and Streaming Viewers [Internet].; 2021 [updated -07-09; cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://variety.com/2021/tv/news/shark-week-vs-sharkfest-battle-cable-streaming-1235015047/.
  28. 28. Huveneers C, Ebert DA, Dudley SFJ. The evolution of chondrichthyan research through a metadata analysis of dedicated international conferences between 1991 and 2014. Afr J Mar Sci. 2015;37:129–39.
  29. 29. Eilperin J. Shark Week at 25: Discovery seeks to keep fest relevant, builds conservation ties. Washington Post. 2012 Aug 09.
  30. 30. Narula SK, Lynch K. What Shark Experts Really Think About Shark Week [Internet].; 2018 [updated -07-26; cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://www.outsideonline.com/culture/books-media/what-shark-experts-really-think-about-shark-week/.
  31. 31. Shiffman D | Perspective Shark scientists explain what’s right and what’s wrong with Shark Week. Washington Post. 2018 July 24.
  32. 32. Shiffman DS, Bittick SJ, Cashion MS, Colla SR, Coristine LE, Derrick DH, et al. Inaccurate and biased global media coverage underlies public misunderstanding of shark conservation threats and solutions. iScience. 2020;23: 101205. pmid:32553133
  33. 33. Duhaime-Ross A. How Shark Week screws scientists [Internet].; 2014 [updated -08-13; cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://www.theverge.com/2014/8/13/5998745/how-shark-week-screws-scientists.
  34. 34. Shiffman D. Shark Week Lied to Scientists to Get Them to Appear in "Documentaries" [Internet].; 2014 [updated Aug 11,; cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://gizmodo.com/shark-week-lied-to-scientists-to-get-them-to-appear-in-1619280737.
  35. 35. Evans S. Shark Week and the rise of infotainment in science documentaries. Commun Res Rep. 2015;32: 265–71.
  36. 36. Davidson J. Discovery Channel Provokes Outrage with Fake Shark Week Documentary [Internet].; 2013 [updated Aug 07,; cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://entertainment.time.com/2013/08/07/discovery-channel-provokes-outrage-with-fake-shark-week-documentary/.
  37. 37. The Real Megalodon: Prehistoric Shark Behind Doc Uproar [Internet].; 2013 [updated 08-08-; cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://www.nationalgeographic.com/animals/article/130807-discovery-megalodon-shark-week-great-white-sharks-animals.
  38. 38. Shimada K. The size of the megatooth shark, Otodus megalodon (Lamniformes: Otodontidae), revisited. Hist Biol. 2021;33: 904–11.
  39. 39. Pimiento C, Ehret DJ, MacFadden BJ, Hubbell G. Ancient nursery area for the extinct giant shark Megalodon from the Miocene of Panama. PloS One. 2010; 2010(E10552).
  40. 40. Pimiento C, Clements CF. When did Carcharocles megalodon become extinct? A new analysis of the fossil record. Plos One. 2014;9(10): e111086. pmid:25338197
  41. 41. Pimiento C, Balk MA. Body-size trends of the extinct giant shark Carcharocles megalodon: a deep-time perspective on marine apex predators. Paleobiology. 2015;41(3): 479–90. pmid:26321775
  42. 42. Gottfried MD, Compagno L, Bowman SC. Size and skeletal anatomy of the giant “megatooth” shark Carcharodon megalodon. In: Klimley AP, Ainely DG, editors. Great white sharks: The biology of Carcharodon carcharias. San Diego: Academic Press; 1996. pp. 55–66.
  43. 43. Boessenecker RW, Ehret DJ, Long DJ, Churchill M, Martin E, Boessenecker SJ. The Early Pliocene extinction of the mega-toothed shark Otodus megalodon: a view from the eastern North Pacific. PeerJ. 2019;7: e6088. pmid:30783558
  44. 44. Ophir Y, Jamieson KH. The effects of media narratives about failures and discoveries in science on beliefs about and support for science. Public Underst Sci. 2021: 09636625211012630.
  45. 45. Aupers S. ‘Trust no one’: Modernization, paranoia and conspiracy culture. Eur J Commun. 2012,;27: 22–34.
  46. 46. Dehnart A. Shark Week and Sharkfest’s experts are mostly white men. Why? And does it matter? [Internet].; 2020 [updated -08-04; cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://www.realityblurred.com/realitytv/2020/08/shark-week-shark-fest-2020-diversity/.
  47. 47. Palmer C. Confessions of a Wildlife Filmmaker: A Memoir: The Challenges of Staying Honest in an Industry Where Ratings Are King. Bluefield Publishing; 2015.
  48. 48. Minorities in Shark Science [Internet]. [cited Aug 9, 2021]. Available from: http://www.misselasmo.org/.
  49. 49. Hill A. Restyling factual TV: Audiences and news, documentary and reality genres. Routledge; 2007.
  50. 50. Blue Planet II Captivated Audiences Abroad. Why Didn’t America Care? [Internet]. 2018 [updated -03-13T22:41:20+00:00; cited Aug 12, 2021]. Available from: https://slate.com/technology/2018/03/why-didnt-america-care-about-blue-planet-ii.html.
  51. 51. NBC’s NHL swan song not too bad by recent standards. [Internet]. 2021 [-07-09T12:47:54–05:00]. Available from: https://www.sportsmediawatch.com/2021/07/stanley-cup-final-ratings-nbc-lightning-canadiens-viewership/
  52. 52. Pedersen E. Shark Week Attack: Discovery’s Sunday Primetime Shows Lead All Networks In Key Demos. [Internet]. 2018 [-07-27T19:51:52+00:00]. Available from: https://deadline.com/2018/07/shark-week-ratings-shaquille-oneal-ronda-rousey-1202435244/
  53. 53. Tseng Y, Tsai C, Hsieh P, Hung J, Huang T. The relationship between exposure to pseudoscientific television programmes and pseudoscientific beliefs among Taiwanese university students. Int J Sci Educ B Commun Public Engagem. 2014;4:107–22.
  54. 54. Tsai C, Shein PP, Jack BM, Wu K, Chou C, Wu Y, et al. Effects of exposure to pseudoscientific television programs upon Taiwanese citizens’ pseudoscientific beliefs. Int J Sci Educ B Commun Public Engagem. 2012;2: 175–94.
  55. 55. Declercq ER, Sakala C, Corry MP, Applebaum S. Listening to mothers II: Report of the second national U.S. survey of women’s childbearing experiences: Conducted January-February 2006 for Childbirth Connection by Harris Interactive(R) in partnership with Lamaze International. J Perinat Educ. 2007;16: 15–7.
  56. 56. Morris T, McInerney K. Media representations of pregnancy and childbirth: an analysis of reality television programs in the United States. Birth. 2010;37: 134–40. pmid:20557536
  57. 57. Grimes DR. Medical disinformation and the unviable nature of COVID-19 conspiracy theories. PLOS ONE. 2021;16: e0245900. pmid:33711025
  58. 58. Boissat L, Thomas-Walters L, Veríssimo D. Nature documentaries as catalysts for change: Mapping out the ‘Blackfish Effect’. People Nat. 2021;3: 1179–1192.
  59. 59. Papson S. “Cross the Fin Line of Terror”: Shark Week on the Discovery Channel. J Am Cult. 1992;15: 67–81.
  60. 60. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings. 1999.
  61. 61. Gow EA, Burant JB, Sutton AO, Freeman NE, Grahame ERM, Fuirst M, et al. Popular press portrayal of issues surrounding free-roaming domestic cats Felis catus. People Nat. 2022;4:143–154.
  62. 62. Nuendorf KA. Content analysis—a methodoloical primer for gender research. Sex Roles 2011;64: 276–289.
  63. 63. Shiffman D. Shark Week 2016 episode reviews. [Internet]. 2016. [-07-04T02:10:40+00:00]. Available from: https://www.southernfriedscience.com/shark-week-2016-episode-reviews/
  64. 64. Lang JT, Hallman WK. Who does the public trust? The case of genetically modified food in the United States. Risk Anal. 2005;25: 1241–1252. pmid:16297228
  65. 65. Dietrich DR, Dekant W, Greim H, Heslop-Harrison P, Berry C, Boobis A, et al. Allowing pseudoscience into EU risk assessment processes is eroding public trust in science experts and in science as a whole: The bigger picture. Chemico-Biol Interact. 2016;257: 1–3. pmid:27453574
  66. 66. Korownyk C, Kolber MR, McCormack J, Lam V, Overbo K, Cotton C, et al. Televised medical talk shows—What they recommend and the evidence to support their recommendations: A prospective observational study. Brit Med J. 2014;349:g7346. pmid:25520234
  67. 67. Cukier W, Jackson S, Gagnon S. The representation of women and racialized minorities as expert sources on-air in Canadian public affairs television. Can J Commun. 2019;44: 25–47.
  68. 68. Castro JI. The Sharks of North America. Oxford University Press. 2010.
  69. 69. McCagh C, Sneddon J, Blache D. Killing sharks: The media’s role in public and political response to fatal human–shark interactions. Mar Pol. 2015;62: 271–278.
  70. 70. Hamilton JT. Channeling Violence: The Economic Market for Violent Television Programming. Princeton University Press; 1998.
  71. 71. Sunstein CR, Zeckhauser R. Overrreaction to fearsome risks. Envtal Resour Econ 2011;48: 435–449.
  72. 72. Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 2004;24: 2.
  73. 73. Sobkow A, Traczyk J, Zaleskiewicz T. The affective bases of risk perception: Negative feelings and stress mediate the relationship between mental imagery and risk perception. Front Psychol. 2016;7: 932. pmid:27445901
  74. 74. Martin GN. (Why) Do you like scary movies? A review of the empirical research on psychological responses to horror films. Front Psychol. 2019;10: 2298.
  75. 75. Zhang Z, Li X. Controversy is marketing: mining sentiments in social media. 2010 43rd Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences, 2010; pp. 1–10.
  76. 76. O’Donnell K. Shark Week and public perceptions of sharks. Ph.D. Dissertation, Duke University. 2019.
  77. 77. Butcher PA, Colefax AP, GorkinIII RA, Kajiura SM, López NA, Mourier J, et al. The drone revolution of shark science: A review. Drones. 2021;5: 8.
  78. 78. Macdonald C. The Dark Side of Being a Female Shark Researcher [Internet].; 2020 [updated Aug 10,; cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/the-dark-side-of-being-a-female-shark-researcher/.
  79. 79. Shiffman DS, Ajemian MJ, Carrier JC, Daly-Engel TS, Davis MM, Dulvy NK, et al. Trends in chondrichthyan research: An analysis of three decades of conference abstracts. Copeia. 2020;108: 122–31.
  80. 80. Zurawski R. How does television represent science? Ph.D. Dissertation, Mount Saint Vincent University. 2010.
  81. 81. Howell L, Singer JB. Pushy or a princess? Women experts and British broadcast news. Journalism Practice. 2017 21;11: 1062–78.
  82. 82. Manne K. Down Girl: The Logic of Misogyny. Oxford University Press; 2017.
  83. 83. Rudman LA, Moss-Racusin CA, Phelan JE, Nauts S. Status incongruity and backlash effects: Defending the gender hierarchy motivates prejudice against female leaders. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2012;48: 165–79.
  84. 84. Holderman LB. Media‐constructed anti‐intellectualism: The portrayal of experts in popular US television talk shows. New Jersey J Commun. 2003;11: 45–62.
  85. 85. Women, Minorities, and Persons with Disabilities in Science and Engineering. Alexandria, VA: National Science Foundation; 2021 April 29.
  86. 86. United States Census QuickFacts. United States Census Bureau; 2021 July 1.
  87. 87. Landivar LC. Disparities in STEM employment by sex, race, and Hispanic origin. US Department of Commerce; 2013 September.
  88. 88. Young H. Secondary education systemic issues: Addressing possible contributors to a leak in the science education pipeline and potential solutions. J Sci Educ Technol. 2005;14: 205–216.
  89. 89. O’Brien L, Bart H, Garcia D. Why are there so few ethnic minorities in ecology and evolutionary biology? Challenges to inclusion and the role of sense of belonging. Soc Psychol Educ. 2020;23: 449–477.
  90. 90. Nosek B, Smyth F, Sriram N, Lindner N, Devos T, Ayala A, et al. National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106: 10593–7. pmid:19549876
  91. 91. Ceci SJ, Williams WM, Barnett SM. Women’s underrepresentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychol Bull. 2009;135: 218–61. pmid:19254079
  92. 92. Clancy KBH, Nelson RG, Rutherford JN, Hinde K. Survey of Academic Field Experiences (SAFE): Trainees Report Harassment and Assault. PLOS ONE. 2014;9: e102172. pmid:25028932
  93. 93. St. John K, Riggs E, Mogk D. Sexual harassment in the sciences: A call to geoscience faculty and researchers to respond. J Geosci Educ. 2016;64: 255–7.
  94. 94. Whitenack LB. Speaking out about sexual harassment in shark science. 2017 Nov 21. In: Southern Fried Science. Available from: https://www.southernfriedscience.com/speaking-out-about-sexual-harassment-in-shark-science/
  95. 95. Long M, Boiarsky G, Thayer G. Gender and racial counter-stereotypes in science education television: A content analysis. Public Underst Sci. 2001;10: 255–69.
  96. 96. Macdonald C. Media representation and gender bias in science. J Environ Media. 2021;2: 7–15.
  97. 97. Ryan L, Steinke J. "I WANT TO BE LIKE…" Middle school students’ identification with scientists on television. Sci Scope. 2010;34: 44–49.
  98. 98. Tan A, Jocz JA, Zhai J. Spiderman and science: How students’ perceptions of scientists are shaped by popular media. Public Underst Sci. 2017;26: 520–530. pmid:26582070
  99. 99. Steinke J, Lapinski MK, Crocker N, Zietsman-Thomas A, Williams Y, Evergreen SH, et al. Assessing media influences on middle school–aged children’s perceptions of women in science using the Draw-A-Scientist Test (DAST). Sci Commun. 2007;29: 35–64.
  100. 100. Buldu M. Young children’s perceptions of scientists: a preliminary study. Educ Res. 2006; 48:121–132.
  101. 101. Song J, Kim K. How Korean students see scientists: the images of the scientist. Int J Sci Educ. 1999;21: 957–977.
  102. 102. Lee JD. Which kids can "become" scientists? Effects of gender, self-concepts, and perceptions of scientists. Soc Psychol Q. 1998;61:199–219.
  103. 103. Good JJ, Woodzicka JA, Wingfield LC. The effects of gender stereotypic and counter-stereotypic textbook images on science performance. J Soc Psychol. 2010;150: 132–147. pmid:20397590
  104. 104. O’Brien LT, Hitti A, Shaffer E, Camp ARV, Henry D, Gilbert PN. Improving girls’ sense of fit in science: Increasing the impact of role models. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2017;8: 301–309.
  105. 105. Van Camp AR, Gilbert PN, O’Brien LT. Testing the effects of a role model intervention on women’s STEM outcomes. Soc Psychol Educ. 2019;22: 649–671.
  106. 106. Steele JE. Experts and the operational bias of television news: The case of the Persian Gulf War. Journal Mass Commun Q. 1995;72:799–812.
  107. 107. Rossiter MW. The Matthew Matilda effect in science. Soc Stud Sci. 1993;23: 325–341.
  108. 108. National Geographic and Nat Geo WILD Partner With Minorities in Shark Science (MISS) to Make Waves for SHARKFEST 2022. BuisnessWire 2021 -05–18.
  109. 109. Alfred SR, Lambert Jr. JT. Systemic practice and action research. Syst Pract Action Res. 2012;25: 323–54.
  110. 110. 2010 Census of Population and Housing. United States Census Bureau; 2012.
  111. 111. Lehohla P Census 2011: Census in brief. Statistics South Africa; 2012.
  112. 112. Panoch R, Pearson EL. Humans and sharks: Changing public perceptions and overcoming fear to facilitate shark conservation. Soc Anim. 2017;25: 57–76.
  113. 113. Weaver R, Ferguson C, Wilbourn M, Salamonson Y. Men in nursing on television: exposing and reinforcing stereotypes. J Adv Nurs. 2014;70: 833–42. pmid:24001311
  114. 114. Neff C, Hueter R. Science, policy, and the public discourse of shark “attack”: a proposal for reclassifying human–shark interactions. J Environ Studies Sci. 2013;3: 65–73.
  115. 115. Kleiven J, Bjerke T, Kaltenborn BP. Factors influencing the social acceptability of large carnivore behaviours. Biodivers Conserv. 2004;13:1647–1658.
  116. 116. Myrick JG, Evans SD. Do PSAs take a bite out of Shark Week? The effects of juxtaposing environmental messages with violent images of shark attacks. Sci Commun. 2014;36: 544–569.
  117. 117. Pepin-Neff C, Wynter T. Shark bites and shark conservation: An analysis of human attitudes following shark bite incidents in two locations in Australia. Conserv Lett. 2018;11: e12407.
  118. 118. Fraser-Baxter S, Medvecky F. Evaluating the media’s reporting of public and political responses to human-shark interactions in N.S.W, Australia. Mar Pol. 2018;97: 109–118.
  119. 119. Viale PH. Celebrities and medicine: A potent combination. J Adv Pract Oncol. 2014;5: 82–84. pmid:25032040
  120. 120. Duthie E, Veríssimo D, Keane A, Knight AT. The effectiveness of celebrities in conservation marketing. PLOS ONE. 2017;12: e0180027. pmid:28686595
  121. 121. Shiffman DS. Shark Riders Pose Threat to Conservation Gains Made with Diving Ecotourism [Internet].; 2014 [updated March 3,; cited Aug 12, 2021]. Available from: https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/shark-riders-pose-threat-to-conservation-gains-made-with-diving-ecotourism-slide-show1/.
  122. 122. Ford AT, Ali AH, Colla SR, Cooke SJ, Lamb CT, Pittman J, et al. Understanding and avoiding misplaced efforts in conservation. Facets. 2021;6: 252–s71.
  123. 123. Barney EC, Mintzes JJ, Yen C. Assessing knowledge, attitudes, and behavior toward charismatic megafauna: The case of dolphins. J Environ Educ. 2005;36:41–55.
  124. 124. Prokop P, Tunnicliffe SD. “Disgusting” animals: Primary school children’s attitudes and myths of bats and spiders. Eurasia J Math Sci Technol Educ. 2008;4: 87–97.
  125. 125. Thompson TL, Mintzes JJ. Cognitive structure and the affective domain: On knowing and feeling in biology. Int J Sci Educ. 2002;24: 645–660.
  126. 126. Hayns-Worthington S.The Attenborough effect: Searches for plastic recycling rocket after Blue Planet II [Internet].; 2018 [updated Jan 5,; cited Aug 6, 2021]. Available from: https://resource.co/article/attenborough-effect-searches-plastic-recycling-rocket-after-blue-planet-ii-12334.
  127. 127. Hynes S, Ankamah-Yeboah I, O’Neill S, Needham K, Xuan BB, Armstrong C. The impact of nature documentaries on public environmental preferences and willingness to pay: entropy balancing and the Blue Planet II effect. J Environ Plan and Manag. 2021;64): 1428–1456.
  128. 128. Jones JPG, Thomas-Walters L, Rust NA, Veríssimo D. Nature documentaries and saving nature: Reflections on the new Netflix series Our Planet. People Nat. 2019;1: 420–425.
  129. 129. Braun T, Cottrell R, Dierkes P. Fostering changes in attitude, knowledge and behavior: demographic variation in environmental education effects. Environ Educ Res. 2018;24: 899–920.
  130. 130. Chotkowski Lafollette M. Science on the Air: Popularizers and personalities on radio and early television. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2008.
  131. 131. Scott E. Science communication: Science fresh from the box. Nature. 2012;492:184–185.
  132. 132. Wheatley H. The Limits of television?: Natural history programming and the transformation of public service broadcasting. Eur J of Cult Stud. 2004;7: 325–339.
  133. 133. Nosal AP, Keenan EA, Hastings PA, Gneezy A. The effect of background music in shark documentaries on viewers’ perceptions of sharks. PLOS ONE. 2016;11: e0159279. pmid:27487003