Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Perceived Parenting Mediates Serotonin Transporter Gene (5-HTTLPR) and Neural System Function during Facial Recognition: A Pilot Study

  • Saori Nishikawa ,

    saolingon@gmail.com (SN); masak@hiroshima-u.ac.jp (MK)

    Affiliations Department of Pediatrics, Graduate School of Biomedical and Health Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima City, Hiroshima, Japan, Phoenix Leader Education Program, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima City, Hiroshima, Japan

  • Tamotsu Toshima ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Tamotsu Toshima, Masao Kobayashi

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Graduate School of Education, Hiroshima University, Higashi Hiroshima City, Hiroshima, Japan

  • Masao Kobayashi

    Contributed equally to this work with: Tamotsu Toshima, Masao Kobayashi

    saolingon@gmail.com (SN); masak@hiroshima-u.ac.jp (MK)

    Affiliations Department of Pediatrics, Graduate School of Biomedical and Health Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima City, Hiroshima, Japan, Phoenix Leader Education Program, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima City, Hiroshima, Japan

Abstract

This study examined changes in prefrontal oxy-Hb levels measured by NIRS (Near-Infrared Spectroscopy) during a facial-emotion recognition task in healthy adults, testing a mediational/moderational model of these variables. Fifty-three healthy adults (male = 35, female = 18) aged between 22 to 37 years old (mean age = 24.05 years old) provided saliva samples, completed a EMBU questionnaire (Swedish acronym for Egna Minnen Beträffande Uppfostran [My memories of upbringing]), and participated in a facial-emotion recognition task during NIRS recording. There was a main effect of maternal rejection on RoxH (right frontal activation during an ambiguous task), and a gene × environment (G×E) interaction on RoxH, suggesting that individuals who carry the SL or LL genotype and who endorse greater perceived maternal rejection show less right frontal activation than SL/LL carriers with lower perceived maternal rejection. Finally, perceived parenting style played a mediating role in right frontal activation via the 5-HTTLPR genotype. Early-perceived parenting might influence neural activity in an uncertain situation i.e. rating ambiguous faces among individuals with certain genotypes. This preliminary study makes a small contribution to the mapping of an influence of gene and behaviour on the neural system. More such attempts should be made in order to clarify the links.

Introduction

Recognition of facial expressions influences our emotion and social behaviours. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by deficits in communication and social interactions [1], and autistic-like behaviors are seen even among healthy individuals [2, 3]. In neuroimaging investigations, associations were found between autistic traits and frontal activation when participants were required to recognize negative expressions [4]. In contrast, an NIRS (Near-Infrared Spectroscopy) study showed that participants with PDD (Pervasive Developmental Disorder) showed reduced oxy-Hb changes during presentation of scared faces [5]. Cognitive rehabilitation has a positive influence on chronic prefrontal lesion patients’ prefrontal activity during a facial recognition task [6].

Over the past few years there have been an increasing number of studies using an endophenotypic approach, which serve to bridge biology and psychology. The serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) is one of the most frequently studied genes relating to personality and behaviour. The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism is based in the promoter region of the gene, with two variants commonly distinguished- the long or L-allele and the short or S-allele. Early studies indicated that SS individuals show an increased risk of depression [7, 8], which might relate to the finding that amygdala responding to negative stimuli is greater among SS individuals [9, 10]. 5-HTTLPR has also been found to moderate an association between emotional behavior and marital satisfaction [11]. Some studies have reported influences of 5-HTTLPR on social anxiety. It was reported that coping potential appears to be reduced in SS carriers [12], who appear to be less successful in reducing negative affect via cognitive reappraisal [13]. These researchers provided support for a mediational model in which 5-HTTLPR contributed to social anxiety symptoms via decreased reappraisal. Another study indicated that negative self-reflection provokes stronger personal distress in SS than in LL individuals [14]. Compared to the LL individuals, SS individuals report higher levels of emotional reactivity (i.e. empathy, self-conscious emotions) [14], are more sensitive to negative views of the self [4], and show more personal distress and physiological arousal when looking at other people’s distress [15]. Among a sample of high-exposure 9/11 survivors, SS individuals reported more post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) than SL/LL individuals, and that both PTSS and the presence of short allele predicted decreased activity in several cortical midline regions when viewing 9/11 images [16]. One study investigated genetic patterns in relation to PMO (Personal Meaning Organization). S allele carriers were more likely to develop an Outward PMO, in which individuals are vulnerable to negative social judgments and tend to perceive their early attachments as less predictable [17].

Early parent-child interactions influence the development of the amygdala and prefrontal cortex [18,19]. More specifically, there is a relationship between infant attachment quality at 18 months and prefrontal region activation during emotion regulation in adulthood [20]. Parental bonding styles during childhood have an impact on recognition of facial emotions later in adulthood. Zeng et al [21] showed an influence of early childhood experience with parents on the decoding process and sensitivity during the processing of emotional facial expression in adulthood. Autonomy from mothers predicts fear recognition accuracy, and maternal care predicted sadness recognition accuracy. The important influences of parenting behaviour and attachment style on emotional wellbeing are well-known [22], and such effects may be amplified as a function of genotype [23]. Individual differences in adapting to different environments might be illuminated by genetic variation. Thus, behaviour may depend on both genes and environment. There have been some attempts to identify links between genes and environment (G×E) or gene and gender interactions on emotional/ behavioural problems and emotional processing or regulation. It was reported that 5-HTTLPR influences impulsive behaviour via memories of paternal and maternal rearing styles among adults [23]. There are effects of 5-HTTLPR on emotion recognition, as well as a gene-environment interaction, with childhood emotional abuse as well as recent life events [24]. Owens et al. [25] supported a moderated model of the effects of 5-HTTLPR and early childhood adversity on cognitive and emotional processing in adolescence. Adolescents who carried the S-allele and who were exposed early childhood adversities were less able to classify negative and neutral stimuli compared to S allele carriers who did not experience early childhood adversities. Influences from both genes and environment show increased stability with age [26]. Gender appears to play different roles in facial recognition. For example, SS homogenous females recognized sadness or anger more quickly than females with other genotypes, whereas a male heterozygous group were quicker than their counterparts with other genotypes [27].

Above all, interpreting results from an endophenotype approach is complex. More recently, studies have suggested that the SS genotype serves as a plasticity factor [28] rather than a risk factor, as argued previously [7, 8]. The SS genotype can be associated with both negative and positive reactions [28]. It was also noted that the effects of genes and environment on outcome could depend on the methodology by which environments are assessed [29]. There have been ethnic differences reported for the association between 5HTT and depression [30], as well as brain function [31, 32]. To our knowledge, the neural influences on the genetic and environmental basis of social cognition among healthy individuals remains poorly understood, particularly for Japanese samples. Furthermore, we sought to address the question of whether perceived parenting mediates or moderates 5-HTTLPR and right/left frontal activation during a facial recognition task, as measured by NIRS. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to assess changes in prefrontal oxy-Hb levels during a face-emotion recognition task in healthy adults. The intent was to examine the interplay between serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), perceived parenting style, and their effects on cognitive/emotional processing. This investigation was a pilot study using 2ch NIRS, and we focused on the prefrontal cortex, according to earlier studies showing links between the prefrontal cortex and facial expression recognition, e.g. [6, 33]. In light of the reported links between genes, attachment with early caregivers, and behaviour, we hypothesized there would be relationships between the 5-HTTLPR genotype and neurological system functioning that are mediated or moderated by perceived parenting style. More specifically, it was expected that a higher perception of parental warmth would predict better face recognition, and this effect would depend on the individuals’ genotype.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

Fifty-three healthy adults (males = 35, females = 18) aged between 22 to 37 years (mean age = 24.05 years old) were recruited from a university in Hiroshima. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Hiroshima University, and the procedure was followed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki [34]. All participants were voluntary and gave their written informed consent. After a brief explanation of the present study, participants provided their informed consent, a saliva sample for DNA analysis, completed a questionnaire assessing perceived parental style, and completed NIRS recording. The entire procedure took approximately 30–40 minutes.

Facial-emotion Recognition Task

Six hundred coloured images depicting four basic emotional expressions (happiness, surprise, anger, sadness) were used. We used standardized images that were developed using a morphing technique from database DB99 (Advanced Telecommunications Research Institute International, Inc, Nara, Japan, http://www.atr-p.com/products/face-db.html). These images have been used in prior studies, e.g. [6, 35]. We prepared one neutral and four emotional expressions from 50% (clear) to 35% (less clear) and 20% (ambiguous) in terms of expression clarity, by morphing photos of the women (see Fig 1). Each image was framed by an oval to avoid influences of hairstyle and clothing. One participant per session arrived at the laboratory and was seated in front of a computer with an NIRS recording device attached. The experimental setting is shown in Fig 2. The stimuli were presented in colour. One image was displayed at a time, centred on the monitor, with four expression descriptors (“happy”, “sad”, “angry” and “surprised”) displayed at the bottom (see Fig 3a & 3b). The participants were asked to click on the best description of the female expression in the centre of the screen. A neutral face without emotion was used for the baseline measurement. The participants were told to click on the text “I have no idea” if the expression was too difficult to rate. According to previous findings using the facial-emotional recognition task with both male and female faces [36], differences in the effects of male and female faces are relatively small. In order to simplify the task for the participants, we used only female faces in the present study.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Emotional facial expressions used in the experiments and intensity of emotion from 20% (ambiguous), 35% (less clear), and 100% (clear).

Note: Permission for publication of images was obtained by Hiroshi Yoshida. Publication of morphed images complies with the Terms of Use of the DB99 database.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134685.g001

thumbnail
Fig 3. (a) & (b). Screen shots of the facial-emotion task.

Note: (a) A screenshot of the control task with a neutral facial expression. In the box, the participants are asked to click on a text “None”, (b) a screenshot of the task—the participants are asked to click on the best description of the female expression in the centre of the screen. The descriptions in the boxes from left to right: “Happy”, “Sad”, “Angry”, “Surprised”, “I have no idea”. Note: Permission for publication of images was obtained by Hiroshi Yoshida. Publication of morphed images complies with the Terms of Use of the DB99 database.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134685.g003

NIRS recordings

To monitor brain activation during the facial-emotion recognition task, we used a non-invasive neuroimaging technique, NIRS (Near-Infrared Spectroscopy). Using a 2-ch NIRS system (NIRO-200NX C10448), with a reflectance mode with different wavelengths (735, 810, 850 nm), hemodynamic concentrations in the cortex were measured. The NIRS probes consist of two sets of one detector and one emitter, which are attached at FP1 and FP2 respectively according to the international 10–20 EEG electrode system. Two probe holders were placed on the left and right sides of the forehead over the eyebrow with double-sided adhesive tape. In order to reduce artifacts, the participants were asked to sit on a chair and not to move any part of their body unrelated to the task. Assuming that the differential path length is 24 cm, the changes in the chromophore concentration were expressed in micromoles per litre. The time resolution of the NIRS recordings was every 1 s. Results based on changes in oxy-Hb concentration were reported, because this is the most sensitive hemodynamic response parameter. First, baseline correction was performed at the pre/stimulus period (-25-0s) for each run. The corrected data were then averaged across two runs and converted to z-scores (by dividing the difference between means during the pre-stimulus and stimulus (10-30sec) periods by the standard deviation during the pre stimulus period). Unlike fMRI, NIRS recordings allow subjects to keep their body positions and the experimental setting is not stressful.

Perceived parental rearing

The EMBU (Egna Minnen Beträffande Uppfostran–“My memories of upbringing”) [37] is originally a Swedish self-report measure of adult perceptions of the rearing behaviour of one’s parents, and includes Rejection, Emotional Warmth, Overprotection, and Favouring Subject subscales. The EMBU consists of 81 items each rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1–4). A Japanese version of the EMBU has been developed and shown to have good psychometric properties [37]. According to the mean values provided by Someya et al [38], participants were divided into high/low rejection or warmth groups for some of the statistical analyses.

Genotyping

Genomic DNA was extracted from oral mucosa collected from participants using the QIAamp DNA Micro Kit (QIAGEN, Tokyo, Japan). This study was performed using ethnically homogeneous individuals (only of Japanese descent). The serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) of the 5-HTT gene regulatory region was amplified via polymerase chain reaction (PCR) with forward (59-GGCGTTGCCGCTCTGAATGC-39) and reverse primers (59-GAGGGACTGAGCTGGACAACCAC-39). For PCR, 10 ng of genomic DNA was used in a 25-mL reaction mixture containing 0.5 U of KOD FX Neo (Toyobo Co., LTD.) and 10 pmoles of each primer in PCR buffer for KOD FX Neo (Toyobo Co., LTD.). Cycling conditions were as follows: Denaturation (94uC for 2 min) and 30 cycles of amplification (98uC for 10 sec, 63uC for 30 sec, and 68uC for 30 sec). PCR products were separated by electrophoresis in a 3% agarose gel and visualized by UV after ethidium bromide staining. A 484 bp band was observed for the S allele, and a 528 bp band for the L allele; heterozygous samples showed both alleles. Two investigators scored allele sizes independently and any inconsistencies were reviewed and rerun.

Data Analysis

SPSS (The Statistical Package for Social Sciences) version 22 [39] was used for computing/performing descriptive statistics, correlations, and ANOVAs (analysis of variance). SEM (Structural Equation Models) were analysed using AMOS 22 on SPSS to evaluate the mediating effects of the variables specified by path diagram models. This software performs analyses of moment structures through maximum likelihood estimation. To investigate whether a variable X (PARE) is a mediator between independent variable A (5HTT) and dependent variable B (RoxH) in the path analysis, a direct path from A to B is drawn during a first analysis. In the next analysis, two paths are added, one from A to X and the other from X to B. If X is a significant mediator, the weight of the path from A to B will decrease in the second analysis in comparison to the first one [40]. The GFI (Goodness of Fit Index) is considered a reasonable statistical index for evaluating a model and assesses the fit between a hypothesized model and the data. AMOS calculates all measures that capture model evaluation that were selected based on different theoretical perspectives, including CMIN/df (the minimum value of sample discrepancy divided by its degree of freedom, smaller values preferable [41]), CFI (The Comparative Fit Index, a measure of the relative amount of variance and covariance, close to 1, over 0.9 is preferable [42]), RMSEA (the root mean square error of approximation based on population discrepancy, smaller values below 0.08 preferable [43]), and IFI (Incremental Fit Index, close to 1, over 0.9 is preferable [44]).

Results

Descriptive analysis

Of the 53 participants, 30 (53.6%) were homozygous for the short allele, 24 (42.9%) carried the heterogeneous genotype, and 2 (3.6%) were homozygous for the long allele. 5-HTTLPR groups did not differ significantly in regard to gender. One-way ANOVAs showed significant effects of genotype on both perceived maternal rejection and maternal emotional warmth. SL/LL carriers reported more maternal rejection, F (1,55) = 4.79, p < .05) and less emotional warmth F (1,55) = 4.41, p < .05) than SS carriers.

Facial-Emotion Recognition Test results

Table 1 shows mean right and left frontal activation values as a function of different facial expressions, gender, genotype, and perceived parenting style. To test whether magnitude of right frontal activation differed across facial expressions, we conducted a series of paired samples t-tests. Right frontal activation during ambiguous faces was significantly higher than that during clear (t (322) = 2.20, p < .05) or less clear expressions (t (322) = −10.65, p < .001; see Fig 4).

thumbnail
Table 1. Mean values of right and left frontal activation on different levels of facial tasks divided in gender, genotype, and perceived parenting.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134685.t001

thumbnail
Fig 4. Comparison of right frontal oxy-Hb changes in the facial-recognition task difficulty.

Note: * p < .05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134685.g004

A Pillai’s MANOVA (2×2 factorial design) was conducted with gene and parental rejection as independent variables and NIRS recordings when rating clear, less clear and ambiguous faces as dependent variables. The analysis was conducted separately for rejection from fathers and mothers. The analysis for fathers yielded a significant main effect of paternal rejection (p = .015, η2 = .194, power = .790). However, there was no significant main effect on gene (p = .883, η2 = .023, power = .158), or an interaction (p = .791, η2 = .021, power = .113) between the independent variables. The analysis for mothers revealed a significant main effect of maternal rejection (p < .000, η2 = .318, power = .979), and an interaction between gene and maternal rejection (p < .005, η2 = .117, power = .744). There was no significant main effect on gene (p = .964, η2 = .014, power = .110).

Univariate F-tests showed significant effects of paternal and maternal rejection on RoxH, (F (1,50) = 7.16, p = .010, η2 = .125, power = .747 for fathers; F (1,50) = 20.94, p < .000, η2 = .295, power = .994 for mothers). These results suggest that individuals who perceived their father or mother as rejecting tended to have lower right frontal activation when rating ambiguous facial expressions.

A univariate F-test showed a significant gene × maternal rejection interaction on RoxH (F (1,50) = 4.84, η2 = .162, p = .012, power = .994), suggesting that individuals who carry the SL or LL genotype and with higher perceptions of rejection from their mothers had less right frontal activation compared to the SL/LL carriers with lower levels of perceived maternal rejection (see Fig 5). The interaction was not shown for paternal rejection (F (1,50) = .010, p = .921, η2 = .000, power = .051).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Comparisons of high/low maternal rejection in right frontal oxy-Hb changes during ambiguous facial expression task within the same genotype group.

Note: * p < .05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134685.g005

SEM Evaluation

Before testing models, intercorrelations among factors were analysed. Table 2 shows correlations between perceived parenting, right/left frontal activation and other variables. Right frontal activation during ambiguous faces was negatively associated with rejection by fathers and mothers (r = —.290 for fathers, r = —.358 for mothers, both p < .001), and positively associated with paternal and maternal emotional warmth (r = .340 for fathers, r = .285 for mothers, both p < .001).

thumbnail
Table 2. Variables assessed in the present study: Means, standard deviations, and correlations between the variables.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134685.t002

Fig 6 presents the standardized SEM solution, specifying 5-HTTLPR (5HTT) as a direct determinant of perceived parental rearing (PARE) and right frontal activation during an ambiguous facial-emotion recognition task (RoxH). Following correlational analysis results, two EMBU subscales (Rejection and Emotional Warmth from fathers or mothers) were used for the model. The estimated mediational model with standardized path coefficients is presented in Table 3. The following strategy was used to test mediation. First, model 1 was explored in Fig 6. The pathways linking 5HTT, PARE, and RoxH were all significant (p < .05); however, the direct pathway from 5HTT to RoxH was not significant. Goodness of fit indices indicated an acceptable fit between model 1 and the data, X2 = 6.61 (df = 6, p = .359), CMIN/df = 1.10, RMSEA = .043, CFI = .94; see Table 3).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Structural Equation Model 1.

Note: All paths significant (** p < .001 * p < .05) unless indicated (ns). 5HTT = 5-HTTLPR, PARE = Perceived parenting style (ReF = Paternal Rejection, ReM = Maternal Rejection, EmF = Emotional warmth from father, EmM = Emotional warmth from mother), RoxH = Right frontal oxygen hemoglobin.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134685.g006

thumbnail
Table 3. Comparisons among pathways from 5-HTTLPR to perceived parenting and right frontal activation, using various measures of model fit.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134685.t003

The next possibility was explored in the model illustrated in Fig 7. Because the pathway between 5HTT and RoxH was not significant in model 1, model 2 was evaluated, in which this pathway is eliminated. Model 2 assessed an indirect influence of 5HTT on RoxH through PARE. Pathways linking 5HTT and PARE with RoxH were both significant (p < .001 and p < .005, respectively). The relationships between all constructs and their indicators were positive and significant (p < .001). Goodness of fit indices indicated an acceptable fit between model 2 and the data, X2 = 7.09 (df = 7, p = .420), CMIN/df = .772, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = .000, IFI = .999; see Table 3).

thumbnail
Fig 7. Structural Equation Model 2.

Note: All paths significant (** p < .001 * p < .05) unless indicated (ns). 5HTT = 5-HTTLPR, PARE = Perceived parenting style (ReF = Paternal Rejection, ReM = Maternal Rejection, EmF = Emotional warmth from father, EmM = Emotional warmth from mother), RoxH = Right frontal oxygen hemoglobin.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134685.g007

Discussion

The present study investigated changes in prefrontal oxy-Hb levels measured by NIRS during a facial-emotion recognition task in healthy adults, and examined interplay between as well as moderation/mediational models of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), perceived parenting, and their effects on cognitive/emotional processing. Correlational analysis indicated that right frontal activation when rating ambiguous expressions was negatively associated with perceived rejection by both fathers and mothers, and was positively associated with paternal and maternal emotional warmth. There was a significant main effect of maternal rejection on right frontal activation, and also a G×E interaction between maternal rejection and 5-HTTLPR on right frontal activation. Finally, SEM analysis indicated that perceived parenting style plays a mediating role in right frontal activation via the 5-HTTLPR genotype. These effects were not shown in an analysis of left frontal activation, and there was no direct effect of genotype on either right or left frontal lobe activation.

Using SEM, two models were evaluated in regard to both direct and indirect relationships between the 5-HTTLPR gene (5HTT) and right frontal activation during the facial-emotion recognition task (RoxH) via perceived parental rearing (PARE). Model 1, which features 5-HTTLPR gene as a moderator of parenting style and right frontal activation, provided a reasonable fit with the data. However, the direct path from 5HTT to RoxH was not significant. The paths featured in model 2 were all significant, and this model showed a good fit with the data.

Among SL/LL carriers, right frontal activation during the facial-emotion recognition task differed as a function of perceived parenting style. When L-allele carriers endorsed positive parenting, right frontal activation was significantly higher than for those who perceived their parents as rejecting and cold. Punitive parenting predicts error-related brain activity [45], and healthy upbringing is linked with positive development of the PFC [18,19]. Furthermore, the results showed that early parenting behaviours interact with genotype in relation to right frontal activation during a facial-emotion recognition task. This result is in line with the results of Moutsiana et al. [20], which suggested that disturbance of early caregiver relationships affects the neural regulation of emotion later in adulthood. Rating the ambiguous faces may let the participants in the uncertain situation. Moreover, it may also be possible to interpret our results such that early negative parenting influences one’s perceptions of uncertain situations, particularly among L-carriers in the present study.

We identified a main effect of perceived maternal rejection and its interaction with gene on right frontal activation during the ambiguous facial-emotion recognition task. These results may be in line with the finding of a strong influence of mothers' rearing style on individuals’ impulsive behaviours [23]. The present study showed no significant main effects of gene on right/left frontal activation during any phrase of the facial-emotion recognition task. It may be that the influence on a single gene might be small for the function of the social brain. However, when applying perceived parenting as interacting with gene, the results supported an influence of perceived parenting only among L-carriers. Despite a study showing a role of the S-allele in highlighting negative thoughts about self and environment [4], the present study showed that individuals carrying one or two L alleles perceived their mothers as more rejecting and as less emotionally warm compared to S homogenous individuals. It remains for further studies to disentangle whether this complex result can be explained on the basis of plasticity in the SS-genotype [28].

It is important to note limitations of the present study. Firstly, the EMBU questionnaire is based on retrospective self-report, and measures the subjective experiences of parental behaviour as reported by participants. We cannot say if the results of the present study are general, or specific to EMBU. For this reason, future work should analyse some other variable, for example “negative emotional traits” as an alternative to the EMBU, in order to see if the recorded effects on the NIRS are robust. Second, the effects of gender on the faces used in the recognition task are unknown. Third, using a complex study design with many variables but few participants makes the results difficult to evaluate. We were unable to perform gender comparisons in the SEM models due to the small sample size. Fourth, we could not assess the effects of parental genotype and their parenting styles. It is known that parental genotypes are related to negative parenting behaviours and of course children share the genotypes of their parents [29]. A recent study found that links between 5-HTTLPR gene of mother and child were related to child brain morphology and behavioural performance [46].

Finally, we have not investigated the functionality of the 5-HTTLPR gene in relation to the other polymorphisms and genotypes. More polymorphisms should be investigated with regard to perceived parenting and neural networks; DRD4 [47], and COMT [48] in particular. A recent study has shown that 5-HTTLPR and COMT modulated the vulnerability for anxiety disorders via different mechanisms. That is, the 5-HTTLPR influenced the quality and intensity of the fear, while COMT seemed to influence inhibition of fear in the presence of safety learning [49].

Despite limitations, the present study makes a small contribution to the mapping of a combined influence of gene and behaviour on neural system functioning. It can be speculated that neural activity engaged in facial recognition contributes to the nature of early interactions with caregiver. Perceived parenting in childhood may constitute a direct factor that affects the neural imaging of facial recognition in adulthood. 5-HTTLPR indirectly influences the neural system underlying facial recognition; however, this influence is dependent on the environment in which the individual was raised, or at least his/her perception of said environment. Furthermore, early childhood experiences may influence neural activity in uncertain situations, in certain genotypes. These should be clarified in the further studies with a sufficient sample size and use of psychophysiologically well-designed experimental tasks. Such an approach would provide further opportunities for further studies of clinical samples as well as children, in order to clarify links and characteristic G×E interaction effects on our cognitive and emotional processes.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge the participants and research assistants for the data collection and laboratory support. We also thank Dr. Hiroshi Yoshida and Dr. Elisabet Sundbom for valuable comments to our manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: SN TT MK. Performed the experiments: SN. Analyzed the data: SN TT MK. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SN TT MK. Wrote the paper: SN.

References

  1. 1. American Psychiatric Association (2013) DSM 5. American Psychiatric Association. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
  2. 2. Halliday DWR, MacDonald SWS, Sherf KS, Tanaka JW (2014) A Reciprocal Model of Face Recognition and Autistic Traits: Evidence from an Individual Differences Perspective. PLoS ONE 9(5): e94013. pmid:24853862
  3. 3. Hosokawa M, Nakadoi Y, Watanabe Y, Sumitani S, Ohmori T (2014) Association of autism tendency and hemodynamic changes in the prefrontal cortex during facial expression stimuli measured by multi‐channel near‐infrared spectroscopy. Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.
  4. 4. Ma Y, Li B, Wang C, Shi Z, Sun Y, Sheng F, et al. (2013) 5-HTTLPR polymorphism modulates neural mechanisms of negative self-reflection. Cerebral Cortex: bht099.
  5. 5. Nakadoi Y, Sumitani S, Watanabe Y, Akiyama M, Yamashita N, Ohmori T (2012) Multi‐channel near‐infrared spectroscopy shows reduced activation in the prefrontal cortex during facial expression processing in pervasive developmental disorder. Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences 66(1): 26–33. pmid:22250607
  6. 6. Shibazaki M, Yoshida H (2014) Cognitive Rehabilitation for Facial Expression Recognition Deficits in a Patient with Prefrontal Lesions: A NIRS Study. World Congress on Facial Expression and Emotion, October 2014, Porto, Portugal.
  7. 7. Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. (2003) Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 301: 386–389. pmid:12869766
  8. 8. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, et al. (1996) Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274(5292): 1527–1531. pmid:8929413
  9. 9. Munafò MR, Brown SM, Hariri AR (2008) Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: a meta-analysis. Biological psychiatry 63(9): 852–857. pmid:17949693
  10. 10. Schardt DM, Erk S, Nüsser C, Nöthen MM, Cichon S, Rietschel M, et al. (2010) Volition diminishes genetically mediated amygdala hyperreactivity. Neuroimage 53(3): 943–951. pmid:19969089
  11. 11. Haase CM, Saslow LR, Bloch L, Saturn SR, Casey JJ, Seider BH, et al. (2013) The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene moderates the association between emotional behavior and changes in marital satisfaction over time. Emotion 13(6): 1068. pmid:24098925
  12. 12. Szily E, Bowen J, Unoka Z, Simon L, Kéri S (2008) Emotion appraisal is modulated by the genetic polymorphism of the serotonin transporter. Journal of neural transmission, 115(6): 819–822. pmid:18273536
  13. 13. Firk C, Siep N, Markus CR (2012) Serotonin transporter genotype modulates cognitive reappraisal of negative emotions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience: nsr091.
  14. 14. Miu AC, Crişan LG, Chiş A, Ungureanu L, Drugă B, Vulturar R. (2012) Somatic markers mediate the effect of serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms on Iowa Gambling Task. Genes, Brain and Behavior 11(4): 398–403.
  15. 15. Gyurak A, Haase CM, Sze J, Goodkind MS, Coppola G, Lane J, et al. (2013) The effect of the serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) on empathic and self-conscious emotional reactivity. Emotion 13(1): 25. pmid:22906085
  16. 16. Olsson A, Kross E, Nordberg SS, Weinberg A, Weber J, Schmer-Galunder S, et al. (2014) Neural and genetic markers of vulnerability to posttraumatic stress symptoms among survivors of the World Trade Center attacks. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci: nsu125.
  17. 17. Nardi B, Marini A, Turchi C, Arimatea E, Tagliabracci A, Bellantuono C (2013) Role of 5-HTTLPR Polymorphism in the Development of the Inward/Outward Personality Organization: A Genetic Association Study. PLoS ONE 8(12): e82192. pmid:24358153
  18. 18. Burghy CA, Stodola DE, Ruttle PL, Molloy EK, Armstrong JM, Oler JA, et al. (2012) Developmental pathways to amygdala-prefrontal function and internalizing symptoms in adolescence. Nature Neuroscience 15(12): 1736–1741. pmid:23143517
  19. 19. Hanson JL, Adluru N, Chung MK, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ, Pollak SD (2013) Early neglect is associated with alterations in white matter integrity and cognitive functioning. Child development 84(5): 1566–1578. pmid:23480812
  20. 20. Moutsiana C, Fearon P, Murray L, Cooper P, Goodyer I, Johnstone T, et al. (2014) Making an effort to feel positive: insecure attachment in infancy predicts the neural underpinnings of emotion regulation in adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 55(9): 999–1008. pmid:24397574
  21. 21. Zheng L, Chai H, Chen W, Yu R, He W, Jiang Z, et al. (2011) Recognition of facial emotion and perceived parental bonding styles in healthy volunteers and personality disorder patients. Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences 65(7): 648–654. pmid:22176284
  22. 22. Nishikawa S, Sundbom E, Hägglöf B (2010) Influence of perceived parental rearing on adolescent self-concept and internalizing and externalizing problems in Japan. Journal of Child and Family Studies 19(1): 57–66.
  23. 23. Nishikawa S, Nishitani S, Fujisawa TX, Noborimoto I, Kitahara T, Shinohara K. (2012) Perceived Parental Rejection Mediates the Influence of Serotonin Transporter Gene (5-HTTLPR) Polymorphisms on Impulsivity in Japanese Adults. PLoS ONE 7(10): e47608. pmid:23112823
  24. 24. Antypa N, Cerit H, Kruijt AW, Verhoeven FEA, Van der Does AJW (2011) Relationships among 5-HTT genotype, life events and gender in the recognition of facial emotions. Neuroscience 172: 303–313. pmid:20971165
  25. 25. Owens M, Goodyer IM, Wilkinson P, Bhardwaj A, Abbott R, Croudace T, et al. (2012) 5-HTTLPR and Early Childhood Adversities Moderate Cognitive and Emotional Processing in Adolescence. PLoS ONE 7(11): e48482. pmid:23209555
  26. 26. Briley DA, Tucker-Drob EM (2014) Genetic and environmental continuity in personality development: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 140(5): 1303–1331. pmid:24956122
  27. 27. Nishizawa S, Benkelfat C, Young SN, Leyton M, Mzengeza SD, de Montigny C, et al. (1997) Differences between males and females in rates of serotonin synthesis in human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 94(10): 5308–5313.
  28. 28. Pluess M, Belsky J, Way BM, Taylor SE (2010) 5-HTTLPR moderates effects of current life events on neuroticism: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 34(6): 1070–1074. pmid:20573579
  29. 29. Van Ijzendoorn MH, Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ (2012) Serotonin transporter genotype 5-HTTLPR as a marker of differential susceptibility? A meta-analysis of child and adolescent gene-by-environment studies. Translational psychiatry 2(8): e147.
  30. 30. Zhang K, Xu Q, Xu Y, Yang H, Luo J, Sun Y, et al. (2009) The combined effects of the 5-HTTLPR and 5-HTR1A genes modulates the relationship between negative life events and major depressive disorder in a Chinese population. Journal of affective disorders 114(1): 224–231.
  31. 31. Long H, Liu B, Hou B, Wang C, Li J, Qin W, et al. (2013) The long rather than the short allele of 5-HTTLPR predisposes Han Chinese to anxiety and reduced connectivity between prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Neuroscience bulletin 29(1): 4–15. pmid:23319313
  32. 32. Li S, Zou Q, Li J, Li J, Wang D, Yan C, et al. (2012) 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts task-evoked and resting-state activities of the amygdala in Han Chinese. PloS ONE 7(5): e36513. pmid:22574175
  33. 33. Nomura M, Ohira H, Haneda K, Iidaka T, Sadato N, Okada T, et al. (2004) Functional association of the amygdala and ventral prefrontal cortex during cognitive evaluation of facial expressions primed by masked angry faces: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 21(1): 352–363. pmid:14741673
  34. 34. WMA News: Revising the Declaration of Helsinki. (2008) World Medical Journal 54(4): 120–25.
  35. 35. Maki Y, Yoshida H, Yamaguchi T, Yamaguchi H (2013) Relative preservation of the recognition of positive facial expression “happiness” in Alzheimer disease. International Psychogeriatrics 25(01): 105–110.
  36. 36. Yoshida H (2012) Gender comparison in cognition of facial emotions. Poster presented at Fukuyama University 68th conference on Psychology in Chugoku/Shikoku region, Hiroshima, Japan (in Japanese).
  37. 37. Perris C, Jacobsson L, Lindstrom H, von Knorring L, Perris H (1980) Development of a new inventory for assessing memories of parental rearing behavior. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 61: 265–274. pmid:7446184
  38. 38. Someya T, Takahashi S, Kadowaki M, Reist C, Tang SW (1997) Development of a Japanese form of EMBU scale and study on its reliability. Seishin-Igaku 38: 1065–1072 (in Japanese).
  39. 39. IBM Corp (2013) IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
  40. 40. Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51: 1173–1182. pmid:3806354
  41. 41. Holmes-Smith P (2000) Introduction to structural equation modeling using AMOS 4.0 & LISREL 8.30. Melbourne, Australia: School Research, Evaluation and Measurement Services.
  42. 42. Bentler PM (1990) Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological bulletin 107(2): 238–246. pmid:2320703
  43. 43. Bollen KA (1989) A new incremental fit index for general structural equation models. Sociological Methods & Research 17(3): 303–316.
  44. 44. Browne MW, Cudeck R (1993) Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Bollen KA, Long JS (Eds.) Testing structural equation models, Sage, Newbury Park, CA, 136–162.
  45. 45. Meyer A, Proudfit GH, Bufferd SJ, Kujawa A J, Laptook RS, Torpey DC, et al. (2014) Self-Reported and Observed Punitive Parenting Prospectively Predicts Increased Error-Related Brain Activity in Six-Year-Old Children. Journal of abnormal child psychology: 1–9.
  46. 46. van der Knaap NJ, El Marroun H, Klumpers F, Mous SE, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A, et al. (2014). Beyond Classical Inheritance: The Influence of Maternal Genotype upon Child's Brain Morphology and Behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(29): 9516–9521. pmid:25031395
  47. 47. Loggia ML, Jensen K, Gollub RL, Wasan AD, Edwards RR, Kong J (2011) The Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) val158met Polymorphism Affects Brain Responses to Repeated Painful Stimuli. PLoS ONE 6(11): e27764. pmid:22132136
  48. 48. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Pijlman FT, Mesman J, Juffer F (2008) Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Developmental psychology 44(1): 293–300. pmid:18194028
  49. 49. Wendt J, Neubert J, Lindner K, Ernst FD, Homuth G, Weike A, et al. (In Press) Genetic Influences on the Acquisition and Inhibition of Fear. International Journal of Psychophysiology.