ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Correspondence

Ethnic differences in names in China: A comparison between Chinese Mongolian and Han Chinese cultures in Inner Mongolia

[version 1; peer review: 2 approved, 1 approved with reservations]
PUBLISHED 18 Jan 2022
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

Abstract

I propose two suggestions on Stojcic et al.’s (2020) Study 3, which examined ethnic differences in individualism between Chinese Mongolian and Han Chinese cultures in China. The authors analyzed the names of all residents in the Inner Mongolia Autonomous Region of China and found that the percentages of common names among Chinese Mongolians were smaller than those among Han Chinese. The authors concluded that Chinese Mongolians are more independent than Han Chinese. However, two questions remain unanswered. First, although the authors analyzed the names of people in all age groups together and did not analyze the names by birth year, how was the effect of time controlled? Second, although the authors treated name indices, which have been used as group-level indicators in previous research, as individual-level indicators, how did the authors confirm whether name indices can be used as individual-level indicators? Addressing these two questions would contribute to a better understanding of ethnic differences in individualism in China.

Keywords

ethnic difference, name, China, Mongol, individualism, culture, uniqueness, cultural change

1. How was the effect of time controlled?

The authors calculated the percentages of the top 1, 10, and 20 most common names by ethnic group and found that those among Chinese Mongolians were smaller than those among Han Chinese.1 They concluded that Chinese Mongolians are more independent than Han Chinese.

However, the names of people in all age groups were analyzed together and were not analyzed by birth year. Although the specific age span of the population was unclear, they wrote that “we included data for Chinese Han and Mongolian living in the same area, i.e., the region of Inner Mongolia, over the span of some 60 years” (p. 7). A name of a person aged 60 years indicates naming behavior approximately 60 years ago. Thus, naming behaviors for more than 60 years were included in the data. This approach is problematic for comparison between ethnicities for at least four reasons.

First, the authors’ analyses did not exclude the possibility that the differences stemmed from the naming behaviors at different time points rather than ethnic differences at a specific time point. In other words, the authors’ analyses may not have compared the behaviors at the same time point. Considering that a prior study insisted that unique names increased over time in China (Cai et al., 2018; but also see Ogihara, 2020b), this confounding could have affected the results. For example, younger people who have unique names may have been included in the analyses more in Chinese Mongolians than in Han Chinese. This may have caused the result that the percentages of the common names among Chinese Mongolians were smaller than those among Han Chinese. Moreover, distribution of age may have differed between the two ethnic groups.

Second, it is unclear what the indicators mean. The authors calculated the percentages of the top 1, 10, and 20 most common names over a period of more than 60 years rather than the common names by year. However, common names can drastically change over time. It is possible that common names 60 years ago are no longer common in the present. The meaning of common names in a given year is clear, but the meaning of names common over a period of more than 60 years is ambiguous.

Third, the analyses contradicted the authors’ claim that past sustenance styles (Mongolian: herding, Han Chinese: farming) affect “present” psychological tendencies (Mongolian: independence/individualism, Han Chinese: interdependence/collectivism). The authors emphasized present psychological tendencies, but they examined past psychological tendencies from more than 60 years ago.

Fourth, Study 3 examined different constructs than those in Studies 1 and 2. The authors stated that “in order to furthermore increase the ecological validity of the current research, in Study 3 we tested our hypothesis in real life setting by investigating the baby naming practices between the Chinese Han and Mongolian” (p. 6). However, in Study 3 the authors analyzed indicators for over 60 years, which was inconsistent with Studies 1 and 2, in which the authors measured psychological tendencies in recent years.2

All of the previous studies that the authors cited in the article controlled this effect by analyzing indicators by year (Ogihara et al., 2015; Twenge et al., 2010) or at the specific year (2007; Varnum & Kitayama, 2011). Thus, analyzing data in recent years (preferably the year when the data were collected in Studies 1 and 2) addresses all four concerns.

The authors should at least explain how these concerns were addressed. Although the authors stated that “since the age did not differ between two ethnic groups, it is quite probable that the age would not moderate the observed tendencies” (p. 9), the authors did not have data on age.3 It is unclear how the authors concluded that the age would not moderate the observed tendencies. Moreover, as stated above, the issue is not only about the group difference in the average age.

2. Can the name index be used as an individual-level indicator?

The authors treated the name indices as individual-level indicators reflecting personal inner characteristics. In presenting the results, they used the term “social cognition” throughout the text and “cognitive tendencies” (p. 10). Moreover, the authors suggested that Study 3 increased the “ecological validity” (p. 6) of the findings in Studies 1 and 2, which measured individual-level psychological tendencies.

However, the previous research the authors cited regarded name indices as group-level indicators that reflect group (e.g., nation, state, and culture) characteristics (Ogihara et al., 2015; Twenge et al., 2010; Varnum & Kitayama, 2011). Because names can be determined by several individuals, such as couples, family members, and community members, naming involves a collective process of decision making. Thus, naming does not necessarily reflect individual psychology and behavior. For example, a husband may suggest a name, but his wife may reject it and choose a different name based on her mother’s advice. In this case, the husband’s psychology and behavior are not reflected in the name. Mongolians may ask lamas and/or elders to name their children. Thus, Studies 1 and 2 and Study 3 treated concepts at different levels. The authors should explain how they confirmed whether the name indices can be used as individual-level indicators. This level of concept (unit of analysis) is important when examining the relationship between culture and psychology (e.g., Cohen & Varnum, 2016; Schwartz, 2014; Vu et al., 2017).

Conclusion

I have proposed two suggestions on Stojcic et al. (2020). I hope these suggestions will contribute to a better understanding of ethnic differences in names, psychology, and culture.

Data availability

No data are associated with this article.

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 18 Jan 2022
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Ogihara Y. Ethnic differences in names in China: A comparison between Chinese Mongolian and Han Chinese cultures in Inner Mongolia [version 1; peer review: 2 approved, 1 approved with reservations] F1000Research 2022, 11:55 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.76837.1)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 18 Jan 2022
Views
11
Cite
Reviewer Report 18 Jul 2022
Han-Wu-Shuang Bao, Manchester China Institute, The University of Manchester, Manchester, UK 
Approved
VIEWS 11
In the correspondence article, Ogihara raised two concerns about the validity of Stojcic et al.’s (2020) Study 3, which suggested that Chinese Mongolian (vs. Han Chinese) were more independent as they were less likely to give the top 1, 10, ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Bao HWS. Reviewer Report For: Ethnic differences in names in China: A comparison between Chinese Mongolian and Han Chinese cultures in Inner Mongolia [version 1; peer review: 2 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2022, 11:55 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.80819.r143438)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
26
Cite
Reviewer Report 09 Mar 2022
Kenta Tsukatsune, Tokyo Metropolitan University, Tokyo, Japan 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 26
General Evaluation
 
Overall, the reviewer can mostly agree with arguments of this paper. Author Ogihara's discussion about the analysis in Study 3 of Stojcic et al.'s (2020) paper on how to handle cohort/age groups, and relationships of ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Tsukatsune K. Reviewer Report For: Ethnic differences in names in China: A comparison between Chinese Mongolian and Han Chinese cultures in Inner Mongolia [version 1; peer review: 2 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2022, 11:55 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.80819.r120384)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
19
Cite
Reviewer Report 14 Feb 2022
Ivana Stojcic, Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China 
Xiaopeng Ren, The Laboratory of Social and Engineering Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China 
Approved
VIEWS 19
In the Ethnic differences in names in China: A comparison between Chinese Mongolian and Han Chinese cultures in Inner Mongolia—Commentary, the author poses two main questions i.e. the issue of time control in baby names analyses and the issue of ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Stojcic I and Ren X. Reviewer Report For: Ethnic differences in names in China: A comparison between Chinese Mongolian and Han Chinese cultures in Inner Mongolia [version 1; peer review: 2 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2022, 11:55 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.80819.r120385)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 18 Jan 2022
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.