ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Article
Revised

Behavioural evidence for self-medication in bumblebees?

[version 3; peer review: 3 approved]
Previously titled: Weak and contradictory effects of self-medication with nectar nicotine by parasitized bumblebees
PUBLISHED 29 Oct 2015
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

This article is included in the Agriculture, Food and Nutrition gateway.

Abstract

The presence of antimicrobial secondary metabolites in nectar suggests that pollinators, which are threatened globally by emergent disease, may benefit from the consumption of nectars rich in these metabolites. We tested whether nicotine, a nectar secondary metabolite common in Solanaceae and Tilia species, is used by parasitized bumblebees as a source of self-medication, using a series of toxicological, microbiological and behavioural experiments. Caged bees infected with Crithidia bombi had a slight preference for sucrose solution laced with the alkaloid and behavioural tests showed that the parasite infection induced an increased consumption of nicotine during foraging activity, though nicotine had an appetite-reducing effect overall. When ingested, nicotine delayed the progression of a gut infection in bumblebees by a few days, but dietary nicotine did not clear the infection, and after 10 days the parasite load approached that of control bees. Moreover, when pathogens were exposed to the alkaloid prior to host ingestion, the protozoan’s viability was not directly affected, suggesting that anti-parasite effects were relatively weak. Nicotine consumption in a single dose did not impose any cost even in starved bees but the alkaloid had detrimental effects on healthy bees if consistently consumed for weeks. These toxic effects disappeared in infected bees, suggesting that detoxification costs might have been counterbalanced by the advantages in slowing the progression of the infection. Nicotine consumption did not affect bee lifespan but the reduction in the parasite load may have other likely unexplored subtle benefits both for individual bees and their colony.  Potential evidence for self-medication is discussed. The contention that secondary metabolites in nectar may be under selection from pollinators, or used by plants to enhance their own reproductive success, remains to be confirmed.

Keywords

Bombus terrestris, Crithidia bombi, foraging, nicotine, pathogens, pollinators, pollinator-plant interactions, secondary metabolites

Revised Amendments from Version 2

We have included additional details regarding the nicotine used in this study including its chirality.

See the authors' detailed response to the review by James D. Thomson
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Michael Simone-Finstrom
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Marla Spivak

Introduction

Parasites can have a dramatic impact on their hosts, and consequently provide a powerful selective force for host defence mechanisms. Molecular mechanisms (e.g. the innate and adaptive immune system) are traditionally considered the major anti-parasite defences in the animal kingdom. However, hosts can rely on a range of alternative defence mechanisms, such as morphological barriers (St Leger, 1991), changes in life-history traits (Michalakis, 2009), symbiont-mediated defences (Oliver et al., 2010) and altered behaviours (de Roode & Lefèvre, 2012; Moore, 2002).

Behavioural immunity is an important modality of defence against diseases (de Roode & Lefèvre, 2012), and medication behaviour is a key immune mechanism in some animals (Clayton & Wolfe, 1993; de Roode et al., 2013). Medication behaviour has been defined as the selective use of anti-pathogenic substances by infected individuals (Lozano, 1998; Singer et al., 2009), with a measurable benefit to host fitness and negative effects on the pathogen (Abbott, 2014; Clayton & Wolfe, 1993; Singer et al., 2009). As an additional criterion to support the notion that the substance is only of value as medication, it has been proposed that healthy individuals must suffer a cost when consuming it (Abbott, 2014).

Evidence for self-medication in animals comes from the consumption of curative plants by vertebrates (Rodriguez & Wrangham, 1993). Many plants contain metabolites that display a wide range of biological activities (Cowan, 1999) which were originally evolved to combat herbivores or plant-parasites (Hadacek, 2002). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, modify their diet to include particular plant species containing medicinal substances to cure helminth infections (Mooney & Agrawal, 2008; Wrangham, 1995). There are also experimental explorations of self-medication in insects, for example two species of woolly bear caterpillars, which increase their preference for pyrrolizidine alkaloids or iridoid glycosides when parasitized, improving their chances of surviving parasitoid infection (Bernays & Singer, 2005; Singer et al., 2009; Smilanich et al., 2011). Similarly, fruit fly larvae infected by parasitoid wasp larvae preferentially consumed high-ethanol fly food as a medicine, again increasing their survival (Milan et al., 2012), while no evidence for self-medication to nematode parasitism has been found in the fly Drosophila putrida (Debban & Dyer, 2013). Trans-generational medication, but not self-medication, has been described in the monarch butterfly (Lefevre et al., 2010) and self-medication has been hypothesized for honeybees that increase plant resin collection in response to a fungal infection (Simone-Finstrom & Spivak, 2012). Finally, ants apply antimicrobial venomous secretion to the cuticle of contaminated larvae to medicate their brood (Tragust et al., 2013).

Animal societies arguably face intense pressure from pathogens, because of the high number of individuals living in high densities, relatively low genetic variability, and the relatively constant, high levels of humidity and warm temperatures within their nests (Schmid-Hempel, 1998). In addition, social pollinators, such as bumblebees and honeybees, are often exposed to an increased risk of infection via flowers (reviewed in McArt et al., 2014), which represent a shared “public place” where homo- and hetero-colonial conspecifics and other heterospecific pollinators feed repeatedly every day. Given the potential importance of parasites and disease in driving declines of managed honeybees (de Miranda & Genersch, 2010; Rosenkranz et al., 2010) and wild bumblebees (Cameron et al., 2011; Fürst et al., 2014; Schmid-Hempel et al., 2014; McMahon et al., 2015), understanding the potential relevance of pharmacophagy to social pollinators may be a key to understanding and managing these declines.

Here we use an important natural and managed pollinator, the bumblebee Bombus terrestris, and its parasite Crithidia bombi to investigate the potential for pharmacophagy in social pollinators. C. bombi, a trypanosome gut parasite, is the most prevalent parasite of bumblebees (Shykoff & Schmid-Hempel, 1991). The parasite, transmitted either vertically or horizontally (Durrer & Schmid-Hempel, 1994; Otterstatter & Thomson, 2007), infects adults per os, and two-three days post infection, infective cells are released through the faeces of bees (Schmid-Hempel & Schmid-Hempel, 1993). Queens infected by C. bombi have a reduced success in colony founding (Brown et al., 2003), and produce fewer reproductive offspring (Brown et al., 2003), while infected workers experience a higher mortality rate under stressful conditions (Brown et al., 2000). Moreover, infection impairs foraging success and learning abilities, inducing additional costs to the colony (Alghamdi et al., 2008; Gegear et al., 2006). Recent research (Manson et al., 2010; Richardson et al., 2015) has shown that several secondary metabolites such as alkaloids (including nicotine) and glycosides, reduce the C. bombi load after consumption by the bumblebee species Bombus impatiens, suggesting that these pollinators might exploit nectar toxins or other metabolites to self-medicate.

To test whether bumblebees are able to self-medicate using naturally occurring nectar secondary metabolites we conducted a series of toxicological, microbiological and behavioural experiments using a different species of Bombus (B. terrestris) and C. bombi as models and nicotine as a natural nectar alkaloid. Nicotine is encountered by pollinators at variable concentrations between 0.1 ng/μl and 3 ng/μl in floral nectar of Nicotiana species (native of South America and naturalised worldwide by humans) and Tilia species (native in most of the temperate Northern Hemisphere) (Detzel & Wink, 1993; Naef et al., 2004; Tadmor-Melamed et al., 2004).

Methods

Insects, pathogens and compounds

All experiments were performed with worker bumblebees (B. terrestris) obtained from a continuous rearing program (provided by Koppert B.V., The Netherlands) and conducted under standardized laboratory conditions. The insects were provided ad libitum with commercial pollen (provided by Koppert B.V., The Netherlands) and 30% sucrose solution as protein source and energy respectively. The parasites (the protozoan flagellates C. bombi) that we used for the experimental infections stemmed from several naturally infected colonies that were laboratory-raised from infected queens. (-)-Nicotine hemisulphate salt (≥95% (TLC), ~40% (w/v) in H2O (based on free base); N1019 Sigma) was used in all experiments.

Infection experiments

To determine whether the nectar alkaloid nicotine influences the severity of C. bombi infections in bumblebees, we designed two experiments following Manson et al. (2010). In the “Continuous Exposure” test, subjects were first inoculated with C. bombi and subsequently fed on a daily supply of nicotine solution or sucrose solution (Control), mimicking the continuous exposure to nectar constituents by a bumblebee worker. In the “Delayed Exposure” test, we first exposed directly C. bombi cells to nicotine or control solutions for two hours before inoculating bees, and then we fed them on a sucrose-only solution. We subsequently compared the parasite load in inoculated bumblebees.

We collected faeces from 30 workers from three infected colonies, in order to generate a mix of different parasite strains. The faeces were mixed for one minute with a vortex mixer and the C. bombi cocktail was left to stand at room temperature for two hours. Following this, the supernatant was removed and thoroughly mixed. Cell counts were made using a haemocytometer. Following Manson et al. (2010), faeces were mixed with sucrose solution, generating an inoculum concentration of 2,000 parasite cells/μl. Before inoculation, bees were not given any nutrition for two hours to facilitate infection. Bees derived from two different healthy colonies were screened to ensure that they were free of parasites. Bees were individually presented with a 10 μl droplet of inoculum. We observed foragers until the inoculum was consumed in its entirety. Each bee thus ingested approximately 20,000 parasite cells. This value is within the range of C. bombi cells present in the faeces of infected workers (Logan et al., 2005), thus mimicking a realistic infection level for transmission to healthy bees.

Post inoculation, in the “Continuous Exposure” test, bees from three colonies were kept individually in Petri dishes and either given a 0.5 ml solution of 2.5 ng/μl nicotine (nectar concentration in the natural range of this alkaloid) in 30% sucrose solution (Experimental bees, n = 20) or 0.5 ml of plain 30% sucrose solution (Control bees, n = 20) every day for 10 days. All bees were given a 1g pollen ball every day. In the “Delayed Exposure” test, the C. bombi inoculum was exposed to nicotine in the dark for two hours before the mixture was offered to bees for ingestion. This mimics a direct exposure of the pathogen to nicotine-laced nectar in a flower. C. bombi cells were placed in a solution of 2.5 ng/μl nicotine in 30% sucrose (Experimental treatment), and in a solution of 30% sucrose only (Control treatment). Two hours later, 20 Experimental bees and 20 Control bees were inoculated (for inoculum preparation see above). The treatment emulates a situation where Crithidia cells are deposited on a flower by infected bees and the flower is then visited by a healthy bee. Post inoculation, bees of both groups were kept individually in Petri dishes. They were provided with a fresh pollen ball and 0.5 ml of 30% sucrose solution every day.

Infection levels were determined 7 and 10 days after inoculation (the period of time in which parasite load peaks and plateaus (Schmid-Hempel & Schmid-Hempel, 1993)). Each bee was removed from its Petri dish and put into a small glass tube until it defecated. In some individual bees, too little rectal fluid was available after the initial screen; in such cases, we repeated the procedure some hours later. Faeces were transferred to a haemocytometer to count the number of parasite cells.

Laboratory toxicity bioassays

In order to determine the impact of nicotine consumption on bumblebee survival and any possible interactive effects of dietary toxin consumption and physiological stress (for which we used starvation, as Crithidia has its biggest detrimental impacts on starved bees (Brown et al., 2000)), we exposed bumblebees to artificial nectars with or without nicotine, and then kept them under starvation or with ad libitum food conditions. “Starved bees” were moved individually from their nest into Petri dishes, starved for two hours and fed either with ad libitum 30% sucrose solution food for 30 minutes (Starved, Control) or 2.5 ng/μl nicotine in 30% sucrose (Starved, Nicotine). Survival censuses were conducted every hour until all bees were dead. “Ad libitum food bees” were kept individually in Petri dishes, and provided, every day, with 0.5 ml of 30% sucrose solution plus a fresh pollen ball (Control ad libitum food), 2.5 ng/μl nicotine in 30% sucrose solution and, again, a fresh pollen ball (Nicotine ad libitum food), 2.5 ng/μl nicotine in 30% sucrose solution on day 0 and 0.5 ml of 30% sucrose solution (Nicotine-once ad libitum food) and a fresh pollen ball on a daily basis (Manson et al., 2010). Survival censuses were conducted daily until all bees had died. For each of the five treatments we chose bees from three different young healthy colonies and we randomised bees across treatment groups. Each treatment group was composed of 60 bees (20 bees per colony). Comparisons of the survival parameters of bumblebees in all treatments allowed us to evaluate the effect of nicotine, starvation, and colony membership on survival. We checked for dead bees twice daily and thus such individuals could be weighed within 12h of their death using a microscale (Navigator N30330, Ohaus, Pine Brook, USA).

Trade-off between detrimental and beneficial effects of nicotine

To evaluate whether infected bees benefit from the consumption of nicotine in terms of survival and/or parasite load, we conducted two additional experiments in which infected bumblebees received artificial nectars enriched with nicotine or not and were maintained either starved (three groups of 30 bees, 10 bees from three different colonies, 90 bees in total) or provided with ad libitum food (three groups of 45 bees, 15 bees from three different colonies, 135 bees in total). In both experiments the three groups of bees were inoculated with C. bombi as described above and individually kept in Petri dishes under three types of diet (each diet consisted of two solutions dispensed by two different Eppendorf tubes): Control Group: 30% sucrose only in both dispensers (Suc-Suc group); Exp. Group 1: 2.5 ng/μl nicotine in 30% sucrose in both dispensers (Nic-Nic group); Exp. Group 2: 30% sucrose only in one dispenser and 2.5 ng/μl nicotine in 30% sucrose in the other one (Suc-Nic group). “Starved bees” were fed for 12 days and then starved until all bees were dead. The infection levels were checked on days 7 and 10 after inoculation. Survival censuses were conducted every hour (starved bees) and every day (ad libitum food bees) until all bees were dead. At the end of the experiment we quantified total consumption of artificial nectars in each dispenser for each bee. Comparison of the survival parameters of bumblebees in all treatments enabled us to quantify the effect of nicotine and starvation on survival.

Behavioural test

For testing, each bee colony was housed in a wooden nest box (28 × 20 × 11 cm) connected to a wooden flight arena with a transparent, UV-transmitting Plexiglas lid (120 × 100 × 35 cm), by means of a transparent Plexiglas tube. Shutters along the length of this tube enabled control of the traffic of bees between nest boxes and flight arena (Chittka, 1998). Each bumblebee was individually marked with a coloured numbered disk.

Bees were pre-trained to forage on 12 square transparent plastic flowers of 24 × 24 mm (Perspex® Neutral) organized in two patches equidistant from the entrance of the nest. Plastic chips were placed on vertical transparent glass cylinders to raise them above the green floor of the flight arena. During the pre-training all flowers were rewarding with a 15 μl droplet of 30% sucrose solution, placed in a well in the centre of the flower (Raine & Chittka, 2008). This provided bees with an equal chance to associate both these patches (left and right) with reward during the pre-training period. Bees were allowed to forage freely on these flowers which were refilled as soon as the bees moved on a different artificial flower. In this way bees never experienced an empty flower with the exception of the last visited one. The number of foraging trips (bouts) made in the flight arena by each bee was observed to ensure only strongly motivated foragers visiting both patches (bees that did at least five consecutive foraging bouts) were selected for the experiment (Raine et al., 2006).

After pre-training, the preference of both healthy and infected pre-trained bees was tested for blue plastic flowers (Perspex® 727) containing nicotine (one patch reward: 2.5 ng/μl nicotine in 30% sucrose solution; one patch reward: only 30% sucrose solution). Each bee (n = 31 infected bees; n = 28 healthy bees) was tested individually and one hundred consecutive choices were recorded after the first bout was initiated. Bees were regarded as choosing a flower when they landed and fed from it. Bees approaching or just briefly touching a flower were not considered as choosing that flower. As in the pre-training, flowers were refilled after the bee moved to a different one so that bees never experienced an empty flower with the exception of the last visited one. Flowers were washed between subsequent bees in order to remove possible scent marks (Saleh & Chittka, 2006). The patch formed by nicotine-containing flowers was swapped from left to right for half the bees of each group (healthy and infected bees). Controlled illumination was provided by high frequency fluorescent lighting [(TMS 24F) lamp with HF-B 236 TLD (4.3 Khz) ballasts, Phillips, Netherlands fitted with Activa daylight fluorescent tubes, Osram] which simulated natural daylight (Dyer & Chittka, 2004). At the end of the experiment all the bees were sacrificed and the concentration of C. bombi in their hind gut was determined (see above).

Statistical analysis

In the infection experiments, 10 out of 80 bees perished by day 10 for unknown causes. Thus, we quantified the infection intensities of 40 (day 7) and 36 (day 10) bees in the “Continuous Exposure” experiment, and 37 (day 7) and 34 (day 10) bees in the “Delayed Exposure” experiment. To compare differences in parasite load between control and experimental bees 7 and 10 days after inoculation in both experiments we used a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM), with pathogen counts as the within-subject variable and C. bombi exposure to nicotine, time (day 7 and day 10), colony of origin, and bee body weight as explanatory factors. As the data were not normally distributed and homogeneity of variances and sphericity could not be assumed in several cases, we performed corrections according to Huynh-Feldt epsilon (Field, 2009). For the statistical evaluations in the survival experiments, we used the classical survival parameters (i.e. the survival distribution and the median survival time (LT50)). The survival distributions for all treatments were quantified using the Breslow Statistic (Mantel–Cox Test). The following variables were entered in the regression model: colony of origin, body weight, nicotine treatment. For the behavioural experiment, a T test was used to examine differences between preferences for nicotine-rich nectar and control nectar in healthy and infected bees. Spearman rank correlation tests were used to correlate parasite load and nicotine preference. All statistical analyses were done on SPSS 13® for Windows.

Results

Infection experiments

In both the “Continuous Exposure” and the “Delayed Exposure” tests control bees had comparable levels of C. bombi infection (t test, day 7: t = 0.16, df = 37, P = 0.98; day 10: t = 0.92, df = 34, P = 0.36). In the “Continuous Exposure” test, a diet laced with nicotine reduced the intensity of C. bombi infections in bee workers (Dataset 1). GLMM analysis revealed significant main effects of nicotine and time since inoculation on infection intensity, but not colony of origin or bee body weight (Table 1). At both 7 and 10 days after inoculation, bees exposed to nicotine had infections that were (on average) 1.11 and 0.56 times respectively less intense than control bees (t test, day 7: n = 20-20, t = 5.2, df = 38, P < 0.001; day 10: n = 18-18, t = 3.47, df = 34, P = 0.001; Figure 1). Infection intensity increased significantly from day 7 to day 10, independently of nicotine treatment (no-significant Nicotine and Colony x Time effect; Table 1).

Table 1. “Continuous Exposure” test: results from GLMM analysis of C. bombi population dynamics in bumblebees.

Factor F-value Df P-value
Nicotine diet35.31,61 0.001
Time since inoculation 16.21,61 0.001
Bee body weight1.071,610.3
Colony0.461,650.8
Interactions--N.S.
24da435d-e750-4f16-83ce-c4ef2a547e9d_figure1.gif

Figure 1. Intensity of C. bombi infections in bumblebees that received either a nicotine diet (Experimental bees, n = 20) or a sucrose only diet (Control bees, n = 20).

Faeces were checked after 7 days and 10 days post inoculation. Box plots show medians, 25th and 75th percentiles (** P < 0.001; * P = 0.001).

In the “Delayed Exposure” test, exposing C. bombi to nicotine for two hours before inoculation had no effect on parasite load (Table 2) (Dataset 1). At 7 days and 10 days post-inoculation, bees exposed to nicotine had infections that on average were as intense as those of control bees (t test, day 7: n = 19-18, t = 0.16, df = 35, P = 0.87; day 10: n = 17-17, t = -0.69, df = 32, P = 0.5; Figure 2). Infections intensified significantly from day 7 to day 10, independently of nicotine treatment (there was no significant Nicotine x Time and Colony x Time effects; Table 2). Taken together, these findings prove the antimicrobial activity of nicotine against the pathogen when ingested by bumblebees, but also indicate that when pathogens are exposed to the alkaloid prior to host ingestion the protozoan’s viability is not strongly affected.

Table 2. Delayed Exposure test: results from the GLMM analysis of C. bombi population dynamics in bumblebees.

Factor F-value Df P-value
Nicotine pre-treatment0.021,620.8
Time since inoculation27.11,600.001
Bee body weight0.521,620.4
Colony2.91,620.1
Interactions--N.S.
Bee CE-ExpParasites 7 CE-ExpParasites 10 CE-ExpColony CE-ExpDiet CE-ExpBody weight CE-ExpBee DE ExpParasites (7) DE ExpParasites (10) DE ExpColony DE ExpDiet DE ExpBody weight DE Exp
1238318110.411469541110.392
2315589110.32401648110.26
3615806110.33855779110.336
4220332110.284801634110.216
5274517110.265532609110.214
6497110.276287110.278
7674748110.337375574110.356
8655839110.228664752110.24
9260350110.239226914210.294
109891108110.2610521615210.236
11468624210.1911378511210.266
12445527210.2512201443210.182
13300535210.3413331456210.27
14445647210.314345851210.188
15422609210.3415405513210.332
16542475210.3116710954210.246
17607583210.2917383868210.274
18401210.2318357391210.29
19238485210.2719337210.208
20444600210.2320532957120.33
21213196120.2621699783120.14
22146243120.2822458520120.272
23110150120.2223397806120.252
24182550120.324271650120.312
25228120.2525775841120.284
26198563120.2226377435120.166
27160309120.2527366453120.332
28214231120.1728429591120.24
29322325120.3729641688120.266
30242427120.2930638757220.28
31355845220.2231267556220.356
32267406220.27324981101220.268
33166142220.2933311477220.32
34293344220.3134489760220.332
35180494220.3835371494220.266
36215302220.2736249220.236
37280220.2537191937220.164
38270327220.3
39138503220.33
Dataset 1.Infection experiments.
Effect of nicotine on parasite load in infected bumblebees.
24da435d-e750-4f16-83ce-c4ef2a547e9d_figure2.gif

Figure 2. Intensity of C. bombi infections in bumblebees inoculated with pathogens previously exposed to nicotine for two hours (Experimental bees, n = 20) or to a control sucrose diet (Control bees, n = 20).

Faeces were checked after 7 days and 10 days post inoculation. Box plots show medians, 25th and 75th percentiles (P = N.S.).

Laboratory toxicity bioassays

In the “Starved” test, statistical evaluation of the survivorship of control and experimental bumblebees revealed that a nicotine diet was not a significant predictor of mortality (Log-rank Mantel Cox test χ2 = 0.21, df = 1, P = 0.88; Figure 3A) (Dataset 2). Furthermore no effect of colony of origin and bee body weight on mortality was found (GLM, treatments: F = 1.1, df = 1, P = 0.29; Colony F = 0.46, df = 2, P = 0.63; body weight: F = 0.19, df = 1, P = 0.66). The median lethal time (LT50) for the two groups did not differ (control LT50: 39 hours, exp. bees LT50 = 37 hours).

24da435d-e750-4f16-83ce-c4ef2a547e9d_figure3.gif

Figure 3.

A: Cumulative survival of healthy bees fed with a sucrose solution with (blue line) or without (green line) nicotine and starved. B: Cumulative survival of healthy bees that received a daily diet of sucrose solution with (beige line), or without nicotine (blue line), or a single dose of nicotine on day one (green line).

In the “ad libitum food” test a Log-rank Mantel Cox test showed that a daily diet including nicotine was a significant predictor of mortality (χ2 = 11.56, df = 2, n = 180, P = 0.003; Figure 3B) (Dataset 2). Pairwise statistical comparisons revealed that bees fed consistently with nicotine had significantly lower survivorship than ‘Nicotine-once’ and ‘Control bumblebees’ (P = 0.001), while the latter two experimental groups did not differ (P = 0.86). LT50 of bees fed daily with nicotine was 39 days while ‘Nicotine-once’ bumblebees and control bees had a LT50 of 44 and 43 days respectively. Colony of origin and body weight did not affect bee mortality (GLM, Colony: F = 0.35, df = 2, P = 0.71; body weight: F = 1.90, df = 1, P = 0.16), but we found a significant interaction between body weight and treatment (larger bees were less susceptible to nicotine, GLM, F = 5.12, df = 1, P = 0.025). Taken together, these findings indicate that nicotine has some detrimental effects on healthy bumblebees if consistently consumed for weeks but also that these effects are possibly quite weak.

Dataset 2.Revision 1. Laboratory toxicity bioassays.
Effect of nicotine on healthy bee survival. The ‘Body weight (ad libitum)’ column previously contained duplicate data, the correct values have now been reinstated.

Trade-off between detrimental and beneficial effects of nicotine

In both “ad libitum food bees” and “starved bees” tests, a nicotine diet was not a significant predictor of survival (Log-rank Mantel Cox test: “ad libitum food bees”: n = 135, Nic-Nic vs Nic-Suc χ2 = 0.3, P = 0.6; Nic-Nic vs Suc-Suc χ2 = 0.01, P = 0.9; Nic-Suc vs Suc-Suc χ2 = 0.7, P = 0.4; “Starved bees”, n = 76; Nic-Nic vs Nic-Suc χ2 = 0.4, P = 0.5; Nic-Nic vs Suc-Suc χ2 = 0.1, P = 0.7; Nic-Suc vs Suc-Suc χ2 = 0.01, P = 0.9) (Dataset 3). Furthermore no effect of colony of origin on mortality was found (GLM, “ad libitum food bees”: F = 1.4, df = 2, P = 0.24; “Starved bees”: GLM, F = 2.02, df = 2, P = 0.14). The median lethal time LT50 for the three groups did not differ (“ad libitum food bees”: Suc-Suc LT50: 22 days, Nic-Suc LT50 = 23, days, Nic-Nic LT50 = 22; “Starved bees”: Suc-Suc LT50: 25 hours, Nic-Suc LT50 = 28 hours, Nic-Nic LT50 = 31 hours).

GLMM analysis revealed significant main effects of treatment (df = 2, F = 3.46, P = 0.03) and time since inoculation (df = 1, F = 57.3, P < 0.001) on infection intensity, but not colony of origin (df = 2, F = 1.64, P = 1.96). No interaction between diet, time and colony was significant. Overall bees caged in Petri dishes consumed less food over the entire duration of the experiment if exposed to nicotine (Anova test: F = 9.68, n = 90, df = 2, 87, P = 0.001; Dunnett T3 post hoc test: Suc-Suc vs Nic-Nic and Suc-Suc vs Nic-Suc P < 0.001) (Dataset 4). Infected bees showed a slight preference (54 ± 17%) for sucrose solution laced with nicotine (Paired samples t test, t = 2.14, df = 29, n = 30, P = 0.04).

Overall these findings indicate that, even though nicotine reduces the parasite load in infected bees, and such bees have a slight preference for sucrose solution laced with the alkaloid, there is no net benefit in term of survival for infected bees.

Bees (Starved)Colony (Starved)Treatment (Starved)Treatnum (Starved)Survival hours (Starved)Colony (ad libitum)Treatment (ad libitum)Treatnum (ad libitum)Survival days (ad libitum)
461NN 321NN112
443NS 231NN113
281NS 2141NN116
213SS 1121NN118
132SS 1191NN119
402NS 291NN121
683SS 1121NN121
713SS 1131NN121
182SS 1181NN123
21SS 1151NN125
511NN 3181NN126
521NN 3161NN127
392NS 2181NN127
622NN 3291NN126
332NS 2171NN128
653NN 3202NN134
202SS 1202NN117
883NN 3192NN117
142SS 1192NN119
41SS 1242NN121
673NN 3242NN121
783NS 2212NN122
61SS 1222NN123
501NN 3252NN123
122SS 1232NN124
412NS 2232NN124
703SS 1232NN124
663NN 3242NN125
291NS 2242NN129
11SS 1252NN131
723SS 1253NN16
231NS 2263NN114
773NS 2273NN116
271NS 2273NN117
422NS 2283NN119
192SS 1293NN121
602NN 3303NN121
241NS 2303NN122
321NS 2303NN122
873NN 3303NN124
471NN 3313NN125
582NN 3323NN125
51SS 1323NN128
301NS 2333NN129
342NS 2343NN130
491NN 3341NS216
562NN 3351NS217
81SS 1351NS219
433NS 2361NS220
112SS 1361NS220
572NN 3361NS221
612NN 3391NS221
172SS 1391NS222
372NS 2401NS224
642NN 3401NS225
31SS 1401NS225
352NS 2401NS227
793NS 2421NS231
552NN 3421NS228
531NN 3431NS228
162SS 1442NS28
451NN 3442NS211
71SS 1452NS216
101SS 1462NS218
541NN 3462NS218
362NS 2472NS220
632NN 3482NS220
261NS 2492NS223
251NS 2502NS224
592NN 3502NS225
693SS 1502NS227
91SS 1512NS229
152SS 1522NS229
311NS 2522NS231
481NN 3522NS227
803NS 2533NS212
223SS13NS218
382NS23NS219
733SS13NS220
743SS13NS221
753SS13NS221
763NS23NS223
813NS23NS223
823NS23NS223
833NS23NS224
843NN33NS226
853NN33NS231
863NN33NS232
893NN33NS231
903NN33NS226
1SS315
1SS33
1SS311
1SS314
1SS314
1SS316
1SS318
1SS318
1SS319
1SS321
1SS322
1SS324
1SS329
1SS327
1SS330
2SS39
2SS315
2SS317
2SS320
2SS320
2SS321
2SS322
2SS322
2SS322
2SS324
2SS324
2SS327
2SS327
2SS328
2SS331
3SS315
3SS319
3SS321
3SS323
3SS324
3SS324
3SS324
3SS325
3SS325
3SS326
3SS328
3SS329
3SS326
3SS326
Dataset 3.Trade-off between detrimental and beneficial effects of nicotine.
Dietary nicotine effect on parasite load and life expectancy in infected bumblebees.
Survival (Nic-Nic)NicotineNicotineTOT Consumption (Nic-Nic)Survival (Suc-Suc)SucroseSucroseTOT Consumption (Suc-Suc)Survival (Suc-Nic)SucroseNicotineTOT consumption (Suc-Nic)
254006001025240200200234006001000
2530010001325240900900273409001240
342001200143422101000101029200100300
2401500152424501001502935011501500
248008001624261001002002014009002300
272002204472910010020019140012002600
214001300172124100200300276004001000
2240014001822271002003002460010001600
23900130022232310014001500207007001400
2160060012212110015001600248005001300
231400200162324120100220239009501850
21700300102128200200400238004001200
2580035011752220014001600223008001100
218006001421203003006002113005001800
21800700152122300300600216006001200
24800900172424300400700308005001300
21120050017212750010060026120011002300
221000300132220600600120019130012002500
25100040014252260070013002114006002000
221100120023222510000100018140013002700
27130012002527251000100020002015004001900
19130014002719221300120025002014007002100
241400110025241814001400280018140013002700
18140015002918191500500200018150014002900
191400150029191915001100260021150010002500
2315002001723211500015003210050150
21150030018212503003002350400450
21150040019213030003002014006002000
261500600212629500500100025150015003000
1915001000251924100150016002813002001500
Dataset 4.Diet preference of caged bees.
Caged infected bee preference for nicotine-laced nectars.

Preference of freely flying bees for nicotine-laced flowers

Infected bumblebees allowed to forage on plastic flowers showed a significantly increased propensity to visit nicotine rewarding flowers when compared to healthy bees (t test, n = 31, 28, t = -2.4, df = 57, P = 0.016; Figure 4) (Dataset 5). Indeed on 100 consecutive choices infected bees visited the nicotine flowers on average 64.5 ± 13.8 (s.d.) times while healthy bees visited them 54.8 ± 19.4 (s.d.) times. Since test bees were introducing nicotine into the colony throughout testing, we controlled for prior exposure to nicotine effect on nicotine preference. Bees tested later in the experiment did not show a higher or lower nicotine preference (Spearman test, Infected bees: ρ = -0.21, n = 31, P = 0.3; Control bees n = 28, ρ = 0.041, P = 0.8). There was no correlation between pathogen load and the propensity of infected bees to visit flowers with nicotine-rich artificial nectar (Spearman test: n = 31, ρ = 0.19, df = 29, P = 0.28).

24da435d-e750-4f16-83ce-c4ef2a547e9d_figure4.gif

Figure 4. Percentage of preferred flowers rewording with nicotine-rich artificial nectar by infected bees (n = 31) and healthy bees (n = 28), (t test, P = 0.016).

Infected bees visited nicotine-containing flowers 64.5 ± 13.8 (s.d.) times while healthy bees visited them 54.8 ± 19.4 (s.d.) times.

ColonyParasitesNicotine visits/100 visits
1048
1059
1058
1060
1065
1059
1050
1048
1063
1065
1057
1046
1059
1062
2058
2018
2021
2071
2015
2085
2069
2065
2080
2077
2070
2016
2029
2033
327781
328368
336690
339282
329864
336638
343256
322967
328059
340470
336167
345359
328564
335354
345557
326266
422954
437359
422046
413353
434888
456595
424379
46776
433350
48362
421259
430950
447581
428056
Dataset 5.Behavioural test.
Percentage of preferred flowers rewording with nicotine-rich artificial nectar by infected and healthy bees.

Discussion

Here we demonstrate that parasitized bumblebees modify their diet preference and foraging behaviour, delaying the development of an infection. In our experimental setup the parasite infection induced an increased consumption of nicotine both in individually caged as well as in foraging bumblebees. Healthy bees exposed to nicotine suffer an appreciable cost, also in line with the key criteria for self-medication (Abbott, 2014; Clayton & Wolfe, 1993; Singer et al., 2009). However, despite this preferential ingestion of a “non-nutritive” antimicrobial alkaloid by infected bees, this behaviour appears to be of limited efficiency since dietary nicotine does not fully cure C. bombi infection, or increase longevity of infected bees, similarly to a recent study on the North American Bombus impatiens (Richardson et al., 2015). Nonetheless bumblebees in our study exhibited a reduced C. bombi load after daily consumption of the alkaloid making the existence of a potential self-medication phenomenon plausible. In nature, infection entails an array of costs and higher mortality under stressful conditions (Alghamdi et al., 2008; Brown et al., 2000; Brown et al., 2003; Gegear et al., 2006). As a consequence, any reduction in the severity or progression of infection in bees, induced by mechanisms such as the consumption of nectar containing curative alkaloids (e.g. gelsemine (Manson et al., 2010), anabasine and nicotine (Richardson et al., 2015)), might be beneficial in terms of fitness for both bees and colonies.

Even if we cannot completely exclude that the limited effect of nicotine is due to the initial challenge being too strong for the nicotine to have a measurable influence on life expectancy, both nicotine concentration and Crithidia inocula used in our study simulated natural doses. It is, however, possible that the nicotine concentration available to workers within the colony is substantially different from that found in flowers. At one end of the scale of possibilities, nicotine-laced nectar might be regurgitated into honeypots containing nicotine-free nectar, resulting in further dilution of the nicotine concentration and even lower anti-microbial effects. On the other hand, if some individual foragers that have discovered alkaloid-containing nectar deposit it predominantly into certain honeypots, and concentration is further increased by evaporation and possibly modified by enzymatic addition, then such honeypots could contain substantially higher nicotine concentrations than found in floral nectar (Thomson, 2015). It is possible that such honeypots could be a kind of colony ‘pharmacy’ specifically used for self-medication of infected workers, or to feed larvae to limit the spread of an infection within the colony (James Thomson, personal communication). Similar considerations apply to pollen, which might also contain alkaloids with antimicrobial properties (Thomson, 2015). Additional field and mesocosm tests, as well measurements of alkaloid concentration in colony honeypots are thus needed to evaluate the actual concentrations to which different colony members and the brood are exposed, and thus to clarify the actual benefits of differential foraging for such substances.

Nicotine also has a costly effect on uninfected individuals, as shown by our toxicological assays. A daily diet containing nicotine, lasting more than two months, reduced the life expectancy of bumblebees, and this effect was stronger in smaller bees. This might possibly be aggravated in the wild, where bees are exposed to other stressors and do not have access to ad libitum food. However, we note that differences in mortality rate between controls and nicotine-treated bees started to be evident only after 20 days from the first exposure suggesting that in nature this detrimental effect may be mitigated due to the relatively short lifespan of foragers in the wild (da Silva-Matos & Garófalo, 2000). Moreover, in nature, bees are unlikely to visit a single nectar source continuously for weeks as in our experiments, further reducing the negative effect of nicotine intake. In infected bumblebees, which show a shorter lifespan than healthy ones, the detrimental effect of nicotine is no longer evident suggesting that detoxification costs might be counterbalanced by the advantages in slowing the progression of the infection. However, contrary to our prediction, we found no trade-off between costs and benefits in terms of survival, and infected bumblebee lifespan was not affected by the consumption on nicotine. Our analysis focussed on individual bumblebees in isolation and there might be many other subtle long term benefits we have not explored, such as possible benefits at the colony level. It might be that lifespan analysis of forager bumblebees would be different in a social setting. Infected bumblebees have impaired learning abilities (Alghamdi et al., 2008; Gegear et al., 2006) and a reduction in parasite load might affect foraging efficiency or nursing ability, in turn enhancing colony productivity. Moreover, nicotine might be beneficial in slowing the progression of the infection through a colony, allowing, for example, the queen to lay more eggs or the larvae to prosper.

The cost imposed by the consumption of nicotine in our experiments may explain why healthy bees did not constantly consume high doses of nicotine (Tiedeken et al., 2014). Similarly, infected bees kept in Petri dishes reduced the overall uptake of food if exposed to nicotine. This is surprising given that those bumblebees also had a slight preference for sucrose solution laced with the alkaloid, and free-flying healthy bumblebees were not repelled by artificial nectar laced with nicotine. While these behavioural preferences may be explained by the impact that some nectar alkaloids, including nicotine, have on learning and memory in bees (Chittka & Peng, 2013; Thany & Gauthier, 2005; Wright et al., 2013), the mechanism behind the overall reduced consumption caused by nicotine remains unexplained. In humans at least, it is well established that nicotine has appetite-reducing effects (Jessen et al., 2005).

Currently it is unclear how nicotine acts on C. bombi. Nicotine is a highly toxic molecule (Benowitz, 1998) that acts against a wide spectrum of bacterial and fungal pathogens (Pavia et al., 2000). House sparrows and several finch species, for example, add smoked cigarette butts retaining substantial amounts of nicotine to their nests to reduce mite infestations (Suárez-Rodríguez et al., 2013). While our in vivo microbiological experiments prove the antimicrobial activity of nicotine against the pathogen when ingested, they also suggest that nicotine does not directly interfere with the protozoan’s viability, at least when measured as infectivity. As suggested by Manson et al. (2010), who found similar effects of the natural alkaloid gelsemine, an alkaloid-rich diet might increase a bee’s excretion rate, as occurs for nectarivorous bird (Tadmor-Melamed et al., 2004), effectively “flushing” C. bombi cells from the gut. Another possibility might be that nicotine, or perhaps its metabolites, directly modify the mid-gut epithelium or the environment of its lumen, making it less suitable for the parasite.

In conclusion, we showed that when infected, bumblebees use a nectar alkaloid, slowing the progression of the infection. Nicotine consumption did not affect bee lifespan but the reduction in the parasite load may have other likely subtle benefits both for individual bees and colony. Recent findings confirm the suggestion that the preferential ingestion of natural nectar secondary metabolites in pollinators might play a key role in mediating pathogen transmission within and between colonies (Richardson et al., 2015) or interactions among pollinators and their parasites (Manson et al., 2010). The observed increased ingestion of a nectar alkaloid might be a generalized response to sickness and not just to Crithidia. Similarly, our results and other recent studies (Gherman et al., 2014; Richardson et al., 2015) provide potential evidence for self-medication mediated by the consumption of plant secondary metabolites. Yet, the conditions under which nicotine and other alkaloids consumption provides benefits to either bees or plants remain to be identified. We thus believe that a careful approach to interpreting impacts of plant metabolites on insect parasites is warranted. The contention that secondary metabolites in nectar may be under selection from pollinators, or used by plants to enhance their own reproductive success (Chittka & Peng, 2013; Thomson et al., 2015; Wright et al., 2013), should ideally be confirmed with further studies, which examine the impacts of these metabolites on both bee and plant fitness under field-realistic conditions.

Data availability

F1000Research: Dataset 1. Infection experiments, 10.5256/f1000research.6262.d44610 (Baracchi et al., 2015a).

F1000Research: Dataset 2. Revision 1. Laboratory toxicity bioassays, 10.5256/f1000research.6262.d48008 (Baracchi et al., 2015b).

F1000Research: Dataset 3. Trade-off between detrimental and beneficial effects of nicotine, 10.5256/f1000research.6262.d44613 (Baracchi et al., 2015c).

F1000Research: Dataset 4. Diet preference of caged bees, 10.5256/f1000research.6262.d44614 (Baracchi et al., 2015d).

F1000Research: Dataset 5. Behavioural test, 10.5256/f1000research.6262.d44615 (Baracchi et al., 2015e).

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 3
VERSION 3 PUBLISHED 19 Mar 2015
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Baracchi D, Brown MJF and Chittka L. Behavioural evidence for self-medication in bumblebees? [version 3; peer review: 3 approved] F1000Research 2015, 4:73 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.6262.3)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 2
VERSION 2
PUBLISHED 26 May 2015
Revised
Views
27
Cite
Reviewer Report 22 Sep 2015
Michael Simone-Finstrom, Department of Entomology, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC, USA 
Approved
VIEWS 27
I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Simone-Finstrom M. Reviewer Report For: Behavioural evidence for self-medication in bumblebees? [version 3; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2015, 4:73 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.7009.r8767)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
37
Cite
Reviewer Report 08 Jul 2015
James D. Thomson, Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada 
Approved
VIEWS 37
I have now looked through the new version and can happily remove my reservations regarding the earlier version.  My main biological concern—that dosages of secondary metabolites need to be considered in the light of honeypot dynamics—is now given an intelligent ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Thomson JD. Reviewer Report For: Behavioural evidence for self-medication in bumblebees? [version 3; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2015, 4:73 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.7009.r9415)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 19 Mar 2015
Views
81
Cite
Reviewer Report 20 Apr 2015
Marla Spivak, Department of Entomology, University of Minnesota, St Paul, MN, USA 
Approved
VIEWS 81
Determining the extent that bumble bees may self-medicate by consuming floral nectar containing antimicrobial secondary plant metabolites when infected with a pathogen is a fascinating line of study.  The experiments presented are well conducted and analyzed, and I found the ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Spivak M. Reviewer Report For: Behavioural evidence for self-medication in bumblebees? [version 3; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2015, 4:73 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.6717.r8216)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 29 Oct 2015
    Lars Chittka, Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Road, London, E1 4NS, UK
    29 Oct 2015
    Author Response
    Referee comment: Determining the extent that bumble bees may self-medicate by consuming floral nectar containing antimicrobial secondary plant metabolites when infected with a pathogen is a fascinating line of study.  ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 29 Oct 2015
    Lars Chittka, Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Road, London, E1 4NS, UK
    29 Oct 2015
    Author Response
    Referee comment: Determining the extent that bumble bees may self-medicate by consuming floral nectar containing antimicrobial secondary plant metabolites when infected with a pathogen is a fascinating line of study.  ... Continue reading
Views
59
Cite
Reviewer Report 10 Apr 2015
Michael Simone-Finstrom, Department of Entomology, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC, USA 
Approved
VIEWS 59
The authors presented a fairly comprehensive set of experiments in order to elucidate the role that nicotine may have in self-medication of bumblebees against Chrithidia infection. The experiments progress well, starting with controlled lab infection studies to lab-based diet and lifespan analysis to ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Simone-Finstrom M. Reviewer Report For: Behavioural evidence for self-medication in bumblebees? [version 3; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2015, 4:73 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.6717.r8215)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 29 Oct 2015
    Lars Chittka, Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Road, London, E1 4NS, UK
    29 Oct 2015
    Author Response
    Referee comment: First, I do think “contradictory” could be removed from the title as I don’t think the results are necessarily contradictory, but do just show a weak effect. Just ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 29 Oct 2015
    Lars Chittka, Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Road, London, E1 4NS, UK
    29 Oct 2015
    Author Response
    Referee comment: First, I do think “contradictory” could be removed from the title as I don’t think the results are necessarily contradictory, but do just show a weak effect. Just ... Continue reading
Views
120
Cite
Reviewer Report 27 Mar 2015
James D. Thomson, Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 120
Content of paper. This paper reports a set of nicely designed experiments aimed at determining whether Bombus terrestris self-medicates against trypanosome (Crithidia bombi) infection by seeking and consuming nicotine-laced floral nectar. Using captive bee colonies and artificial flowers in laboratory ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Thomson JD. Reviewer Report For: Behavioural evidence for self-medication in bumblebees? [version 3; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2015, 4:73 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.6717.r8036)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 29 Oct 2015
    Lars Chittka, Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Road, London, E1 4NS, UK
    29 Oct 2015
    Author Response
    Referee comment: …Any anti-parasitic medicinal effects of toxic compounds will depend on dose rates. Too much medicine may harm the host; too little may exert no therapeutic effect. Baracchi et al. state ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 29 Oct 2015
    Lars Chittka, Department of Biological and Experimental Psychology, School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Road, London, E1 4NS, UK
    29 Oct 2015
    Author Response
    Referee comment: …Any anti-parasitic medicinal effects of toxic compounds will depend on dose rates. Too much medicine may harm the host; too little may exert no therapeutic effect. Baracchi et al. state ... Continue reading

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 3
VERSION 3 PUBLISHED 19 Mar 2015
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.