Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

What is the level of evidence for the amnestic effects of sedatives in pediatric patients? A systematic review and meta-analyses

  • Karolline Alves Viana,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Programa de Pós-graduação em Odontologia, Universidade Federal de Goiás (UFG), Goiânia, Goiás (GO), Brazil

  • Anelise Daher,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Programa de Pós-graduação em Odontologia, Universidade Federal de Goiás (UFG), Goiânia, Goiás (GO), Brazil

  • Lucianne Cople Maia ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Lucianne Cople Maia, Paulo Sucasas Costa, Carolina de Castro Martins, Saul Martins Paiva

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departmento de Odontopediatria e Ortodontia, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

  • Paulo Sucasas Costa ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Lucianne Cople Maia, Paulo Sucasas Costa, Carolina de Castro Martins, Saul Martins Paiva

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departmento de Pediatria, Faculdade de Medicina, UFG, Goiânia, GO, Brazil

  • Carolina de Castro Martins ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Lucianne Cople Maia, Paulo Sucasas Costa, Carolina de Castro Martins, Saul Martins Paiva

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Odontopediatria e Ortodontia, Faculdade de Odontologia, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil

  • Saul Martins Paiva ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Lucianne Cople Maia, Paulo Sucasas Costa, Carolina de Castro Martins, Saul Martins Paiva

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Departamento de Odontopediatria e Ortodontia, Faculdade de Odontologia, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil

  • Luciane Rezende Costa

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    lsucasas@ufg.br

    Affiliation Departamento de Saúde Oral, UFG, Goiânia, GO, Brazil

Abstract

Background

Studies have suggested that benzodiazepines are amnestic drug par excellence, but when taken together, what level of evidence do they generate? Are other sedatives as amnestic as benzodiazepines? The aim of this study was to assess the level of scientific evidence for the amnestic effect of sedatives in pediatric patients who undergo health procedures.

Methods

The literature was searched to identify randomized controlled trials that evaluated anterograde and retrograde amnesia in 1-19-year-olds who received sedative drugs during health procedures. Electronic databases, including PubMed, Scopus and Cochrane Library besides clinical trial registries and grey literature were searched. Two independent reviewers performed data extraction and risk of bias assessment using the Cochrane Collaboration's Tool. The meta-analyses were performed by calculating relative risk (RR) to 95% confidence intervals (CI). The quality of the evidence was assessed using Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation approach.

Results

Fifty-four studies were included (4,168 participants). A higher occurrence of anterograde amnesia was observed when benzodiazepines, the most well-studied sedatives (n = 47), were used than when placebo was used (n = 12) (RR = 3.10; 95% CI: 2.30–4.19, P<0.001; I2 = 14%), with a moderate level of evidence. Higher doses of alpha2-adrenergic agonists (clonidine/dexmedetomidine) produced more anterograde amnesia than lower doses (n = 2) (RR = 1.83; 95% CI: 1.03–3.25; P = 0.038; I2 = 0%), with a low level of evidence; benzodiazepines’ amnestic effects were not dose-dependent (n = 3) (RR = 1.54; 95% CI: 0.96–2.49; P = 0.07; I2 = 12%) but the evidence was low. A qualitative analysis showed that retrograde amnesia did not occur in 8 out of 10 studies.

Conclusions

In children, moderate evidence support that benzodiazepines induce anterograde amnesia, whereas the evidence for other sedatives is weak and based on isolated and small studies. Further clinical trials focused on the amnesia associated with non-benzodiazepine sedatives are therefore needed.

Trial registration

PROSPERO CRD42015017559.

Introduction

Health procedures can lead to fear, anxiety and behavior management problems in children and adolescents. Pediatric patients can benefit from sedation, a pharmacological approach that aims to control anxiety and behavior, reduce physical discomfort, promote patient safety, and minimize the chance of psychological trauma by maximizing the potential for amnesia [1].

It is widely accepted that some responsiveness is expected during sedation. Specifically, patients may respond normally or purposefully to a stimulus [2]. However, sedation can fail in some situations, meaning that the patient has unwanted degrees of uncooperative behavior that requires intervention, such as protective stabilization. Over the past five years, it has been shown that the success rate of sedation in children and adolescents in a medical or a dental setting ranges from 26.7% [3] to 96.2% [4]. Thus, if pediatric patients are responsive and/or show a distressed reaction while sedated, the question of whether they remember perioperative events is an important one. In the practice of anesthesia, amnesia is a therapeutically desirable effect [5] that is considered one of the key pillars of the triad of anesthesia [6]. Remembering an aversive stimulus can lead to the development of psychological trauma [7]. In fact, it has been demonstrated that memories of distressing events play a significant role in the development of dental phobias [8]. Moreover, children that experience traumatic clinical procedures are expected to display negative behaviors in future dental appointments [9,10].

Thus, amnesia is an important component of sedation for pediatric patients who will undergo uncomfortable healthcare procedures. Some sedatives can impair memory temporarily either directly, via drug interference with memory process (drug-induced amnesia), or indirectly, by impairing attention and arousal secondary to their sedative effects [11,12]. A wide variety of drugs have been investigated to explore their effects on either anterograde or retrograde amnesia, which refer to the inability to consciously recall information presented after or before drug intake, respectively [12]. Although the memory effects of sedatives have been reported in children, it is still unclear to what extent amnesia is present in clinical practice [11, 1319]. The degree of amnesia can differ according to the drug, the measurement used to study memory, and the characteristics of the participants [11]. While in some studies, all participants presented complete anterograde amnesia for procedure [1317], in other investigations, only a few patients failed to remember perioperative events [18,19]. New knowledge regarding the potential of different sedatives to produce amnesia would therefore be pivotal to the decision-making process that takes place when choosing a sedative regimen for a pediatric patient because young children can show uncooperative behavior even when sedated.

Interestingly, the outcomes of randomized clinical trials on sedation related amnesia have not been pooled for systematic analyses. Hence, the aim of this systematic review was to highlight the level of evidence for the amnestic effects of sedatives in pediatric patients who undergo health procedures.

Methods

Protocol and registration

The methods used to perform this systematic review were previously reported as a study protocol (S1 Protocol) [20]. This study is registered in the PROSPERO database under the number CRD4201501755. The present report follows the recommendations of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA; S1 Appendix) [21].

Eligibility criteria

The eligibility criteria were chosen using the PICOS (population, intervention, comparator, outcomes and study design) strategy: 1) population: pediatric patients aged 1–19 years old who received sedative drugs as a premedication or as agents for procedural sedation; 2) intervention: any sedative regimen administered by a health professional in an outpatient setting or operating room; 3) comparator: placebo, variations of the same sedative regimen (i.e., dosage, route and timing of administration) or an alternative sedation regimen; 4) outcome: anterograde amnesia (primary endpoint) was defined as the loss of memories of events that occurred after sedative administration, while retrograde amnesia (secondary endpoint) was defined as the loss of memories of events that occurred before sedative administration; and 5) study design: randomized controlled trials (RCT).

Studies involving patients with cognitive or neurological impairments in addition to RCTs that reported pooled and undifferentiated data on both adolescents and adults were excluded. No restrictions were placed on the date of publication, the publication status or the publication language.

Search strategy and information sources

The search strategy was developed under the guidance of a librarian. Controlled vocabulary, synonyms, related terms and free terms related to children/adolescents, sedatives and memory were combined and searched without filters or limits. The search strategy followed the syntax rules of each database (S1 Table). One reviewer (KAV) performed the electronic searches between September 26th and October 1st 2015, and another (AD) reviewed them to check for errors.

To identify trials eligible for inclusion in this review, the searches were performed in the electronic bibliography databases of the Public Medical Literature Analysis and Retrieval System Online (PubMed), Scopus, The Cochrane Library, the Latin American and Caribbean Health Sciences Literature database (LILACS), the Brazilian Library in Dentistry (BBO), the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL), the Web of Science, the Excerpta Medica Database (EMBASE) and PsycINFO. Additionally, the grey literature was searched in the OpenGrey, "ProQuest dissertations and Theses full text" and Periódicos Capes. Theses databases through the Brazilian agency Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior–CAPES). To locate unpublished and ongoing trials, the following trial registries were searched: Current Controlled Trials, the US National Institutes of Health, the Brazilian Clinical Trials Registry (Registro Brasileiro de Ensaios Clínicos–ReBEC) and the UK National Institute for Health and Care Excellence.

Study selection and data collection process

Study selection was performed in duplicate by two independent and calibrated reviewers (KAV and AD). First, duplicate references were removed using the software program EndNote® (EndNote X7, Thomson Reuters, New York, USA). Next, as a training and calibration exercise, the independent reviewers applied the eligibility criteria to 10% of the titles/abstracts of the retrieved studies, and inter-examiner agreement was calculated. This exercise was repeated until there was almost perfect agreement, which was achieved at a Kappa coefficient ≥ 0.8 [22] (Kappa = 0.81; CI 95% 0.70–0.92). Disagreements were resolved by consensus using the supervision of a gold standard (LRC). Finally, each independent reviewer selected the remaining studies by their titles and abstracts based on the eligibility criteria.

The full-text of the potentially eligible studies, which were those that at least one reviewer regarded as having met the inclusion criteria, were read and judged based on the eligibility criteria. Discrepancies were solved by a third reviewer (LRC).

Two trained independent reviewers (KAV and AD) performed data collection in duplicate. A standardized data collection form was developed and pilot-tested using a randomized sample of three included trials. This form was refined accordingly. The following data were recorded for each included study: article identification characteristics; study design; patient information; type of intervention and comparisons used; whether the sedative was used as procedural sedation or premedication; setting (i.e., outpatient setting or operating room) and treatment performed; type of memory or amnesia; type of outcome measurement; statistical techniques used and results of the study analysis. Disagreements were solved by consensus.

Risk of bias in individual studies

The risk of bias assessment in the included studies was evaluated using the Cochrane Collaboration’s Tool for Assessing Risk of Bias in Randomized Trials [23]. The assessment criteria contained seven specific domains: random sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding of participants and personnel, blinding of outcome assessment, incomplete outcome data, selective reporting and other bias.

For each domain, the risk of bias was graded as high, low or unclear based on criteria described in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic reviews of Interventions 5.1.0 (http://handbook.cochrane.org) [24]. Only four out of the seven domains were considered key domains for assessing the risk of bias of the studies: random sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding of participants and personnel, and blinding of outcome assessment. Studies were considered to be at 'low' risk of bias when there was 'low' risk of bias in all of these key domains. When the study was judged as 'high' or 'unclear' in one of these key domains, it was considered, respectively, at 'high' or 'unclear' risk of bias.

Any disagreements between the reviewers were solved by consensus or by consulting a third reviewer (LRC).

Synthesis of the results

A narrative summary of study characteristics is provided in the text and presented in the tables. Outcome results/conclusions are shown as numerical data and/or statistical results when this information were available in studies. Differences are reported as statistically significant if the trial reported P<0.05.

Included studies with low and uncertain risk of bias were analyzed in relation to clinical and methodological heterogeneity to determine whether a meta-analysis could be performed. Among the studies that could be meta-analyzed, statistical heterogeneity was evaluated using chi-squared tests and Higgins and Thompson’s I2. When heterogeneity was substantial (I2 >50%, P<0.1) [24], a sensitivity analysis was performed to explore the influence of each study on the pooled data. To summarize the data obtained from each study, the relative risk (RR) was calculated with a 95% confidence interval (CI). A random-effects model was used. All analyses were conducted using The Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software program, version 3 (Biostat, Inc., Englewood, USA). Publication bias was evaluated by visually inspecting the funnel plot and using Egger’s test when at least 10 studies were included in meta-analysis. For other studies, bias was assessed by verifying the presence of both significant and non-significant outcomes.

The quality of the included evidence was evaluated using the approach described in Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) [25]. The assessments were based on study design, risk of bias, presence of imprecision, inconsistency, indirectness and publication bias.

Results

Study selection

A total of 6,112 studies were identified in the search. After duplicates were removed, 3,178 studies remained. Screening of the titles and abstracts resulted in the exclusion of 2,894 records (Fig 1). The main reasons for exclusion were non-related subjects (n = 2,550), non-RCT study (n = 208) and other setting (n = 89). Of the 284 potentially eligible studies, 54 met the inclusion criteria and were included in the systematic review. Among these, the data from 16 studies were meta-analyzed: 12 were pooled to compare benzodiazepines vs placebo, 3 to compare dosages of benzodiazepines and 2 to compare dosages of alpha2-adrenergic agonists. One study was included in two different meta-analyses.

Characteristics of included articles

The demographic (location/year) and intervention (sedative use/procedure/setting) characteristics of all the included articles were summarized in Table 1, whereas detailed characteristics of each study were stated in S2 to S6 Tables.

The included studies were performed in 20 different countries, and most of them (n = 19, 35.2%) were performed in the United States [7,1416, 2640]. The studies were published from 1969 to 2015.

The number of patients included in the studies ranged from 12 [32] to 260 [34] (total = 4,168). The age range varied from 0 months to 18 years, but the outcome ‘memory’ was not evaluated in younger children (<1 year).

In almost half of studies, sedatives were used as agents for procedural sedation [1315,19,2730,3234,3639, 4151] instead of premedication, and half of the trials were performed in an outpatient setting. In a great deal of studies, only medical procedures were performed [7,1319,26,28,30,31,3335,3840,46,47,5265].

A wide variety of sedative regimens were compared. The studies differed according to the drug used and its dose and route, and there were additional differences in the comparator arm. Benzodiazepines, used solely or in combination, were the most well-studied drugs: only 7 out 54 trials did not include a benzodiazepine group [13,17,30,38,46,49,65]. Drugs currently used in clinical practice are reported in the following studies: midazolam was investigated in 33 trials [7, 1416, 19, 27, 28, 31, 33, 35, 3945, 47, 48, 5054, 59, 61, 63, 6671], nitrous oxide in seven [26, 30, 34, 42, 46, 50, 51], ketamine in eight [1317, 26, 34, 37, 44, 49, 70], dexmedetomidine in one [49] and propofol in four [13, 14, 46, 47]. Besides, all drugs investigated in 23 studies were the ones currently used: 19 involved midazolam [7, 19, 27, 31, 33, 35, 40, 41, 5052, 54, 61, 63, 6670], three nitrous oxide [46, 50, 51], four ketamine [13, 17, 49, 70], one dexmedetomidine [49] and two propofol [13,46].

Anterograde amnesia was evaluated in all studies, whereas retrograde amnesia was assessed in only 10 studies [7,33,35,37,39,40,52,54,60,66]. Amnesia was evaluated mainly by measuring patient recall of pictures/toys and events, except for the six trials [13,41,16,19,38,42]. The results that were reported in all of the trials were predominantly dichotomous (e.g. the presence/absence of amnesia/recall).

Risk of bias in included studies

The risk of bias assessment performed on the included studies is presented in Fig 2. Studies included in meta-analyses were underlined in this figure. Most of the studies were found to have unclear risk (n = 32; 59.3%), 11 had low risk, and 9 had high risk.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Risk of bias assessment of the included studies (The Cochrane Collaboration tool for assessing risk of bias).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.g002

Considering the key domains, in the majority of trials (n = 30, 55.6%), the method used for sequence generation was unclear, as was the reporting of the allocation concealment. The domain ‘blinding of participants and health care providers’ was judged ‘low’ in 40 full text articles (74.1%), and a similar result was found for the domain ‘blinding of outcome assessment’ (n = 39; 72.2%).

Regarding other domains, most studies (n = 29; 53.7%) had low risk for the domain 'incomplete outcome data assessment'. Nearly all studies were considered to have a low risk of bias with regard for selective reporting (n = 50, 92.6%) and other biases (n = 53, 98.1%).

Evidence synthesis

Because a wide variety of sedative regimens were investigated in the included studies, we categorized the trials into five comparison topics with regard for the outcome ‘anterograde amnesia’: 1. single benzodiazepine versus placebo (S2 Table), 2. single benzodiazepine versus single benzodiazepine (S3 Table), 3. single benzodiazepine versus non-benzodiazepine drug (S4 Table), 4. benzodiazepine in combination with other drugs versus any sedative (S5 Table), and 5. non-benzodiazepine drug versus non-benzodiazepine drug (S6 Table). We attempted to compare sedatives with placebo; where this was not possible, we considered benzodiazepines as standard amnestic drugs.

Trials that evaluated multiple comparisons are presented only once in the tables according to the sequence of categorizations listed above. However, for evidence synthesis and to assess the quality of the presented evidence (Table 2), trials were considered to belong to more than one topic when needed, and in these cases, we took into account only the data related to the drugs being compared. In this systematic review, we excluded the trials’ participants who did not participate in the memory assessment; so, the number of individuals was adjusted to perform meta-analyses as well as in the tables’ column "outcome result/conclusion". No differences were found in the following comparisons when the studies were pooled according to the type of procedure performed (medical or dental) and the setting (outpatient or operating room).

thumbnail
Table 2. Quality of evidence on sedatives’ amnestic effects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.t002

Anterograde amnesia.

1. Single benzodiazepine versus placebo

Benzodiazepines used alone were compared to placebos in 17 studies [7,18,19,3133,35,36,40,52,58,59,63, 64,66,68,69], twelve of which were included in the meta-analysis. The remaining trials were excluded because it was impossible to group the data [40,52,63] or there was an overall high risk of bias [64] or after the sensitivity analysis [35]. In the last case, when the study was removed, heterogeneity decreased from 91% to 14%.

The meta-analysis of 12 studies included 746 pediatric patients and revealed that patients who received benzodiazepines displayed stronger anterograde amnesia (223/418) than the patients who received placebo (50/328) (RR = 3.10; 95% CI: 2.30–4.18; P<0.001; I2 = 14%). This association remains in the analysis of subgroups, when considering sedative used as premedication (RR = 2.80; 95% CI: 2.04–3.85, P<0.001; I2 = 16%,) or as agents for procedural sedation (RR = 7.20; 95% CI: 2.87–18.07, P<0.001; I2 = 0%) (Fig 3). The benzodiazepines included in the studies were midazolam, lorazepam, diazepam, temazepam and triazolam.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Forest plot of the meta-analysis performed to compare amnestic effects between benzodiazepines and placebos analyzed by subgroups.

Premedication: RR = 2.80; 95% CI: 2.04–3.85, P<0.001; Procedural sedation: RR = 7.20; 95% CI: 2.87–18.07, P<0.001; Overall: RR = 3.10; 95% CI: 2.30–4.18, P<0.001. Heterogeneity: premedication I2 = 16%, P = 0.30; procedural sedation I2 = 0%, P = 0.96; overall I2 = 14%, P = 0.30.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.g003

The funnel plot for these data demonstrates that the included studies were asymmetrically distributed and that there was a lack of small studies falling toward the left of the mean effect. These results are in agreement with Egger's test, which revealed there was statistically significant publication bias (P = 0.001) (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Funnel plot of studies that compared amnestic effects between benzodiazepines and placebos.

Egger’s test: P = 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.g004

The quality of the evidence was moderate, indicating a moderate level of confidence in the effect estimate. Further research is therefore likely to both impact confidence in the estimate of the effect and change the estimate [25]. The quality of the evidence was downgraded because of concerns about publication bias (Table 2).

2. Single benzodiazepine versus single benzodiazepine

Benzodiazepines were compared to each other in 17 studies [7,18,27,29,31,39,41,43,53,54,55,57,59,61,62,64,67]. Six of these studies compared different benzodiazepines [18,39,43,53,57,62], six compared dosages [29,31,41,55,61,64], one compared the time of administration [7], and 4 compared routes [27,54,59,67]. Only four studies found significant differences among groups: in one study, intravenous midazolam was more likely than intravenous diazepam to produce amnesia for pain, but there was no difference with regard for amnesia of events [39]. Other studies have shown that anterograde amnesia is more likely to occur when oral midazolam is used than when oral diazepam was used [53], when oral flunitrazepam was used than when oral diazepam was used [62], and when a higher dose of triazolam was delivered [29].

A meta-analysis of 3 studies (n = 55) that compared dosages demonstrated that higher doses of benzodiazepines did not favor the occurrence of anterograde amnesia compared to lower doses (RR = 1.54; 95% CI: 0.96–2.49; P = 0.07; I2 = 12%) (Fig 5).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Forest plot of the meta-analysis performed to compare amnestic effects between dosages of benzodiazepines.

Overall effect: RR = 1.54; 95% CI: 0.96–2.49; P = 0.07. Heterogeneity: I2 = 12%, P = 0.32.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.g005

The quality of evidence was low, indicating that because there was limited confidence in the effect estimate, further research is very likely to both impact confidence in the estimate of the effect and change the estimate [25]. The quality of the evidence was therefore downgraded because of the degree of imprecision that was observed in these studies (Table 2).

3. Single benzodiazepine versus non-benzodiazepine drug

Benzodiazepines were compared to non-benzodiazepines in 13 studies [18,44,45,48,50,51,5660,70,71]. In four studies, anterograde amnesia was significantly more likely when benzodiazepines were used than when nitrous oxide [50], antihistamines [45,48,60], or triclofos [48] were used. However, one study showed the anterograde amnesia was less likely to occur when benzodiazepines were used than when butorphanol was used [71].

The quality of the evidence was judged as low in consideration of the degree of imprecision that was observed (Table 2).

4. Benzodiazepine in combination with another drug versus any sedative

Benzodiazepines were used in combination with another drug and compared to any sedative in 11 studies [1416,26,28,34,37,42,44,47,70]. Only three studies found that anterograde amnesia was significantly more likely in the benzodiazepine group than when another sedative, including meperidine, promethazine and chlorpromazine [26], meperidine alone [28], or melatonin plus nitrous oxide [42] was used. One study showed that anterograde amnesia was more likely when propofol was used than when midazolam was used [47].

A meta-analysis was not performed because there was substantial clinical heterogeneity.

Given the imprecision that was observed in these studies, the quality of evidence was determined to be low (Table 2).

5. Non-benzodiazepine drugs versus non-benzodiazepine drugs

Eight studies compared non-benzodiazepine drugs [13,17,30,38,42,46,49,65]. Anterograde amnesia was more likely when nitrous oxide/propofol was used instead of propofol/lidocaine [46], or oral ketamine was used instead of oral dexmedetomidine [49]. Considering the alpha2-adrenergic agonists (clonidine 2mcg/kg versus 1mcg/kg PO and dexmedetomidine 5mcg/kg versus 3mcg/kg PO), a meta-analysis of 2 studies (n = 96) found that higher doses of them increased occurrence of anterograde amnesia compared to lower doses (RR = 1.83; 95% CI: 1.03–3.25; P = 0.038; I2 = 0%) (Fig 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Forest plot of the meta-analysis performed to compare amnestic effects between dosages of alpha2-adrenergic agonists.

Overall effect: RR = 1.83; 95% CI: 1.03–3.25; P = 0.04; I2 = 0%, P = 1.000.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.g006

Imprecision downgraded the quality of the evidence to low (Table 2).

Retrograde amnesia.

Retrograde amnesia was assessed in ten studies [7,33,35,37,39,40,52,54,60,66]. Six studies compared midazolam to placebo [7,33,35,40,52,66] and showed contradictory results: in one study, greater recall was reported after oral midazolam (0.5 mg/kg) was used [7], while the other studies found that patients had equivalent recall after midazolam was administered via different routes [33,40,52,66] or a lower rate of recall after oral midazolam (0.5 mg/kg) was used [35]. No differences were reported in other studies that compared midazolam to diazepam [39], midazolam delivered via two routes [54], benzodiazepine delivered with antihistamine [60], or ketamine delivered in different doses with diazepam [37].

The quality of the evidence in this group was low (Table 2).

Discussion

This systematic review was performed to determine the level of scientific evidence to support the amnestic effects of different sedatives in pediatric patients. While some commonly used drugs do appear to have the potential to induce anterograde amnesia, the only group of studies that provided more than a low level of evidence were those that compared benzodiazepines to placebos, both when the sedative was used as premedication or as agent for procedural sedation. The findings related to retrograde amnesia remain controversial and with low level of evidence.

In nearly all of the included trials, memory was assessed using the study-test paradigm, which included a learning phase, a retention interval, and a test phase [5]. However, trials differed greatly in the target materials presented during learning and the duration of the retention interval. Differences between memory tasks are to be expected because there is currently no accepted standard reference memory test [5]. It can therefore be argued that a subject’s responses during a task aimed at evaluating the recall of materials, such as pictures and toys, may not reflect their responses in real life situations [5]. Nevertheless, the current systematic review found no difference in the rate of amnesia between events and pictures. Some studies have assessed the recall of both events and pictures, and there was no difference in the rate of amnesia achieved according to stimuli.

Pediatric patients that received benzodiazepines were three times more likely to experience anterograde amnesia than those who received a placebo. The likelihood of experiencing anterograde amnesia was higher when the sedatives were used as agent for procedural sedation than when used as premedication. In fact, it is widely believed that benzodiazepines impair the retention of memories of information acquired after the administration of a benzodiapine drug [72]. According to the present results, the benzodiazepine-related amnesia is independent of the method employed to evaluate it, including whether the means of recall involved pictures or events. It has been shown that memory impairments remain even when a task that mimics a real world situation is used [52,73]. Hence, benzodiazepines impair memories of study items in addition to personally relevant events [52,73].

The apparent lack of dose response of anterograde amnesia induced by benzodiazepines might be due to the fact that the lowest doses of benzodiazepines can cause sufficient amnesia, thus increasing the dose does not increase amnesia. However, the results related to dose-dependent amnesia should be viewed with skepticism due to the poor nature and imprecision of the 3 studies included in the meta-analyses. The level of evidence for this analysis was low, and it should be noted that the lower limit of the confidence interval was 0.96. Similarly, even though there was a significant dose-related amnesia in children that received alpha2-adrenergic agonists in the 2 studies included, in agreement with a previous animal study [74], the level of evidence was also low, with the lower limit of the confidence interval being 1.03.

The studies included in this systematic review provided different results regarding amnesia related to information that was acquired before sedative administration (retrograde amnesia). Interestingly, whereas nearly all of the studies reported equivalent recall when a benzodiazepine was compared with placebo or active drug [52,66,33,39,54,60,37,40], one trial demonstrated that using a sedative increased recall [7]. Improved recall of material acquired before the administration of a benzodiazepine has been reported in previous laboratory-based studies. In these cases, retrograde facilitation may be secondary to anterograde amnesia: the reduced ability to learn information after drug intake has been associated with lower levels of interference and a decrease in the chance of forgetting information that was acquired before drug intake [75,76].

Despite the importance of including a placebo control group when assessing the influence of a drug on learning and performance, nearly 70% of the included studies failed to use a placebo, probably for ethical reasons. Nonetheless, among the non-placebo-controlled studies, almost all of the trials (n = 30/36) included a group that was treated with a benzodiazepine, which is considered a standard drug for comparisons with other sedatives [12]. When considering a comparison between benzodiazepines, two studies found that greater anterograde amnesia was produced by midazolam than diazepam, and these findings were in accordance with those reported in another systematic review [77].

It is indisputable that the gold standard design for drug studies is a randomized, parallel, placebo-controlled and double-blinded trial [12]. The current systematic review considered results from only randomized controlled trials. To identify relevant studies, including both published and unpublished studies, a sensitive search strategy was implemented and used to search in different electronic databases without restrictions on language or the date of publication. Additionally, all steps were performed by two trained and calibrated reviewers, and this minimized errors and reduced potential biases [5].

The present systematic review and meta-analysis has some limitations. First, in several studies (n = 45), memory was not a primary outcome, indicating that it was not the most important outcome that was examined and that it was therefore not a basis for estimating the sample size [78]. This could have compromised the external validity of the results of these studies. Second, it was not possible to rule out the possibility that publication bias could have impaired the results of the meta-analysis. However, efforts were made to obtain unpublished, potentially relevant articles. Third, five potentially eligible papers could not be captured. However, because a large number of articles were assessed in this review, it should not be expected that the inclusion of a small number of additional articles would have altered the results. Finally, the studies included in the meta-analyses presented some clinical heterogeneity based on the age ranges of their participants and the methods used to assess amnesia. Nevertheless, these features did not impact our results according to the sensitivity analysis.

The evidence used to show that benzodiazepines produce greater rates of anterograde amnesia than placebo were of moderate quality, regardless of the age of the participants, the method used to assess memory, and the stimulus (study items or real events) being evaluated. However, the broad range of sedatives that were used in these studies and the wide variety of drug comparisons that were evaluated resulted in significant heterogeneity among the studies. Additionally, the evidence for other sedatives was of low quality and possessed limited generalizability. For these reasons, benzodiazepines can be considered a preferred option in a clinical setting when amnesia is desired.

Conclusions

In this systematic review, we found that randomized clinical trials investigating the amnestic effects of sedatives in pediatric patients are heterogeneous, which made it difficult to obtain a high level of evidence to support conclusions relating to this topic. Nevertheless, the anterograde amnesia that is produced by benzodiazepines is well-demonstrated, and the likelihood of anterograde amnesia is higher when these sedatives are used as agents for procedural sedation than when used as premedication: the quality of evidence supporting its efficacy is moderate. The evidence for other sedatives is based only on isolated and small trials, and it should therefore be viewed with caution. The lack of high-quality evidence regarding the amnestic effects of non-benzodiazepine sedatives in children/adolescents suggests that future randomized clinical trials aimed at studying pediatric sedation should include amnesia (or, more accurately measures of memory performance) as the primary or key secondary end-point. Recommendations regarding the quality of methodology used to assess memory function should be adhered to closely.

Supporting information

S1 Protocol. Copy of the registration protocol in PROSPERO.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.s001

(PDF)

S1 Table. Search strategy used for some database searches.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.s003

(DOC)

S2 Table. Amnestic effects: Comparisons between benzodiazepines and placebos.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.s004

(DOC)

S3 Table. Amnestic effects of benzodiazepines: Comparison among benzodiazepines.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.s005

(DOC)

S4 Table. Amnestic effects: Comparisons between benzodiazepines and non-benzodiazepine sedatives.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.s006

(DOC)

S5 Table. Amnestic effects: Comparisons between benzodiazepines in combination with other drugs and any sedative.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.s007

(DOC)

S6 Table. Amnestic effects: Comparisons of non-benzodiazepine drugs and non-benzodiazepine drugs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0180248.s008

(DOC)

References

  1. 1. Coté CJ, Wilson S, American Academy of Pediatrics, American Academy of Pediatric Dentistry. Guidelines for monitoring and management of pediatric patients before, during, and after sedation for diagnostic and therapeutic procedures: Update 2016. Pediatrics. 2016;138(1). pmid:27354454
  2. 2. American Society of Anesthesiologists. Continuum of depth of sedation definition of general anesthesia and levels of sedation/analgesia [homepage on the Internet]. 2014. Available from: http://www.asahq.org/~/media/Sites/ASAHQ/Files/Public/Resources/standards-guidelines/continuum-of-depth-of-sedation-definition-of-general-anesthesia-and-levels-of-sedation-analgesia.pdf
  3. 3. Toomarian L, Salem K, Ansari G. Assessing the sedative effect of oral vs submucosal meperidine in pediatric dental patients. Dent Res J. 2013;10: 173–9. pmid:23946732
  4. 4. Li BL, Yuen VM, Song XR, Ye J, Ni J, Huang JX, et al. Intranasal dexmedetomidine following failed chloral hydrate sedation in children. Anaesthesia. 2014;69: 240–4. pmid:24447296
  5. 5. Ghoneim MM. Drugs and human memory (Part 1): Clinical, theoretical, and methodologic issues. Anesthesiology. 2004;100: 987–1002. pmid:15087638
  6. 6. Veselis RA. The problem with amnesia. Anesthesiology. 2012;117: 940–1. pmid:22929728
  7. 7. Kain ZN, Hofstadter MB, Mayes LC, Krivutza DM, Alexander G, Wang SM, et al. Midazolam: Effects on amnesia and anxiety in children. Anesthesiology. 2000;93:.676–84. pmid:10969300
  8. 8. van Houtem CMHH, van Wijk AJ, de Jongh A. Presence, content, and characteristics of memories of individuals with dental phobia. Appl Cognit Psychol. 2015;29: 515–23.
  9. 9. Pringle B, Dahlquist LM, Eskenazi A. Memory in pediatric patients undergoing conscious sedation for aversive medical procedures. Health Psychol. 2003;22: 263–9. pmid:12790253
  10. 10. Jensen B, Schröder U. Acceptance of dental care following early extractions under rectal sedation with diazepam in preschool children. Acta Odontol Scand. 1998;56: 229–32. pmid:9765015
  11. 11. Ghoneim MM, Hinrichs JV. Drugs, memory, and sedation: Specificity of effects. Anesthesiology. 1997;87: 734–6. pmid:9357870
  12. 12. Ghoneim MM. Drugs and human memory (Part 2): Clinical, theoretical, and methodologic issues. Anesthesiology. 2004;100: 1277–97. pmid:15114228
  13. 13. Ghadami Yazdi A, Ayatollahi V, Hashemi A, Behdad S, Ghadami Yazdi E. Effect of two different concentrations of propofol and ketamine combinations (ketofol) in pediatric patients under lumbar puncture or bone marrow aspiration. Iran J Ped Hematol Oncol. 2014;3: 187–92. pmid:24575262
  14. 14. Godambe SA, Elliot V, Matheny D, Pershad J. Comparison of propofol/fentanyl versus ketamine/midazolam for brief orthopedic procedural sedation in a pediatric emergency department. Pediatrics. 2003;112: 116–23. pmid:12837876
  15. 15. Lee-Jayaram JJ, Green A, Siembieda J, Gracely EJ, Mull CC, Quintana E, et al. Ketamine/midazolam versus etomidate/fentanyl: procedural sedation for pediatric orthopedic reductions. Pediatr Emerg Care. 2010;26: 408–12. pmid:20502386
  16. 16. Marx CM, Stein J, Tyler MK, Nieder ML, Shurin SB, Blumer JL. Ketamine-midazolam versus meperidine-midazolam for painful procedures in pediatric oncology patients. J Clin Oncol. 1997;15: 94–102. pmid:8996129
  17. 17. Zsigmond EK, Kovacs V, Fekete G. A new route, jet-injection for anesthetic induction in children—II. Ketamine dose-range finding studies. Int J Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1996;34: 84–8. pmid:8929752
  18. 18. Burtles R, Astley B. Lorazepam in children. A double-blind trial comparing lorazepam, diazepam, trimeprazine and placebo. Br J Anaesth. 1983;55: 275–9. pmid:6132612
  19. 19. Hedén L, Von Essen L, Frykholm P, Ljungman G. Low-dose oral midazolam reduces fear and distress during needle procedures in children with cancer. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2009;53: 1200–4. pmid:19688837
  20. 20. Viana KA, Daher A, Maia LC, Costa PS, Martins CC, Paiva SM et al. Memory effects of sedative drugs in children and adolescents–protocol for a systematic review. Syst Rev. 2016;5: 34. pmid:26892743
  21. 21. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6: e1000097. pmid:19621072
  22. 22. Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33: 159–74. 843571 pmid:843571
  23. 23. Higgins JP, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. The Cochrane Collaborations tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2011;343: d5928. pmid:22008217
  24. 24. Higgins J, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions, version 5.1 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from: http://handbook.cochrane.org
  25. 25. Atkins D, Best D, Briss PA, Eccles M, Falck-Ytter Y, Flottorp S, et al. Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ. 2004;328: 1490. pmid:15205295
  26. 26. Auden SM, Sobczyk WL, Solinger RE, Goldsmith LJ. Oral ketamine/midazolam is superior to intramuscular meperidine, promethazine, and chlorpromazine for pediatric cardiac catheterization. Anesth Analg. 2000;90: 299–305. pmid:10648310
  27. 27. Aydintug YS, Okcu KM, Guner Y, Gunaydin Y, Sencimen M, et al. Evaluation of oral or rectal midazolam as conscious sedation for pediatric patients in oral surgery. Mil Med. 2004; 169: 270–3. pmid:15132227
  28. 28. Bahal-O'Mara N, Nahata MC, Murray RD, Linscheid TR, Fishbein M, Heitlinger LA, et al. Sedation with meperidine and midazolam in pediatric patients undergoing endoscopy. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 1994;47: 319–23. pmid:7875182
  29. 29. Coldwell SE, Awamura K, Milgrom P, Depner KS, Kaufman E, Preston KL, et al. Side effects of triazolam in children. Pediatr Dent. 1999;21: 18–25. pmid:10029963
  30. 30. Evans JK, Buckley SL, Alexander AH, Gilpin AT. Analgesia for the reduction of fractures in children: A comparison of nitrous oxide with intramuscular sedation. J Pediatr Orthop. 1995;15: 73–7. pmid:7883933
  31. 31. Feld LH, Negus JB, White PF. Oral midazolam preanesthetic medication in pediatric outpatients. Anesthesiology. 1990;73: 831–4. pmid:2240672
  32. 32. Flaitz CM, Nowak AJ, Hicks MJ. Evaluation of the anterograde amnesic effect of rectally administered diazepam in the sedated pedodontic patient. ASDC J Dent Child. 1986;53: 17–20. pmid:3455953
  33. 33. Friedman AG, Mulhern RK, Fairclough D, Ward PM, Baker D, Mirro J, et al. Midazolam premedication for pediatric bone marrow aspiration and lumbar puncture. Med Pediatr Oncol. 1991;19: 499–504. pmid:1961137
  34. 34. Kennedy RM, Porter FL, Miller JP, Jaffe DM. Comparison of fentanyl/midazolam with ketamine/midazolam for pediatric orthopedic emergencies. Pediatrics. 1998;102: 956–63. pmid:9755272
  35. 35. Liacouras CA, Mascarenhas M, Poon C, Wenner WJ. Placebo-controlled trial assessing the use of oral midazolam as a premedication to conscious sedation for pediatric endoscopy. Gastrointest Endosc. 1998;47: 455–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0016-5107(98)70244-5 pmid:9647368
  36. 36. Raadal M, Coldwell SE, Kaakko T, Milgrom P, Weinstein P, Perkis V, et al. A randomized clinical trial of triazolam in 3- to 5-year-olds. J Dent Res. 1999;78: 1197–203. pmid:10371242
  37. 37. Sullivan DC, Wilson CF, Webb MD. A comparison of two oral ketamine-diazepam regimens for the sedation of anxious pediatric dental patients. Pediatr Dent. 2001;23: 223–31. pmid:11447952
  38. 38. Terndrup TE, Dire DJ, Madden CM, Gavula D, Cantor RM. Comparison of intramuscular meperidine and promethazine with and without chlorpromazine: A randomized, prospective, double-blind trial. Ann Emerg Med. 1993;22: 206–11. pmid:8427433
  39. 39. Tolia V, Fleming SL, Kauffman RE. Randomized, double-blind trial of midazolam and diazepam for endoscopic sedation in children. Dev Pharmacol Ther. 1990;14: 141–7. pmid:2194770
  40. 40. Twersky RS, Hartung J, Berger BJ, McClain J, Beaton C. Midazolam enhances anterograde but not retrograde amnesia in pediatric patients. Anesthesiology. 1993;78: 51–5. pmid:8424571
  41. 41. Davies FC, Waters M. Oral midazolam for conscious sedation of children during minor procedures. J Accid Emerg Med. 1998;15: 244–8. pmid:9681307
  42. 42. Isik B, Baygin O, Bodur H. Premedication with melatonin vs midazolam in anxious children. Paediatr Anaesth. 2008;18: 635–41. pmid:18616492
  43. 43. Jensen B, Schröder U, Månsson U. Rectal sedation with diazepam or midazolam during extractions of traumatized primary incisors: A prospective, randomized, double-blind trial in Swedish children aged 1.5–3.5 years. Acta Odontol Scand. 1999;57: 190–4. pmid:10540928
  44. 44. Koirala B, Pandey RK, Saksen AK, Kumar R, Sharma S. A comparative evaluation of newer sedatives in conscious sedation. J Clin Pediatr Dent. 2006;30: 273–6. pmid:16937848
  45. 45. Kupietzky A, Holan G, Shapira J. Intranasal midazolam better at effecting amnesia after sedation than oral hydroxyzine: a pilot study. Pediatr Dent. 1996;18: 32–4. pmid:8668567
  46. 46. Lembert N, Wodey E, Geslot D, Ecoffey C. Prevention of pain on injection of propofol in children: comparison of nitrous oxide with lidocaine. Ann Fr Anesth Reanim. 2002;21: 263–70. pmid:12033094
  47. 47. Sienkiewicz E, Albrecht P, Ziółkowski J, Dziechciarz P. Propofol-alfentanyl versus midazolam-alfentanyl in inducing procedural amnesia of upper gastrointestinal endoscopy in children-blind randomised trial. Eur J Pediatr. 2015;174: 1475–80. pmid:25956274
  48. 48. Singh N, Pandey RK, Saksena AK, Jaiswal JN. A comparative evaluation of oral midazolam with other sedatives as premedication in pediatric dentistry. J Clin Pediatr Dent. 2002;26: 161–4. pmid:11874008
  49. 49. Singh C, Pandey RK, Saksena AK, Chandra G. A comparative evaluation of analgo-sedative effects of oral dexmedetomidine and ketamine: A triple-blind, randomized study. Paediatr Anaesth. 2014;24: 1252–9. pmid:25065424
  50. 50. Wilson KE, Girdler NM, Welbury RR. A comparison of oral midazolam and nitrous oxide sedation for dental extractions in children. Anaesthesia. 2006;61: 1138–44. pmid:17090232
  51. 51. Wilson KE, Welbury RR, Girdler NM. Comparison of transmucosal midazolam with inhalation sedation for dental extractions in children. A randomized, cross-over, clinical trial. Acta Anaesthesiol Scan. 2007;51: 1062–7. pmid:17697301
  52. 52. Buffett-Jerrott SE, Stewart SH, Finley GA, Loughlan HL. Effects of benzodiazepines on explicit memory in a paediatric surgery setting. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2003;168: 377–86. pmid:12845417
  53. 53. Chavarry Broncales CS, Segura de la Cruz MS, Salazar Briceño L. Sedation in pediatrics pre anesthesia: study comparative of diacepam and midazolam. Actas Peru Anestesiol. 1995;8: 74–80.
  54. 54. De Jong PC, Verburg MP. Comparison of rectal to intramuscular administration of midazolam and atropine for premedication of children. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 1988;32: 485–9. pmid:3176836
  55. 55. Fell D, Gough MB, Northan AA, Henderson CU. Diazepam premedication in children. Plasma levels and clinical effects. Anaesthesia. 1985;40: 12–7. pmid:3970333
  56. 56. Gordon NH, Turner DJ. Oral paediatric premedication: A comparative trial of either phenobarbitone, trimeprazine or diazepam with hyoscine, prior to guillotine tonsillectomy. Br J Anaesth. 1969;41: 136–42. pmid:4886768
  57. 57. Lindgren L, Saarnivaara L, Himberg JJ. Comparison of i.m. pethidine, diazepam and flunitrazepam as premedicants in children undergoing otolaryngological surgery. Br J Anaesth. 1979;51: 321–7. pmid:37862
  58. 58. Padfield NL, Twohig MM, Fraser ACL. Temazepam and trimeprazine compared with placebo as premedication in children. An investigation extended into the first 2 weeks at home. Br J Anaesth. 1986;58: 487–93. pmid:2870728
  59. 59. Payne KA, Coetzee AR, Mattheyse FJ. Midazolam and amnesia in pediatric premedication. Acta Anaesthesiol Belg. 1991;42: 101–5. pmid:1927235
  60. 60. Peters CG, Brunton JT. Comparative study of lorazepam and trimeprazine for oral premedication in paediatric anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth. 1982;54: 623–8. pmid:6123337
  61. 61. Piotrowski R, Petrow N. Rectal premedication with midazolam in children. A comparative clinical study. Anaesthesist. 1989;38: 16–21. pmid:2645808
  62. 62. Richardson FJ, Manford ML. Comparison of flunitrazepam and diazepam for oral premedication in older children. Br J Anaesth. 1979;51: 313–7. 37861 pmid:37861
  63. 63. Stewart SH, Buffett-Jerrott SE, Finley GA, Wright KD, Valois Gomez T. Effects of midazolam on explicit vs implicit memory in a pediatric surgery setting. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2006;188: 489–97. pmid:16633798
  64. 64. Van de Velde A, Schneider I, Camu F. A double-blind comparison of the efficacy of lorazepam FDDF versus placebo for anesthesia premedication in children. Acta Anaesthesiol Belg. 1987;38: 207–12. pmid:3425213
  65. 65. Mj Yun. The effect of intravenous clonidine premedication on pediatric adenotonsillectomy. Korean J Anesthesiol. 2003;45: 715–9. 10.4097/kjae.2003.45.6.715
  66. 66. Dubost J, Roche C, Kalfon F. The amnesic effects of midazolam in premedication of children. Cah Anesthesiol. 1991;39: 9–12. pmid:2054701
  67. 67. Jensen B, Matsson L. Oral versus rectal midazolam as a pre-anaesthetic sedative in children receiving dental treatment under general anaesthesia. Acta Paediatr. 2002;91: 920–5. pmid:12222716
  68. 68. Kaviani N, Shahtusi M, Haj Norousali Tehrani M, Nazari S. Effect of oral midazolam premedication on children's co-operation before general anesthesia in pediatric dentistry. J Dent (Shiraz). 2014;15: 123–8. pmid:25191661
  69. 69. Millar K, Asbury AJ, Bowman AW, Hosey MT, Martin K, Musiello T, et al. A randomised placebo-controlled trial of the effects of midazolam premedication on children's postoperative cognition. Anaesthesia. 2007;62: 923–30. pmid:17697220
  70. 70. Funk W, Jakob W, Riedl T, Taeger K. Oral preanaesthetic medication for children: double-blind randomized study of a combination of midazolam and ketamine vs midazolam or ketamine alone. Br J Anaesth. 2000;84: 335–40. pmid:10793592
  71. 71. Singh V, Pathak M, Singh GP. Oral midazolam and oral butorphanol premedication. Indian J Pediatr. 2005;72: 741–4. pmid:16186674
  72. 72. Ghoneim MM, Mewaldt SP. Benzodiazepines and human memory: a review. Anesthesiology. 1990;72: 926–38. pmid:1971164
  73. 73. Buffett-Jerrott SE, Stewart SH. Cognitive and sedative effects of benzodiazepine use. Curr Pharm Des. 2002;8: 45–58. pmid:11812249
  74. 74. Galeotti N, Bartolini A, Ghelardini C. Alpha-2 agonist-induced memory impairment is mediated by the alpha-2A-adrenoceptor subtype. Behav Brain Res. 2004;153: 409–17. pmid:15265636
  75. 75. Hinrichs JV, Ghoneim MM, Mewaldt SP. Diazepam and memory: retrograde facilitation produced by interference reduction. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1984;84: 158–62.pmid:6438674
  76. 76. Reder LM, Oates JM, Dickison D, Anderson JR, Gyula F, Quinlan JJ, et al. Retrograde facilitation under midazolam: the role of general and specific interference. Psychon Bull Rev. 2007;14: 261–9. pmid:17694911
  77. 77. Conway A, Rolley J, Sutherland JR. Midazolam for sedation before procedures. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016 20;(5): CD009491. pmid:27198122
  78. 78. Andrade C. The primary outcome measure and its importance in clinical trials. J Clin Psychiatry. 2015;76: e1320–3. pmid:26528658