Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Reducing the stigma of mental illness in undergraduate medical education: a randomized controlled trial

Abstract

Background

The stigma of mental illness among medical students is a prevalent concern that has far reaching negative consequences. Attempts to combat this stigma through educational initiatives have had mixed results. This study examined the impact of a one-time contact-based educational intervention on the stigma of mental illness among medical students and compared this with a multimodal undergraduate psychiatry course at the University of Calgary, Canada that integrates contact-based educational strategies. Attitudes towards mental illness were compared with those towards type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM).

Method

A cluster-randomized trial design was used to evaluate the impact of contact-based educational interventions delivered at two points in time. The impact was assessed by collecting data at 4 time points using the Opening Minds Scale for Health Care Providers (OMS-HC) to assess changes in stigma.

Results

Baseline surveys were completed by 62% (n=111) of students before the start of the course and post-intervention ratings were available from 90 of these. Stigma scores for both groups were significantly reduced upon course completion (p < 0.0001), but were not significantly changed following the one-time contact based educational intervention in the primary analysis. Student confidence in working with people with a mental illness and interest in a psychiatric career was increased at the end of the course. Stigma towards mental illness remained greater than for T2DM at all time points.

Conclusions

Psychiatric education can decrease the stigma of mental illness and increase student confidence. However, one-time, contact-based educational interventions require further evaluation in this context. The key components are postulated to be contact, knowledge and attention to process, where attending to the student’s internal experience of working with people with mental illness is an integral factor in modulating perceptions of mental illness and a psychiatric career.

Peer Review reports

Background

Mental illness remains profoundly stigmatized despite numerous initiatives to combat the negative stereotypes [15]. Many of these campaigns have focused on changing the attitudes of medical professionals, as they often carry an equal or greater degree of stigma towards mental illness than do those in the general public [1, 511]. Stigma can be understood as a combination of problems of knowledge (ignorance), attitudes (prejudice) and behaviour (discrimination) [3] and has been described as a “primary barrier” to treatment and recovery [1, 12]. It can be particularly damaging when it comes from medical professionals, to whom people turn for help, and has well documented detrimental effects on both patient care and physician health [2, 79, 13]. Not only does stigma add to disease burden by preventing people from seeking timely help [14], but it is also perpetuated as future generations of doctors assimilate stereotypes from the medical culture [1, 15]. Thus, there is a need to explore effective interventions that reduce the negative attitudes that health care providers may have towards individuals with mental illness.

Amongst health care providers, medical students are a particularly important group to target with regards to attitudes towards people with mental illness. Attitudes early on in training tend to be more amenable to change and tend to harden as students progress through medical school and residency [1517]. As future doctors, they will be influential in shaping the culture of medicine and their responses, or lack of, to incidents of stigmatizing behavior or attitudes will model for others what physicians consider to be appropriate behavior [1]. Furthermore, medical students and physicians are at higher risk of burnout and addictions than others in the general public yet are reluctant to seek help due to the associated stigma [13, 18, 19]. Part of this reluctance might also stem from the potential negative consequences on a physician’s career if they disclose having a mental illness, as medicine is a regulated profession in which disclosure of a mental illness can limit a physician’s ability to practice [20]. It has also been postulated that the stigma of mental illness contributes to the shortage of medical students choosing a psychiatric career, due to perceptions of it being an unrewarding and stressful profession [17, 21, 22]. Students are exposed to a medical culture in which psychiatrists have a more pessimistic view of mental illness than those in the general public [79]. This “physician bias” may be due to psychiatrist’s clinical experiences of trying to treat those who are most ill, do not recover fully or relapse frequently, ultimately shaping their perspectives on mental illness, recovery and patient care [10]. Thus, finding effective methods to improve medical student attitudes towards mental illness and psychiatry may be a potent way to disrupt the cycle of stigma in the medical culture and improve patient care [23].

According to a recent position paper by the Canadian Psychiatric Association, “conventional education on mental illness and mental health alone does not help reduce stigmatizing attitudes and behaviors” [1] and some have even described medical schools as a “breeding ground for stigma and discrimination” towards mental illness [1, 2, 7]. Numerous strategies have been proposed to help combat mental illness stigma in medical students, including knowledge and contact-based initiatives [1, 2, 5, 6, 2427]. Among these, contact-based educational strategies have emerged as particularly effective and involve statements made by people about their lived experience of mental illness and their interaction with the health care system [1, 2, 2527]. This strategy of using social contact aims to reduce stigma by providing the opportunity for interpersonal contact between people who have a history of mental illness and audiences who may be stigmatizing towards them [24]. Although some medical schools employ contact-based teaching methods as part of their psychiatric curricula, few have evaluated the effectiveness of the programs on reducing the stigma of mental illness and increasing student confidence in working with people with mental illness.

Furthermore, stigma has been criticized as being too vaguely defined and individually focused [28] and thus existing models have defined stigma as a dynamic interrelationship of components. This interrelationship involves cognitive, affective and behavioural components. In our study we focus on the affective component of stigma.

Our conceptualization of stigma is the tri-partite model, which proposes that stigma is an overarching term including three core elements: knowledge (misinformation/differences in understanding due to culture or religion), attitudes (prejudice) and behaviour (discrimination) [29]. The knowledge, attitudes and behaviour framework allows clear intervention targets and units of measurement [29, 30]. The importance of knowledge, attitudes and behaviour has been established in medical education with medical students, nurses, and other health care providers [2932]. The knowledge, attitudes and behaviour framework is also one that is widely used in health promotion [33, 34]. The tri-partite model focuses on the problem of attitudes in the form of prejudice which can be elicited as common stereotypes or emotional reactions rather than separating them like other out-dated models. The tri-partite model is adaptable in that it allows for attitudes towards people with mental illness to be comprised of the various dimensions of stigma [29].

These dimensions include: 'perceived stigma’, which refers to one’s belief that others perceive an individual as socially unacceptable [9, 3436] and 'self-stigma’, which refers to a similar, internalized perception of oneself leading to the fear of seeking help or disclosing one’s mental illness due to the stigma associated with mental illness [37]. Other dimensions of stigma also include social distance, which refers to one’s desire to maintain distance from people with mental illness [28, 38], 'dangerousness’, which refers to one’s belief that the individual is dangerous [36], recovery, which refers to one’s belief that people with mental illness can recover [39]. Emotional reactions [40] such as a lack of social responsibility as well as a lack of empathy or compassion towards people with mental illness are also dimensions of stigma [40]. While some may consider that compassion and social responsibility may be paternalistic and stigmatizing towards people with mental illness [41], these dimensions can be seen as important indicators of non-stigmatizing attitudes and are acknowledged widely as significant competencies of health care providers and those in training to become a health care provider [9, 42].

In our study we targeted attitudes with our intervention which was contact-based. We conducted a cluster-randomized, “wait-list” controlled trial to evaluate the impact of a one-time contact-based educational intervention on the stigma of mental illness as measured by attitudes among medical students as compared with a multimodal undergraduate psychiatry course that integrates contact-based educational strategies. Attitudes towards mental illness were compared with those towards type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM), a stigmatized but non-mental health-related illness [43].

Methods

A randomized control trial was designed to assess the impact of two different educational interventions on medical student attitudes towards mental illness: a one-time contact based educational intervention and a 4 week mandatory psychiatry course at the University of Calgary, in Calgary (U of C), Canada. The Psychiatry and Family Violence Course is part of the U of C Medical School’s three-year, year-round program where clinical presentations are the foundation of the curriculum [44] and the majority of students have an undergraduate or graduate university degree prior to entering medical school. Students completed the course in their second year immediately prior to starting the clerkship component of their education.

The course learning objectives were based on the Medical Council of Canada’s (MCC) Objectives for the Qualifying Examination Part I, which are based on the Royal College of Physicians and Surgeons of Canada’s CanMEDS (Canadian Medical Educations Directives for Specialists) framework for physicians’ roles [45].

The course content was organized according to the following topics: Psychiatric interviewing and the Mental Status Examination, addictions, ADHD, anxiety disorders, eating disorders, emergency psychiatry, psychiatry and the law, psychiatry of the elderly, organic causes of mental illnesses, mood disorders, mood disorders in children and adolescents, personality disorders, pharmacology, psychosis, psychosomatic disorders, psychotherapy and the general physician, the sexually concerned patient, suicide, family issues and violence.

Students were taught about mental illness using the biopsychosocial model [46]. Mental disorders were classified according to criteria established by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) [47].

The course has been the highest rated course in the medical school based on student evaluations for the last seven years, and incorporates various teaching methods including didactic teaching (30 hours), case-based teaching with group discussions (12 hours) and an optional movie night with a post-movie discussion about mental illness (3–4 hours). It also includes two teaching methods that involved contact with people who have mental illness: patient presentations (2 hours) and “clinical correlations” sessions (6 hours).

“Clinical correlations” are small group teaching sessions where 5–6 students, paired since the first day of medical school, are mentored by a psychiatrist and directly interact with patients with a mental illness in an inpatient or out-patient setting. They provide students with an opportunity to practice their psychiatric skills and to process their reactions and experiences with the psychiatrist. Anecdotal reports suggest that students value these sessions (6 hours in total) and we hypothesized that students would rank this course component as the most effective teaching method.

The patient presentation component (a one-time contact based educational intervention) consisted of two, one-hour oral presentations given by patients who shared their story of having a mental illness. The first patient had recovered from medication-induced depression with psychotic symptoms and the second patient had narcolepsy and narcissistic personality disorder. Students had an opportunity to ask questions of the presenters and of the psychiatry course chair, who moderated the sessions.

To determine the impact of the entire psychiatry course on the stigma of mental illness and how this compared with the one-time contact-based educational intervention, students were cluster randomized according their clinical correlation groups into either an early intervention group or the late intervention group using a computer generated random sequence. The early group (intervention group) received the patient presentation on the first day of class, prior to the commencement of the regular course curriculum. The late intervention group (control group) received the same presentation at the very end of the course. Apart from the timing of the contact-based sessions, both groups participated in the same curriculum.

Data were collected on-line using the Opening Minds Scale for Health Care Providers (OMS-HC; Additional file 1), a validated twenty item scale [48]. Data were collected at four different time points: prior to the beginning of the course (T1), after a randomization step on the first day of class (T2), upon completion of the course (T3), and three months after the course was completed (T4). To assess for the consistency of student responses over time and to place medical student attitudes towards mental illness into a greater context, attitudes towards mental illness were compared with those for Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus (T2DM), a stigmatized but non-mental health related illness [43], using questions 4, 5, 6 and 7 of the OMS-HC (Additional file 1). The third survey, upon course completion, included additional questions to assess medical student perceptions of the course and its impact on their attitudes and behaviors.

The study was approved by the U of C Ethics Board and participation was voluntary. Students were assigned a unique identifier to maintain confidentiality and investigators directly involved with the course remained blinded to student participation. The following demographic information was collected to assess its impact on medical student attitudes: age, gender, contact with people with mental illness (family member, close friend, or having treated a patient with mental illness) and a personal history of being treated for a mental illness. Students were also asked if they have completed any psychiatry electives and if they would consider a career in psychiatry.

Data was entered twice and checked for errors and outliers. The analysis used the statistical program STATA version 11.0 [49]. Authors used frequency distributions to characterize the participants with respect to their responses on each individual questionnaire item. Two-way frequency distributions and cross-tabulations were used in the analysis.

Attitudes towards people with mental illness were measured at baseline (T1) and compared to the ratings after the intervention (T2) and at the end of the course (T3). Attitudes were found to be substantially right skewed, whereas changes in those ratings were approximately normally distributed. The authors therefore examined changes between the various sets of ratings, so that parametric statistical tests could be used. The primary analysis examined the change between the first and second set of ratings to allow for a comparison of changes at a point in time where one half of the sample had received the contact-based intervention (intervention group) and one had not (control group). It was restricted to people who completed the baseline (T1) and post-intervention survey (T2), and linear regression analysis with adjustment for baseline score was used in these comparisons. The null hypothesis that the change score did not depend on group was assessed using a likelihood ratio test. The unit of randomization (clinical correlation groups) was included in the analysis as strata. The three month follow-up provided an opportunity to examine whether changes observed during the course were sustained. The secondary analysis examined the change between the baseline ratings and the post-course ratings.

Results

Sample characteristics

Of the 179 students eligible to participate in the study, 111 completed a baseline survey (62.0% response rate). Of these, 81.0% (n=90) completed the second survey, 86.5% (n=96) completed the third survey and 52.1% (n=50) completed the 3 month follow-up survey. Although 96.1% (n=172) of the class responded to the third survey, only data from students who completed the baseline survey was used to assess the impact of the contact-based interventions.

Baseline measures

Participant characteristics are summarised in Table 1. Approximately two-thirds of respondents who completed the baseline survey were female (59.5%), between the ages of 18–25 (63.9%), and of “white” ethnicity (69.9%). Over 88% of students did not have any previous exposure to psychiatry through electives and 13.5% reported that they had been treated for a mental illness in the past. The second and third columns of Table 1 compare participant characteristics across the intervention and control groups. The two groups differed by age and interest in a psychiatric career, with more older students and those with a greater baseline interest in psychiatry in the control group. There were no other statistically significant differences between the two groups.

Table 1 Description of study sample at baseline (n = 111), n (%)

OMS-HC scores

The internal consistency of the OMS-HC, measured using Cronbach’s alpha was 0.78 at baseline, 0.75 at T2, 0.81 at T3, and 0.84 at T4. The OMS-HC produces scores between 20 (no stigma) and 100 (extreme stigma). The overall OMS-HC totals at each of the four time points were T1 = 48.6 (47.5-49.8); T2 = 48.1 (47.1-49.2); T3= 44.2 (42.8 - 45.4) and T4 = 45.2 (43.1-47.3). Table 2 shows unadjusted total stigma scores over time stratified according to intervention group. Negative change scores demonstrate decreases in stigma. At each of the four time points, there was no statistically significant difference in the scores between the two groups. Scores were lowest (least stigma) immediately following the psychiatry course.

Table 2 OM survey totals, by group

Impact of the one-time contact based intervention

The primary analysis examined if the changes in OMS-HC score depended on group. The analysis was restricted to students (n=90) that completed the baseline (T1) and post-intervention (T2) surveys. Table 3 shows the change scores between different time points for all participants and for the two groups. The model included adjustment for baseline OMS-HC scores. The null hypothesis that the change score did not depend on group was assessed via likelihood ratio tests for the main effect of group. This test showed no evidence that the change between T1 and T2 differed by group (p=0.05). There was no statistical evidence that the one-time contact-based educational intervention had differing effects on men and women.

Table 3 Changes in unadjusted OMS-HC scores according to intervention group, by study interval

However, as there were statistically significant differences between the two groups despite randomisation (Table 1) a model was created to adjust for baseline score, age, sex, career intention and group (Table 4, Model 1). After this adjustment there was a statistically significant difference in the change scores between the two groups between T1 and T2 (p = 0.03), suggesting that the effect of contact was significant. The model was repeated using a command to adjust for clustering based on clinical correlations groups (Table 4, Model 2) which again suggested that the one-time contact based educational intervention had a significant effect (p = 0.01). However, it should be emphasized that these were not preplanned analyses.

Table 4 Post-Hoc analysis using linear regression models for OMS-HCP change score by group, gender, age, and interest in psychiatry

Impact of the course

The secondary analysis examined the impact of the psychiatry course on the overall OMS-HC scores. The analysis included respondents who had completed the baseline (T1) and post-course surveys (T3). Negative scores correlate with less stigma. The psychiatry course was associated with a significant overall decrease in change scores: -4.4 OMS-HC points (95% CI: -5.8 to -3.1; p<0.0001), suggestive of less stigma upon course completion. An analysis of differences between T2 (start of the course but after one group received an intervention) and the end of the course (T3) also showed a significant overall decrease in change scores for both groups. An exploratory analysis found that covariates (age, sex, previous mental health electives, receiving treatment for a mental illness, treating a patient with a mental illness) did not predict post-course stigma scores. Stigma ratings at the 3-month follow-up remained significantly lower than baseline ratings and were not significantly different from scores at the end of the course.

Attitudes and disclosure/help-seeking sub-scales

Additional analysis examined the impact of the course on two sub-scales [29]: 1) attitudes towards people with mental illness and 2) disclosure and help seeking (Additional file 1). The psychiatry course was associated with a significant overall decrease (improvement) in both attitude items [-1.6 OMS-HC points (95% CI: -2.2 to -1.0; p<0.0001)] and disclosure items [-2.3 OMS-HC points (95% CI: -2.9 to -1.7; p<0.0001)].

Attitudes towards mental illness vs type 2 diabetes mellitus

Student survey responses were placed into a greater context by comparing their attitudes towards mental illness with T2DM. Figure 1 shows the combined mean Likert scores for four questions comparing mental illness stigma with T2DM stigma at three different times. At all three time point students carried more stigma towards mental illness than towards T2DM. No significant differences were observed between the control and intervention groups and there was no significant difference in medical student attitudes towards T2DM at each of the time points. With regards to attitudes towards mental illness, there was no difference in mean responses towards mental illness at T1 vs T2 but there was a significant improvement in student attitudes towards mental illness upon completion of the course (T3) when compared with baseline (T1) and with T2. Despite this improvement, student attitudes towards mental illness continued to be more stigmatised than those for T2DM upon course completion.

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean Likert scores for medical student responses to four questions about attitudes towards mental illness and type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM), asked at three different time points. A larger value corresponds with more stigmatizing attitudes. Error bars indicate standard error. T1 = baseline; T2 = following the intervention; T3 = following the course.

Interest in psychiatry

There was an increased interest in psychiatry as a career upon completion of the course. Of those that replied to the first three surveys, 25 students indicated that “yes” they would consider a career in psychiatry prior to the start of the course. This decreased to 13 after the first patient presentation and increased to 28 upon course completion.

Evaluation of the course and course components

Additional questions were asked at the end of the course (T3) to assess medical student perceptions of the course and its impact on their attitudes and behaviors (n = 174). The vast majority of students (89%, CI: 82.9-95.1%) thought that the course was “useful in reducing prejudice and discrimination against people with mental illness” and two thirds (66%, 95% CI: 56.8%-75.2%) thought that their “behavior towards people with mental illness will be different than what it would have been before Course VII”. The two contact-based educational methods emerged with the highest rankings when students ranked the effectiveness of each teaching method for learning about people with mental illness: 49% ranked the clinical correlations as “extremely effective” compared with 26 % for the patient presentations, clinical cases (18%), didactic teaching (4%) and movie night (3%). The students also ranked the clinical correlations component as having the greatest impact on increasing their confidence in working with people with mental illness: 58% strongly agreed vs 40% for didactic teaching and 33% for patient presentation (additional data not shown).

Discussion

This study is novel in its use of a cluster randomized control trial to evaluate contact-based educational interventions aimed at reducing the stigma of mental illness among medical students. The results support previous findings which show that medical students do carry a stigma towards mental illness [17, 19]. It also supports the notion that comprehensive medical education can be effective in reducing the stigma of mental illness and can increase medical students’ confidence in working with people with mental illness. These results suggest that it is possible to create an environment in which medical student attitudes towards mental illness can be shifted in a positive direction. However, despite the improvement in attitudes, the stigma towards mental illness remained greater than that for a stigmatized physical illness [43], type 2 diabetes mellitus.

In contrast to the positive impact of the course, the evidence was less robust that one-time contact-based educational sessions can alter medical student attitudes towards mental illness as an effect was only evident in post-hoc analysis. Although contact-based education has been proposed as a key component in stigma reduction strategies [1, 24, 50, 51] contact alone may be insufficient to significantly alter medical student attitudes towards mental illness. Some have suggested that the type of contact needs to be appropriate, where, for example, patients must be seen as having equal status [4] and where the person has successfully recovered from a mental illness [1]. Thus perhaps the patient presentations in this study were not adequate in disconfirming stereotypes of people with mental illness or the study was underpowered.

However, such requirements may not be realistic representations of clinical practice. Medical students and physicians often work with individuals with severe and refractory illness, thus see a skewed sample of those with mental illness [7, 8]. Furthermore, there is an innate power differential that exists between a physician and a patient [25, 52, 53] which may contribute to stigma and to the notion of “us” and “them” [1].

Although the evidence for one-time contact based sessions altering stigma was less robust, the course as a whole was clearly effective in improving students’ attitudes towards mental illness. One possibility is that these medical students required additional knowledge, acquired throughout the course, to help tackle the stigma of mental illness. Accurate knowledge can help correct misinformation, reduce ignorance and improve mental health literacy, especially when combined with contact strategies [6, 25, 51]. However, some types of knowledge might not be helpful in reducing the stigma of mental illness. There is emerging evidence that education about the biological nature of mental illness and framing it as a neurological brain disease may perpetuate the stigma by suggesting to some that it is irreversible and creating wider social divisions [1, 54, 55].

Moreover, the combination of knowledge and contact has not consistently reduced stigma in medical students [2, 14, 23]. Researchers have been further perplexed that improvements in attitudes towards mental illness do not necessarily translate into an increased interest in pursuing a psychiatric career [24], suggesting underlying concerns remain. This may be related to the stress that medical students experience when treating very ill or challenging patients on psychiatric rotations [21, 22, 56]. Furthermore, students can struggle with under empathizing or over-empathizing, potentially leading to poor quality care and burn out among medical students and physicians [22, 56]. These factors likely contribute to medical students lacking the confidence that they can help those with mental illness [22]. Student attitudes towards mental illness are also influenced by the culture of the medical community, which has historically had negative views of psychiatry and psychiatrists [2, 8, 57]. This includes both physician and the allied mental health care providers [2, 8]. Thus, although students may have improved attitudes towards psychiatry and mental illness, they may be reluctant to pursue a career in which they themselves are stigmatized “by association” [2, 7, 57]. Ultimately, these factors might not only result in fewer medical students choosing a psychiatric career, but could also impact the treatment that people with mental illness receive in all areas of medicine [23].

Perhaps there is an additional component that is integral in shaping medical student attitudes - attention to process (Figure 2). Process refers to “how” we do things and non-verbal behavior [58]. Recently the Canadian Psychiatric Association [1] advocated for psychiatrists to act as role models for trainees by directly addressing, debriefing and processing incidents of stigma and discrimination, and suggested that this can be a powerful tool in combating stigma and discrimination. Others have also found that “face-to-face” clinical teaching is highly valued by students and contributes to positive attitudes towards psychiatry [59], and conclusions drawn from a recent meta-analysis suggest that there is a need to “identify moderators of the effects of both education and contact” [5].

Figure 2
figure 2

Model for decreasing stigma and improving medical student attitudes towards mental illness. The authors propose that changing medical student attitudes towards mental illness requires a combination of accurate knowledge, contact-based educational methods and attention to process factors. In this model, attending to the student’s internal experience of working with people with mental illness is particularly important as it provides an opportunity to correct misconceptions that have occurred as a result of student’s knowledge and contact-based educational experiences, and can help increase student confidence in working with people with mental illness.

When examining the effectiveness of the U of C course’s individual components, the clinical correlations sessions were rated as most effective in increasing student confidence in working with people with mental illness. In this particular component of the course, attention is paid to the student’s experience of working with people at various stages of recovery from mental illness. Medical students have an opportunity to discuss their internal reactions with a psychiatrist, who can help them make sense of these. It also creates an opportunity for psychiatrists to correct misconceptions that may have arisen as a result of medical students’ education (knowledge) and clinical (contact) experiences. One could therefore speculate that paying attention to process is a crucial step in not only reducing stigma but in increasing future physician’s confidence in treating people with mental illness.

Thus, perhaps the course’s success in improving attitudes towards mental illness is due to the combined approach of knowledge, contact and attention to process: it provides a solid grounding in knowledge through the use of diverse teaching strategies that include contact-based educational methods, and perhaps most importantly, attention is paid to the student’s experience of working with people with mental illness.

One limitation of this study is that we were unable to assess the specific impact of the clinical correlations sessions due to methodological limitations such as each group having their sessions at different time points throughout the course. Future studies using qualitative approaches to examine this aspect of the course in greater depth could help ascertain which factors within the clinical correlations sessions are of greatest benefit in reducing stigma and improving student interest in psychiatry. The study may also have been underpowered to detect differences based on age, gender, past exposure to psychiatry and personal history of mental illness. Surprisingly, the student response rate was greater after the course (96%) than at baseline (62%) which could be indicative of greater student engagement following the course. This may potentially be a source of bias given that only those who initially responded were included when comparing stigma scores.

Our 3 month follow-up data had a reduced response rate and students had varied clinical experiences during the follow-up time frame depending upon which clerkship rotations they had completed. Thus, caution needs to be taken when interpreting this data. However, the availability of national statistics for this class’ career choices provides some helpful information about student’s ultimate first choice of residency greater than one year later: 9.7% (n=17) of the U of C medical students ultimately chose and matched to psychiatry as their first choice of career, a higher percentage than any other Canadian medical school and almost twice as high as the national 2012 average for Canadian Medical Graduates (5.1%) (CaRMS) [60] and US medical graduates (3.9%) [61]. This suggests that this cohort’s interest in psychiatry persisted beyond the course and throughout clerkship. A future follow-up study of these same students while in residency would be helpful to measure how exposure to the medical culture impacts medical student attitudes towards mental illness and if changes in attitudes correspond with knowledge and behavioral changes. For example, oral exams can be used to investigate knowledge and objective structured clinical examinations (OSCEs) can be used to examine behavior. This would take into account all components of the tri-partite model [29].

Conclusions

The stigma of mental illness among medical students is a prevalent concern that has far reaching negative consequences, yet not all educational interventions have been effective in reducing this stigma. Reducing the stigma of mental illness appears to require the combined effect of various components within medical education curricula: knowledge, contact-based interventions, and attending to the student’s internal experience of working with people with mental illness.

Authors’ information

At the time this study was conducted, AP was a psychiatry resident in the University of Calgary’s Faculty of Medicine and is now a clinical assistant professor at the University of Saskatchewan’s College of Medicine, Regina Campus. AK was a research associate with the Opening Minds Initiative for the Mental Health Commission of Canada and is now an assistant professor in the Department of Community Health Sciences within the University of Calgary’s Faculty of Medicine. GM is a research associate with the Mental Health Commission of Canada. GV was a senior medical student at the University of Calgary Medical School at the time of data collection. LZ was the chair of the undergraduate psychiatry course and is a clinical assistant professor in the Department of Psychiatry within the University of Calgary’s Faculty of Medicine. SP was the post-doctoral supervisor for AK, is a professor in the Departments of Community Health Sciences and Psychiatry at the University of Calgary and a Senior Health Scholar with Alberta Innovates, Health Solutions.

Abbreviations

T2DM:

Type 2 diabetes mellitus

OMS-HC:

Opening minds scale for health care providers

U of C:

University of Calgary

T1:

Time point 1, prior to the beginning of the course

T2:

Time point 2, after half the class receives the intervention on the first day of class

T3:

Time point 3, upon completion of the course

T4:

Time point 4, three months after the course was completed

CI:

Confidence interval

CaRMS:

Canadian residency matching service.

References

  1. Abbey S, Charbonneau M, Tranulis C, Moss P, Baici W, Dabby L, Gautam M, Paré M: Stigma and discrimination [position paper]. Can J Psychiatry. 2011, 56 (10): 1-9.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Sartorius N, Gaebel W, Cleveland HR, Stuart H, Akiyama T, Arboleda-Florez J, Baumann AE, Gureje O, Jorge MR, Kastrup M, Suzuki Y, Tasman A: WPA guidance on how to combat stigmatization of psychiatry and psychiatrists. World Psychiatry. 2010, 9 (3): 131-144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Thornicroft G, Rose D, Kassam A, Sartorius N: Stigma: ignorance, prejudice or discrimination?. Br J Psychiatry. 2007, 190: 192-193. 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.025791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Corrigan PW, Penn DL: Lessons from social psychology on discrediting psychiatric stigma. Am Psychol. 1999, 54 (9): 765-776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Corrigan PW, Morris SB, Michaels PJ, Rafacz JD, Rusch N: Challenging the public stigma of mental illness: a meta-analysis of outcome studies. Psychiatr Serv. 2012, 63 (10): 963-973. 10.1176/appi.ps.201100529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Thornicroft G, Brohan E, Kassam A, Lewis-Holmes E: Reducing stigma and discrimination: candidate interventions. Int J Ment Health Syst. 2008, 2 (1): 3-10.1186/1752-4458-2-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Thornicroft G, Rose D, Mehta N: Discrimination against people with mental illness: what can psychiatrists do?. Adv Psychiatr Treat. 2010, 16: 53-59. 10.1192/apt.bp.107.004481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Schultz B: Stigma and mental health professionals: a review of the evidence on an intricate relationship. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2007, 19 (2): 137-155. 10.1080/09540260701278929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Corrigan P: How stigma interferes with mental health care. Am Psychol. 2004, 59 (7): 614-625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Jorm AF, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Christensen H, Henderson S: Attitudes towards people with a mental disorder: a survey of the Australian public and health professionals. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1999, 33 (1): 77-83. 10.1046/j.1440-1614.1999.00513.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Nordt C, Rossler W, Lauber C: Attitudes of mental health professionals towards people with schizophrenia and major depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006, 114 (2): 145-146.

    Google Scholar 

  12. US Dept of Health and Human Services: Mental health: a report of the surgeon general. 1999, Bethesda, MD: US Department of Health and Human Services

    Google Scholar 

  13. Gautam M: Physicians and Depression. The Handbook of physician health: the essential guide to understanding the health needs of physicians. Edited by: Goldman LS, Myers M, Dickstein LJ. 2000, Chicago, IL: American Medical Association, 80-94.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Smith JK, Weaver DB: Capturing medical students’ idealism. Ann Fam Med. 2006, 4: S32-S37. 10.1370/afm.543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Nieuwhof MG, Rademakers JJ, Kuyvenhoven MM, Soethout MB, Ten Cate TJ: Students’ conceptions of the medical profession: an interview study. Med Teach. 2008, 27: 709-714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Korszun A, Dinos S, Ahmed K, Kamaldeep B: Medical attitudes about mental illness: does medical-school education reduce stigma?. Acad Psychiatry. 2012, 36: 197-204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Schwenk TL, Davis L, Wimsatt LA: Depression, stigma, and suicidal ideation in medical students. JAMA. 2010, 304 (11): 1181-1190. 10.1001/jama.2010.1300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Dyrbye LN, Thomas MR, Massie FS, Power DV, Eacker A, Harper W, Durning S, Moutier C, Szydio DW, Novotny PJ, Sloan JA, Shanafelt TD: Burnout and suicidal ideation among U.S. medical students. Ann Intern Med. 2008, 149 (5): 334-341. 10.7326/0003-4819-149-5-200809020-00008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Chew-Graham CA, Rogers A, Yassin N: 'I wouldn’t want it on my CV or their records’: medical students’ experiences of help-seeking for mental health problems. Med Educ. 2003, 37 (10): 873-880. 10.1046/j.1365-2923.2003.01627.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Canadian Medical Protective Association (CMPA): Physician personal Health Information: Supporting public safety and individual privacy. 2010, http://www.cmpa-acpm.ca/cmpapd04/docs/submissions_papers/com_physician_personal_health_information-e.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Cutler JL, Harding KJ, Mozian SA, Wright LL, Pica AG, Masters SR, Graham MJ: Discrediting the notion “working with 'crazies’ will make you 'crazy”’: addressing stigma and enhancing empathy in medical student education. Adv in Health Sci Educ. 2009, 14: 487-502. 10.1007/s10459-008-9132-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Cutler JL, Alspector SL, Harding KJ, Wright LA, Graham MJ: Medical students’ perceptions of psychiatry as a career choice. Acad Psychiatry. 2006, 30: 144-149. 10.1176/appi.ap.30.2.144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Feldman TB: Medical students’ attitudes toward psychiatry and mental disorders. Acad Psychiatry. 2005, 29 (4): 4-356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Pinfold V, Thornicroft G, Huxley P, Farmer P: Active ingredients in anti-stigma programmes in mental health. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2005, 17 (2): 123-131. 10.1080/09540260500073638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Arboleda-Florez J, Stuart H: From sin to science: fighting the stigmatization of mental illnesses. Can J Psychiatry. 2012, 57 (8): 457-463.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Couture S, Penn DL: Interpersonal contact and the stigma of mental illness: a review of the literature. J Ment Health. 2003, 12 (3): 291-303. 10.1080/09638231000118276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Kassam A, Glozier N, Leese M, Loughran J, Thornicroft G: A controlled trial of mental illness related stigma training for medical students. BMC Med Educ. 2011, 11 (1): 1-51. 10.1186/1472-6920-11-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Link BG, Cullen FT, Frank J, Wozniak J: The social rejection of ex-mental patients: understanding why labels matter. Am J Sociol. 1987, 92: 1461-1500. 10.1086/228672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Thornicroft G: Shunned: Discrimination against people with mental illness. 2006, New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  30. Johnston JM, Leung GM, Fielding R, Tin KY, Ho LM: The development and validation of a knowledge, attitude and behaviour questionnaire to assess undergraduate evidence-based practice teaching and learning. Med Educ. 2003, 37: 992-1000. 10.1046/j.1365-2923.2003.01678.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Hoke MM, Robbins LK: Continuing the cultural competency journey through exploration of knowledge, attitudes, and skills with advanced practice psychiatric nursing students: an exemplar. Nurs Clin North Am. 2011, 46 (2): 201-205. 10.1016/j.cnur.2011.02.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Kajermo KN, Tyni-Lenné R, Guidetti S, Samuelsson M, Andersson IL, Wengström Y: Evidence-based practice: attitudes, knowledge and behaviour among allied health care professionals. Int J Qual Health Care. 2011, 23 (2): 198-209. 10.1093/intqhc/mzq083.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. La Torre G, Semyonov L, Mannocci A, Boccia A: Knowledge, attitude, and behaviour of public health doctors towards pandemic influenza compared to the general population in Italy. Scand J Public Health. 2011, 0: 1-7.

    Google Scholar 

  34. National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence: Behaviour change at population, community and individual levels. 2007, London, England: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence

    Google Scholar 

  35. Link BG, Cullen FT: Contact with the mentally ill and perceptions of how dangerous they are. J Health Soc Behav. 1986, 27: 289-302. 10.2307/2136945.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Corrigan P, Markowitz FE, Watson A, Rowan D, Kubiak MA: An attribution model of public discrimination towards persons with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav. 2003, 44: 162-179. 10.2307/1519806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Corrigan PW, Watson AC: The paradox of self-stigma and mental illness. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2002, 9 (1): 35-53. 10.1093/clipsy.9.1.35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Bentz WK, Hollister WG, Kherlopian M: Attitudes of social distance and social responsibility for mental illness: a comparison of teachers and the general public. Psychol Sch. 1970, 7 (2): 198-203. 10.1002/1520-6807(197004)7:2<198::AID-PITS2310070218>3.0.CO;2-D.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Mueser KT, Corrigan PW, Hilton DW, et al: Illness management and recovery: a review of the research. Psychiatr Serv. 2002, 53: 1272-1284. 10.1176/appi.ps.53.10.1272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Angermeyer MC, Holzinger A, Matschinger H: Emotional reactions to people with mental illness. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2010, 19 (1): 26-32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Jacoby A: Felt versus enacted stigma: a concept revisited: Evidence from a study of people with epilepsy in remission. Soc Sci Med. 1994, 38 (2): 269-274. 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90396-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Spandler H, Stickley T: No hope without compassion: the importance of compassion in recovery-focused mental health services. J Ment Health. 2011, 20 (6): 555-566. 10.3109/09638237.2011.583949.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Hopper S: Diabetes as a stigmatized condition: the case of low-income clinic patients in the United States. Soc Sci Med Med Anthropol. 1981, 15B (1): 11-19.

    Google Scholar 

  44. University of Calgary Medical School operating Philosophy. 2010, Calgary, Canada, http://www.ucalgary.ca/mdprogram/operatingphilosophy.

  45. Medical Council of Canada’s (MCC) Objectives for the Qualifying Examination Part I. http://mcc.ca/examinations/objectives-overview/.

  46. Engel GL: The clinical application of the biopsychosocial model. Am J Psychiatry. 1980, 137: 535-544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 2000, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 4

    Google Scholar 

  48. Kassam A, Papish A, Modgill G, Patten S: The development and psychometric properties of a new scale to measure mental illness related stigma by health care providers: the opening minds scale for health care providers (OMS-HC). BMC Psychiatry. 2012, 12: 62-10.1186/1471-244X-12-62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Stata Corporation: Stata, version 11.0. [11.0]. 2009, College Station, TX: Stata Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  50. Nguyen E, Chen TF, O’Reilly CL: Evaluating the impact of direct and indirect contact on the mental health stigma of pharmacy students. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012, 47 (7): 1087-1098. 10.1007/s00127-011-0413-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Corrigan PW, River LP, Lundin RK, Penn DL, Uphoff-Wasowski K, Campion J, Mathisen J, Gagnon C, Bergman M, Goldstein H, Kubiak MA: Three strategies for changing attributions about severe mental illness. Schizophr Bull. 2001, 27 (2): 187-195. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Nadelson C, Noteman M: Boundaries in the doctor-patient relationship. Theor Med Bioeth. 2002, 23 (3): 191-201. 10.1023/A:1020899425668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Link BG, Phelan JC: Conceptualizing stigma. Annu Rev Sociol. 2001, 27: 363-385. 10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Schnittker J: An uncertain revolution: why the rise of a genetic model of mental illness has not increased tolerance. Soc Sci Med. 2008, 67 (9): 1370-1381. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.07.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Pescosolido B, Martin J, Long JS, Medina T, Phelan J, Link B: “A disease like any other?” a decade of change in public reactions to schizophrenia, depression, and alcohol dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2010, 167: 1321-1330. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09121743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Niedermier JA, Bornstein R, Brandemihl A: The junior medical student psychiatry clerkship: curriculum, attitudes, and test performance. Acad Psychiatry. 2006, 30: 136-143. 10.1176/appi.ap.30.2.136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Kurtz S, Silverman J, Benson J, Draper J: Marrying content and process in clinical method teaching: enhancing the calgary-cambridge guides. Acad Med. 2003, 78: 802-809. 10.1097/00001888-200308000-00011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Lampe L, Coulston C, Walter G, Mahli G: Up close and personal: medical students prefer face-to-face teaching in psychiatry. Australas Psychiatry. 2010, 18 (4): 354-360. 10.3109/10398561003739620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Canadian Residency Matching Service (CaRMS) Reports and Statistics. 2012, http://www.carms.ca/eng/operations_R1reports_12_e.shtml.

  60. Mark M, Fewer US: Grads matching in psychiatry. Am Psychiatr Assoc Psychiatr News. 2012, 47 (8): 1b-30b.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Stampfer H: The recruitment problem in psychiatry: a critical commentary. Educ Res Perspect. 2011, 38 (2): 1-19.

    Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the University of Calgary’s Undergraduate Medical Education support staff for their assistance with survey distribution and communication with the medical students.

Financial contributions

Funding for this work was in part made possible through funding from the Opening Minds anti-stigma initiative of the Mental Health Commission of Canada, including funding for GM and AK. The work of the Mental Health Commission of Canada is supported by a grant from Health Canada. SP’s research is supported by an Alberta Mental Health and Addictions Partnership Committee’s Collaborative Research Grant and by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. The University of Calgary’s Department of Psychiatry provided funding for page charges.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andriyka Papish.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

AP designed and conducted the study, was involved in data interpretation and drafted the manuscript. AK assisted with study design and oversaw data collection and data analysis. GM was involved with implementing the study, data collection and statistical analysis. GV assisted with implementation of the study. LZ was the course chair and assisted with study design, patient coordination and supervision of AP. SP provided guidance throughout all stages of the study and supervision of AK and GM. All authors were involved with writing and editing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Papish, A., Kassam, A., Modgill, G. et al. Reducing the stigma of mental illness in undergraduate medical education: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Med Educ 13, 141 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6920-13-141

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6920-13-141

Keywords