The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Letters to the EditorFull Access

Smoking and Cognitive Performance: The Chicken and Egg Problem

To the Editor: In the November 2018 issue of the Journal, Vermeulen et al. (1) presented an interesting comparison of cognitive performance in smokers and nonsmokers (nonsmokers did better), and in smokers with psychosis who did and did not manage to stop smoking (quitters had a better score in one subtest at follow-up). However, the authors’ conclusion that smoking cessation may improve processing speed, and the accompanying editorial (2) that goes much further and asserts that the study shows that smoking has adverse effects on, and that stopping smoking leads to a rapid recovery of, cognitive performance, may be an overinterpretation of the findings.

Adolescents with lower education and socioeconomic status are more likely to become smokers (3, 4), and it is possible that the lower cognitive performance in smokers in this sample was not caused by smoking, but preceded it. Regarding the finding that patients who achieved smoking abstinence had faster processing speed at follow-up, it is likely that patients who are in good remission are more likely to stop smoking than those who are not, and that it is the remission, rather than smoking cessation, that is accompanied by cognitive improvement (especially as it is usually also accompanied by reduced medication that the yes/no medication variable that was controlled for may not capture). This interpretation is supported by the fact that such improvements were not seen in the other cohorts in this study.

Rather than concluding that smoking impairs and that stopping smoking improves cognitive performance, a possibility needs to be considered that the results simply confirm that people who find smoking rewarding differ from those who do not and that patients in good remission are more likely to stop smoking than those whose health status and medication levels have not changed.

Wolfson Institute of Preventive Medicine, Queen Mary University of London, London.
Send correspondence to Dr. Myers Smith ().

Dr. Hajek has received research funding from and has served as a consultant to manufacturers of medications for smoking cessation. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Vermeulen JM, Schirmbeck F, Blankers M, et al.: Association between smoking behavior and cognitive functioning in patients with psychosis, siblings, and healthy control subjects: results from a prospective 6-year follow-up study. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175:1121–1128LinkGoogle Scholar

2 Harvey PD: Cigarette smoking, cognitive performance, and severe mental illness: quitting smoking really does seem to matter (editorial). Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175:1054–1055LinkGoogle Scholar

3 Hiscock R, Bauld L, Amos A, et al.: Socioeconomic status and smoking: a review. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2012; 1248:107–123Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Gilman SE, Martin LT, Abrams DB, et al.: Educational attainment and cigarette smoking: a causal association? Int J Epidemiol 2008; 37:615–624Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar