Introduction: The aims were to examine if the total and item scores on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and self-reported memory problems differed between older women and men, and if self-reported memory problems were associated with scores on the 2 tests. Methods: We included 309 home-dwelling people aged 70 years and older, 155 women, mean age 75.6 (SD 4.1) years, and 154 men, mean age 76.0 (SD 4.6) years. They were examined with MoCA and MMSE, and they answered 2 questions: “have you experienced any memory problems” and “have you experienced significant memory problems the last 5 years?” Results: The participants scored significantly higher on the MMSE (women 28.0 [1.8], men 28.4 [1.4]) than on MoCA (women 24.6 [3.3], men 24.3 [3.1]). Spearman’s rho was 0.36 between the tests. Women scored significantly higher than men on delayed recall of MoCA (3.0 [1.6] vs. 2.4 [1.6]), whereas men scored significantly higher on visuoconstruction (3.8 [1.2] vs. 3.5 [1.0]) and serial subtraction on MoCA (2.7 [0.6] vs. 2.5 [0.8]) and serial sevens on MMSE (4.5 [0.8] vs. 4.1 [1.1]). Multivariate linear regression analyses revealed that female sex, younger age, and higher education were associated with a higher score on MoCA, whereas age and education were associated with a higher score on MMSE. About half of the participants (no sex difference) had experienced significant memory problems the last 5 years, and they had significantly lower scores on both tests. Conclusions: The MoCA score was associated with sex, age, and education, whereas sex did not influence the MMSE score. The question “have you experienced significant memory problems the last 5 years?” may be useful to evaluate older people’s cognition.

With a growing number of older adults worldwide, it is estimated that in 2050, >130 million people will have dementia [1, 2]. In most cases, the first sign of dementia is a decline in cognition, most commonly a decline in episodic memory from a previous level. Today, the general approach is to identify older people with such a decline and follow them up over years because 20–50% of people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) will develop dementia [3‒5]. As no curative treatment exists for any brain disorder leading to dementia, screening is not recommended. However, simple, cheap, and valid case-detection tools to identify persons with high risk of developing dementia are requested so that these people can make plans for the future.

For >40 years, the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) has been the most used instrument to detect cognitive impairment in older adults, and in Norway, a validated version of the MMSE has been in use for the last 30 years [6, 7]. Depending on age (higher age is associated with lower score), educational level (higher education is associated with higher score), and cultural background, a score below 24 or 25 or even 26 has been recommended for the definition of cognitive impairment of significant degree [8, 9]. Using one of these cut-offs, the MMSE has been found to be poor in detecting persons with MCI but valid to detect persons with dementia [10, 11]. For this reason, and in addition that the MMSE is under copyright restriction, new case-detection tools have been developed and validated [12]. One instrument that has gained popularity is the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) that was developed mainly as an instrument to detect MCI [13]. The general view is that a score below 26 on MoCA, adjusted to 25 among people with education of 12 years and below, will identify people with MCI. Adjustment for age could also be considered [9, 13‒15]. A substantial number of studies conducted across the world, included in the systematic reviews by Pinto et al. [16] and O’Driscoll and Shaikh [17], have shown that MoCA is superior to MMSE in detecting MCI, but not dementia. However, one should bear in mind that most of these studies used the Mayo criteria for the diagnosis of MCI [18]. One of the Mayo criteria is that a diagnosis of MCI can only be made when a person has a score of −1.5 standard deviation below what is normal for the person’s age and education. If a score on MoCA or MMSE correlates highly with the cognitive test used to diagnose MCI, this could lead to a circularity problem. To avoid circularity problems, a cognitive test should be used that correlates poorly with MoCA and MMSE.

The main differences between the 2 instruments are that the MMSE has a larger focus on verbal abilities than MoCA, and it is easier to achieve a high score, for example, 28–30 points on the MMSE compared to MoCA [16, 18]. MoCA contains more items testing executive and visuoconstructive function and covers more cognitive domains than MMSE. Both instruments are scored from 0 to 30, and many studies have explored how the test scores of the 2 tests correspond to each other [9, 15‒17, 19‒22]. The main explanations for the different scores are that MoCA is more comprehensive and challenging as it is developed to detect MCI, whereas the MMSE originally was developed to detect delirium and psychiatric condition among patients in specialist health care [6].

Studies comparing the MMSE and MoCA have to a large degree included clinical samples, not community samples, to examine the discriminatory power of the tests to separate cognitively normal older adults from those with MCI and dementia [9, 13, 15‒17, 19‒22]. In these studies, adjustments have been made according to people’s age and education, but not according to sex [9, 13, 20‒22]. A few community studies have examined whether older women and men score differently on the 2 tests, and whether sex influences differently on the scores of the 2 tests [23‒27]. These studies could report sex differences, especially by the use of MoCA, with higher scores among women [23‒26]. One larger community study reported that older women had a higher score on MMSE than men [27]. Surprisingly, few studies have examined whether sex differences can be detected using the MMSE and MoCA as it is well-known since >50 years that women outperform men on verbal tasks and men perform better than women on spatial tasks [28, 29]. Later, studies have showed that these sex differences also exist in older adults, with similar sex-associated differences seen in for example, verbal memory tests, perhaps caused by underlying pathological mechanisms, or it could indicate that the sex differences found early in life persist by increasing age [29‒33].

The association between memory complaints and significant cognitive impairment and dementia has been examined in numerous studies, but only some studies have compared self-reported cognitive complaints (SCC) with scores on the MMSE and MoCA among home-dwelling older people [34‒42]. All these studies indicate that older adults with SCC are at higher risk of developing dementia than those with no complaints, although a systematic review concludes that one should not use SCC as a predictor of future development of dementia [35]. Comparison of scores on the MMSE and MoCA with a single question of memory problems has been studied in clinical samples but less in community samples [43]. In the recent study of Wanrooji et al. [43], it was found that a single question of memory complaints was associated with recall of 3 words as measured by the MMSE and development of dementia after a mean of 6.7 years. This finding needs to be replicated because if a single question could be applied as a prescreening tool, we could better target who should be tested with the MMSE or MoCA. In addition, it adds knowledge to what has been proven useful in clinical practice and research in many areas of medicine: we should listen to the patient’s experience of own disease. Probably, a general question of any memory or other cognitive problems would not be useful, whereas a specific question related to duration and severity of memory problems or memory problems related to certain situations would be more sensitive. Further, it is suggested that older women more often complain about memory problems than men, but we do not know whether this could be due to depression, which is more prevalent in women [44, 45]. Thus, it is still of interest to examine whether older women experience more about memory problems than men and if such experiences are associated with scores on the MMSE and MoCA.

To summarize, by searching the literature, only a few community studies were found that compared the total and the domain or item scores on the MMSE and MoCA between older women and men. Although many studies have been carried out to examine associations between self-report of memory problems and future development of dementia, few community studies have been published that examined the association between a single question of memory complaint and the scores on the MMSE and MoCA in older women and men. Thus, the aims of the present study were 3-fold: (1) to examine whether sex influenced on the total and domain or item scores on the MMSE and MoCA, (2) to examine sex differences between complainers and non-complainers of memory problems, and (3) to examine whether scores on the MMSE and MoCA differ between complainers and non-complainers. Based on the literature, our hypotheses are that women compared to men will score higher on MoCA, but not on the MMSE, and we expect that self-report of memory problems is more common in women than men, whereas the question is open whether older adults with self-report of memory problems score lower on the MoCA and MMSE.

Design and Participants

This cross-sectional study took place between April and June 2019 together with the population-based the Trøndelag Health Study in the city of Trondheim (HUNT4 Trondheim 70+). For the present study to compare the results on the MoCA and MMSE, we invited 504 home-dwelling people aged 70 years and older who had participated in the HUNT study. In all, 309 (61.3%) people consented to participate. The mean age of the 309 participants was 75.8 (SD 4.4) years; 155 were women with a mean age of 75.6 (SD 4.1) years, and 154 were men with a mean age 76.0 (SD 4.6) years. Those who declined participation were 100 women and 95 men. They did not differ from those who participated with regard to age (p = 0.91), sex (p = 0.81), or educational level (p = 0.67). As all 504 participants were tested with MoCA as part of the ongoing HUNT4 study, we could compare the scores of the 309 participants with the 195 who declined for being tested with the MMSE. Those who declined had a significantly lower score on MoCA than those who participated (23.5 [SD 3.5] vs. 24.5 [SD 3.2], p = 0.002).

Assessments

All participants were tested with the 3rd Norwegian version of the MMSE and the Norwegian version (7.1) of MoCA at a test station at 2 visits within 1 week by trained health personnel. MoCA was applied at the first visit and the MMSE at the second. The MMSE is a global screening test that tests orientation, memory, language, and visuospatial function with scores from 0 to 30; a higher score indicates better cognition [6]. The MoCA scale is also a multidomain cognitive screening instrument, testing memory, visuospatial and executive functions, naming, attention, abstraction, language, and orientation with scores from 0 to 30; higher scores indicate better cognitive function [13]. In addition, we used the Word List Memory Task (WLMT) from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) [46]. It tests memory with a list of ten words, which are repeated in a different order 3 times, that should be recalled immediately after each presentation (immediate recall or learning) and recalled after 10 min (delayed recall). The maximum score for immediate recall is 30 and for delayed recall, 10. A higher score indicates better verbal memory performance [46]. We asked the participants 2 patient-related outcome measures (PROM): a general question about memory problems: “have you experienced any memory problems?” (yes/no) and a specific question: “have you experienced significant memory problems the last 5 years?” (yes/no). The age at test date, sex, and self-report of the educational level expressed as years of schooling were recorded.

Statistics

For data recording and analyses, we used SPSS, 25th edition. Before analyses, we divided the participants into 3 age groups: 70–74 years, 75–79 years, and 80+ years. The educational level was divided into 3 groups according to the Norwegian school system 50 years ago. Ten years of schooling represented primary school + middle school, 11–12 years high school, and 13 years and above academic education. The χ2 test was used to test for differences in table analyses and with linear-by-linear association for 3 × 2 tables. As the normality assumption was violated for the test scores, Mann-Whitney U and Kruskal-Wallis tests were used for group comparison of results on the MMSE and MoCA. Bonferroni adjustment was used for multiple testing. Spearman’s rho, a rank correlation test, was used for correlation analyses. To explore which variables were associated with the total MMSE and total MoCA scores, and the difference between MMSE and MoCA-scores, we conducted 3 multivariate linear regression analyses including sex, age, education, and self-report of memory problems as independent variables. Enter, forward, and backward stepwise methods were performed, which did not change the results. The results of the enter method were recorded.

Personal characteristics of the participants and the results of the WLMT and the 2 PROM questions divided by sex are shown in Table 1. As can be seen, no differences were found between women and men related to age groups, but fewer women had higher academic education than men (p < 0.001). Women outperformed men on WLMT.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the participants

 Characteristics of the participants
 Characteristics of the participants

MMSE and MoCA Scores Broken Down by Sex, Age, and Education

Results of the MMSE and MoCA broken down by sex, age, educational groups, and the 2 self-reported questions of memory problems are displayed in Table 2. To further illustrate the different distribution of the scores between the 2 tests, we added the number of people with a score below 26 on each test. It should be noted that only 5 people had a score below 24, and 6 scored 24 on the MMSE. No one scored below 21 on the MMSE, whereas 31 women and 32 men, together 63 (20.4%) reached the maximum score of 30.

Table 2.

Total scores on the MMSE and MoCA by sex, age, education, and self-report of memory problems

 Total scores on the MMSE and MoCA by sex, age, education, and self-report of memory problems
 Total scores on the MMSE and MoCA by sex, age, education, and self-report of memory problems

Although the total score on the MMSE and MoCA did not differ between women and men (Table 2), stratifying the analysis by taking education into the analyses showed one difference. Women with 11–12 years of schooling scored significantly higher on the total MoCA than men, 24.4 (SD 3.4) versus 22.7 (SD 3.4), p = 0.005 (adjusted with Bonferroni correction). Otherwise, we found no differences in the total MMSE and MoCA scores between women and men in the various age and educational groups or according to self-report of memory problems.

Examination of the scores on the various domains/items revealed some differences between women and men. On MoCA, women scored significantly higher on the item delayed recall (see Table 3). This sex difference was also found in the age group 70–74 years, age group 80 years+, and the group with education of 11–12 years with mean scores (SD) for women and men: 3.1 (1.5) versus 2.6 (1.4), p = 0.003; 3.0 (1.7) versus 2.0, p = 0.04; and 3.2 (1.5) versus 1.8 (1.5), p < 0.001, respectively.

Table 3.

Gender differences in mean scores on 10 sub-items/scale of MoCA

 Gender differences in mean scores on 10 sub-items/scale of MoCA
 Gender differences in mean scores on 10 sub-items/scale of MoCA

Men had significantly higher scores than women on the items visuoconstruction (copying a cube and clock drawing test) and serial subtraction on MoCA (see Table 3), and on the item serial sevens on the MMSE, 4.5 (0.8) versus 4.1 (1.1.), p < 0.001. Such differences between men and women were further found for some subgroups: visuocontruction (MoCA) men 70–74 years 3.9 (0.8) versus women 3.5 (1.2), p 0.01; serial subtraction (MoCA) men 75–79 years 2.8 (0.6) versus women 2.4 (0.9), p = 0.04; and serial sevens (MMSE) men with 13+ years of education 4.7 (0.6) versus women 4.2 (1.1), p = 0.001.

Correlations between MMSE and MoCA

We found a weak to moderate rank correlation between the total score on the 2 tests (Spearman’s rho 0.36). We further calculated rho for women and men separately and for participants according to age and education. Spearman’s rho varied and was lowest for the group with ≤10 years of schooling (0.16) and highest for the group with 11–12 years of schooling (0.43).

The mean difference between the MMSE and MoCA scores was 3.7 (SD 2.7), highest on the MMSE, and varied between 3.4 (SD 2.7) for participants with 13+ years of education (lowest) and 4.3 (SD 3.9) for those with education of 10 years and below (highest). Among all, 277 (89.6%) had a higher score on the MMSE than on MoCA, 18 (5.8%) had an equal score on both tests (11 were women), and 14 (4.5%, 11 were women) scored higher on MoCA. Those who scored higher on MoCA or equal on both tests were significantly younger (p = 0.004), had higher education (p = 0.02), and were women (p = 0.02).

Self-Report of Memory Problems

For the 2 self-reported questions, the prevalence of a “yes” was not different for women and men. For the question “have you experienced significant memory problems the last 5 years?” we found several significant differences in scores on both the MMSE and MoCA for people who answered “yes” (lower scores) compared to those who answered “no” (higher scores) to this question (see Table 4).

Table 4.

Scores on the MMSE and MoCA among those who reported significant memory problems the last 5 years, broken down by sex, age, and educational groups, n = 309

 Scores on the MMSE and MoCA among those who reported significant memory problems the last 5 years, broken down by sex, age, and educational groups, n = 309
 Scores on the MMSE and MoCA among those who reported significant memory problems the last 5 years, broken down by sex, age, and educational groups, n = 309

Multivariate Linear Regression Analyses

Lastly, we performed 3 linear regression analyses (Table 5). Using the MMSE score as the dependent variable, we found that a higher score on the MMSE was associated with younger age, higher education, and reporting no significant memory problems the last 5 years. A higher score on MoCA was associated to sex, age, education, and reporting no memory problems the last 5 years. These 2 regression models accounted for 15 and 14% of the explained variance, respectively. The model using the difference of MMSE-MoCA as the dependent variable explained only 5% of the variance.

Table 5.

Three multivariate linear regression analyses with the MMSE, MOCA, and the difference between MMSE-MoCA scores as dependent variables and sex, education, age, and self-report of significant memory problems the last 5 years as independent variables

 Three multivariate linear regression analyses with the MMSE, MOCA, and the difference between MMSE-MoCA scores as dependent variables and sex, education, age, and self-report of significant memory problems the last 5 years as independent variables
 Three multivariate linear regression analyses with the MMSE, MOCA, and the difference between MMSE-MoCA scores as dependent variables and sex, education, age, and self-report of significant memory problems the last 5 years as independent variables

To summarise, according to the adjusted regression analysis, we could confirm our hypothesis that women performed better on MoCA than men, but not on the MMSE. We further found that women had a higher score on the delay recall item on MoCA, and men outperformed women on the items serial sevens on the MMSE and serial subtraction and on the domain visuoconstruction on MoCA. Self-report of memory problems was equally common in men and women, and complaints about significant memory problems the last 5 years were associated with lower scores on the MMSE and MoCA. In addition, the participants had in general a higher score on the MMSE than on MoCA, and the rank correlation between the 2 test scores was poor to moderate. Not surprisingly, we found that education and age influenced on both test scores.

In the adjusted regression analyses (Table 5), we found that female sex was associated with a higher score on MoCA, but not on the MMSE. This finding is in line with a few community studies [23‒27]. However, this sex difference was not found by comparing the total MoCA scores among women and men, as displayed in Table 2. The explanation for this diverse result shown in Tables 2 and 5 is probably because fewer women than men had education of 13 years and above, and very few men had low education (Table 1). This imbalance could influence the total scores on both tests. In the multivariate regression analyses, the difference in education between sexes was controlled (men with education of 13 years and older had the highest score on MoCA). We did not find an association between sex and MMSE score in the adjusted regression analysis (Table 5) nor in the total MMSE score. This suggests that the score on the MMSE is not influenced by sex, possibly due to the recall item of the MMSE (recalling 3 words), and the only spatial item (drawing 2 pentagons) is not sensitive enough to separate between the women and men in a community study. Our finding is, however, in contrast to another and larger community study that reported a higher MMSE score in older women [27]. It should be noted that only 21 scored below 26 and 63 had a maximum score of 30 on the MMSE. This narrow distribution of MMSE scores, having most scores between 26 and 29, could have influenced the results.

The standardized beta in the regression analyses was about the same for the variable sex as for age and half of that of education. Comparable betas are reported by Borland et al. [25] and Thomann et al. [26], 2 community-based MoCA studies. Thomann et al. [26] estimated normative data of MoCA and could show that the score defining −1.5 SD was 1 point higher in women than in men, regardless of age and educational level [26]. We suggest that the estimated higher score in women in the study by Thomann et al. [26] can be related to better verbal memory performance in older women, but neither Borland et al. [25] nor Thomann et al. [26] reported scores on domains/items of the MoCA, so we cannot compare our findings with results from these 2 studies [25, 26]. Direct comparison is further difficult as we have no normative data on MoCA in Norway. Several studies have shown that women, also older women, performed better than men on verbal memory tests [25‒33, 47, 48]. Two larger studies could confirm that these differences were stable over a period of 9–10 years in older adults [30, 33]. The present study confirms these previous findings as we could document significantly higher scores on the item delayed recall on MoCA and WLMT in older women than in older men. However, performance on the delayed recall on the MMSE did not differ between sexes probably because it is less challenging to remember 3 words of the MMSE compared to 5 on the MoCA and ten on the WLMT. We suggest that MoCA is superior to the MMSE in this respect and is one reason why MCI is better detected by MoCA than the MMSE as most MCI cases are of amnestic nature [3]. Another explanation could be that MoCA and the MMSE are differently constructed as MoCA covers more cognitive domains than the MMSE, which could have implications when summarizing scores from sub-items into a total score. It is likely that sub-items testing many different cognitive functions (MoCA) will result in a different total score compared to sub-items testing less different cognitive functions (MMSE). For this reason, MoCA could be more sensitive to cognitive decline.

Not surprisingly, men scored better than women on serial sevens on the MMSE and serial subtraction and visuoconstruction on MoCA, as found in previous studies [27‒33]. However, these differences are judged to be of little significance for the total scores of the 2 tests, since the results had no significant effect in the adjusted regression analysis. The male sex was not associated with neither the MoCA nor MMSE total score. We suggest that the sex differences on MoCA total and item scores should be further examined, since these differences could probably have a significant impact on normative data. Our results do not support that normative data for MMSE should be adjusted for sex differences.

About half of both women and men reported significant memory problems the last 5 years, and no difference was seen between sexes. We suggest that this result is valid. A recent large epidemiological community-based study from Norway reported that 35% of adults older than 70 years had a diagnosis of MCI, and a Swiss community-based study reported a prevalence of 18.5%. In addition, a review reported a prevalence of 23.8% for the diagnosis of subjective cognitive decline [42, 49, 50]. We suggest that raised awareness about memory problems as a possible first sign of dementia and insight into their own decline of memory function by increasing age could explain the high proportion of complainers of both sexes. There is no reason to believe that raised awareness and insight into their own memory decline should be different in older women and men.

The association between scores on self-report of memory problems was not of significance for the general question of “do you experience any memory problems?” but it was for the more specific question “have you experienced significant memory problem the last 5 years?” This finding is in line with our hypothesis. In the unadjusted group comparison analysis (Table 4) and the adjusted regression (Table 5), this specific question was significantly associated with the score on both MMSE and MoCA. The standardized beta for this PROM question in the adjusted regression analyses was almost as high as for education in explaining the scores on the 2 tests, indicating that self-report of memory problem could be used as a first question to older people before performing a cognitive test. Older people answering “yes” to the question have higher probability to have a lower score on tests like MMSE and MoCA. The association between self-report of memory problems the last 5 years and scores on the 2 tests indicates that this question could be used as one of various tools in the assessment of cognition and dementia. A few standardized self-report scales have been developed for clinical use, which are useful, but more time-consuming than a single question [37, 39, 40]. One of these is the Cognitive Functional Impairment (CFI) scale, which contains 14 specific questions of cognitive problems [51]. In a recent study, we found that this scale was valid for detecting older people with MCI, but not dementia [39]. This also indicates that people with normal or subnormal cognitive performance or even MCI have good insight into their own cognitive problems [39].

We could confirm the well-known findings that the educational level and age influence on both test scores, which indicate that the data used in the present study are comparable with data of many other studies in the field [9, 13, 20‒22, 52, 53]. We could also confirm that older people score higher on the MMSE than MoCA probably because MoCA is more challenging, and lastly, we have shown that the rank correlation between the 2 tests is weak and that the scores cannot be used side by side.

Clinical Implications

Having confirmed that age and educational level influence the total scores on both tests, the novel finding in this study is that sex as well influence the MoCA score, but not on the MMSE score. This is only reported in a few previous normative studies of MoCA, and only the study by Thomann et al. [26] calculated separate normal scores for older women and men [23‒26]. The difference in the MoCA score in the Swiss normative study was small, and the influence of sex according to the linear regression analysis (Table 5) on the MoCA score in the present study was as well small. Based on the results of the present study, we cannot recommend any normative scores for older women and men. We suggest that future normative large-scale studies of MoCA should include sex, not only age and education in developing normative scores.

Limitations

Although the older adults were selected randomly, we cannot claim that the sample is representative for the older population of Trondheim, since 195 of 504 persons declined participation. Those who declined had significantly lower score on MoCA, but they did not differ from the participants regarding sex, age, and education. According to the MMSE scores, few participants had severe cognitive impairment meaning that the sample is not representative for this group of older adults. But, as the main aim of the study was to examine differences between sexes with regard to scoring on the MMSE, MoCA, and self-report of memory loss, we assume that the sample is representative in that respect. Another limitation is that several health workers did the testing, and we cannot exclude that test results could have been influenced by the health worker’s various experience. To increase inter-rater reliability, all health workers did the same training before the study. Some of the items of the MMSE and MoCA are similar, but most of them are not identical. Therefore, some learning effects could not be excluded as the 2 tests were administered within 1 week. The sample is relatively small, but as we found comparable results with other larger studies examining the influence of age and education, we assume that our dataset was satisfying to answer our research questions [16, 17]. Another limitation to consider is that we did not further examine if the participants had a diagnosis of dementia, and thus, we cannot exclude that some of the participants have dementia. Nevertheless, most participants had a high score on both tests, making it unlikely that many could have dementia. Only 11 persons scored between 21 and 24 on the MMSE, a finding that also support that only a few could have had dementia.

The main results of the study are that MoCA scores are associated with younger age, higher education, and female sex, whereas sex was not associated with the MMSE score. Older women scored significantly better than men on the item recalling 5 words on MoCA, whereas men scored significantly higher on the domain visuoconstruction on MoCA and the items serial subtraction on MoCA and serial sevens on the MMSE. The question “have you experienced significant memory problems the last 5 years?” was significantly associated with both MoCA and MMSE scores and may be useful to evaluate older people’s cognition.

We thank the Trøndelag Health Study (the HUNT study) for the use of data. The HUNT study is a collaboration between the HUNT Research Centre, (Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, NTNU, Norwegian University of Science and Technology), Trøndelag County Council, Central Norway Regional Health Authority, and the Norwegian Institute of Public Health. We further thank the participants in the current study, the students from the Norwegian University of Science and Technology in Trondheim who participated in the data collection, and Trondheim municipality and the Norwegian National Advisory Unit for Aging and Health for co-operation in the study.

All participants received oral and written information about the study and gave informed written consent. The project was submitted to the Regional Ethics Committee for Medical and Health Research in mid-Norway (reference REK 2018/1812) and was approved. It was further approved according to the General Data Protection Regulation by the Norwegian Centre for Research Data.

None of the authors have conflicts of interest.

The Norwegian National Advisory Unit on Aging and Health funded the study.

K.E. designed the study, analysed and evaluated the data, and wrote the preliminary and final draft of the manuscript and approved it. L.G. took part in design of the study, organised data, and gave input to evaluation of the analysis and the preliminary and final draft of the manuscript, and approved it. T.C.B., P.T., G.G., and L.E. gave input to evaluation of the analysis and the preliminary and final draft of the manuscript, and approved it. G.G. took part in design of the study and gave input to evaluation of the analysis and the preliminary and final draft of the manuscript, and approved it.

1.
World Health Organization
.
Dementia fact sheet
.
2017
. Available from: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs362/en/.
2.
Prince
M
,
Bryce
R
,
Albanese
E
,
Wimo
A
,
Ribeiro
W
,
Ferri
CP
.
The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2013 Jan
;
9
(
9
):
63
e2
.
3.
Rosebud
R
,
Knopman
DS
.
Classification and epidemiology of MCI
.
Clin Geriatr Med
.
2013 Nov
;
29
(
4
):
753
72
.
4.
Gao
Q
,
Gwee
X
,
Feng
L
,
Nyunt
MSZ
,
Feng
L
,
Collinson
SL
, et al
.
Mild cognitive impairment reversion and progression: rates and predictors in community-living older persons in the Singapore longitudinal ageing studies cohort
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra
.
2018 Jun 19
;
8
(
8
):
226
37
.
5.
Engedal
K
,
Barca
ML
,
Høgh
P
,
Bo Andersen
B
,
Winther Dombernowsky
N
,
Naik
M
, et al
.
The power of EEG to predict conversion from mild cognitive impairment and subjective cognitive decline to dementia
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
.
2020
;
49
(
49
):
38
47
.
6.
Folstein
MF
,
Folstein
SE
,
McHugh
PR
.
“Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician
.
J Psychiatr Res
.
1975
;
12
(
3
):
189
98
.
7.
Engedal
K
,
Haugen
P
,
Gilje
K
,
Laake
P
.
Efficacy of short mental tests in the detection of mental impairment in old age
.
Compr Gerontol A
.
1988
;
2
(
2
):
87
93
.
8.
Arevalo-Rodriguez
I
,
Smailagic
N
,
Roqué
I
,
Figuls
M
,
Ciapponi
A
,
Sanchez-Perez
E
, et al
.
Mini-mental state examination (MMSE) for the detection of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias in people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI)
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev
.
2015 Mar 5
;
2015
(
3
):
CD010783
.
9.
Bergeron
D
,
Flynn
K
,
Verret
L
,
Poulin
S
,
Bouchard
RW
,
Bocti
C
, et al
.
Multicenter validation of an MMSE-MoCA conversion table
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2017 May
;
65
(
65
):
1067
72
.
10.
Lonie
JA
,
Tierney
KM
,
Ebmeier
KP
.
Screening for mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review
.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2009 Sep
;
24
(
9
):
902
15
.
11.
Spencer
RJ
,
Wendell
CR
,
Giggey
PP
,
Katzel
LI
,
Lefkowitz
DM
,
Siegel
EL
, et al
.
Psychometric limitations of the mini-mental state examination among nondemented older adults: an evaluation of neurocognitive and magnetic resonance imaging correlates
.
Exp Aging Res
.
2013
;
39
(
39
):
382
97
.
12.
Newman
JC
,
Feldman
R
.
Copyright and open access at the bedside
.
N Engl J Med
.
2011
;
365
(
26
):
2447
9
.
13.
Nasreddine
ZS
,
Phillips
NA
,
Bédirian
V
,
Charbonneau
S
,
Whitehead
V
,
Collin
I
, et al
.
The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment
.
J Am Geriatr Soc
.
2005
;
53
(
53
):
695
9
. Erratum in: J Am Geriatr Soc. 2019;67(9):1991.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x.
14.
Roalf
DR
,
Moberg
PJ
,
Xie
SX
,
Wolk
DA
,
Moelter
ST
,
Arnold
SE
.
Comparative accuracies of two common screening instruments for classification of Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, and healthy aging
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2013
;
9
(
5
):
529
37
.
15.
Trzepacz
PT
,
Hochstetler
H
,
Wang
S
,
Walker
B
,
Saykin
AJ
;
Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
.
Relationship between the montreal cognitive assessment and mini-mental state examination for assessment of mild cognitive impairment in older adults
.
BMC Geriatr
.
2015
;
7
(
15
):
107
.
16.
Pinto
TCC
,
Machado
L
,
Bulgacov
TM
,
Rodrigues-Júnior
AL
,
Costa
MLG
,
Ximenes
RCC
, et al
.
Is the montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) screening superior to the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) in the detection of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) in the elderly
.
Int Psychogeriatr
.
2019
;
31
(
31
):
491
504
.
17.
O’Driscoll
C
,
Shaikh
M
.
Cross-cultural applicability of the montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA): a systematic review
.
J Alzheimers Dis
.
2017
;
58
(
3
):
789
801
.
18.
Petersen
RC
.
Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity
.
J Intern Med
.
2004
;
256
(
3
):
183
94
.
19.
Ciesielska
N
,
Sokołowski
R
,
Mazur
E
,
Podhorecka
M
,
Polak-Szabela
A
,
Kędziora-Kornatowska
K
.
Is the montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) test better suited than the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) detection among people aged over 60? Meta-analysis
.
Psychiatr Pol
.
2016 Oct 31
;
50
(
5
):
1039
52
.
20.
Rambe
AS
,
Fitri
FI
.
Correlation between the montreal cognitive assessment-Indonesian version (Moca-INA) and the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) in elderly
.
Open Access Maced J Med Sci
.
2017 Nov 25
;
5
(
7
):
915
9
.
21.
Falkowski
JA
,
Hynan
LS
,
Krishnan
K
,
Carter
K
,
Lacritz
L
,
Weiner
M
, et al
.
Conversion of MoCA to MMSE scores
.
Alzheimers Dement
.
2015 Mar 29
;
1
(
1
):
125
.
22.
Lawton
M
,
Kasten
M
,
May
MT
,
Mollenhauer
B
,
Schaumburg
M
,
Liepelt-Scarfone
I
, et al
.
Validation of conversion between mini-mental state examination and montreal cognitive assessment
.
Mov Disord
.
2016
;
31
(
4
):
593
6
.
23.
Konstantopoulos
K
,
Vogazianos
P
,
Doskas
T
.
Normative data of the montreal cognitive assessment in the Greek population and Parkinsonian dementia
.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol
.
2016
;
31
(
3
):
246
53
.
24.
Larouche
E
,
Tremblay
MP
,
Potvin
O
,
Laforest
S
,
Bergeron
D
,
Laforce
R
, et al
.
Normative data for the montreal cognitive assessment in middle-aged and elderly Quebec-French people
.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol
.
2016 Nov 22
;
31
(
7
):
819
26
.
25.
Borland
E
,
Nägga
K
,
Nilsson
PM
,
Minthon
L
,
Nilsson
ED
,
Palmqvist
S
.
The montreal cognitive assessment: normative data from a large Swedish population-based cohort
.
J Alzheimers Dis
.
2017
;
59
(
3
):
893
901
.
26.
Thomann
AE
,
Goettel
N
,
Monsch
RJ
,
Berres
M
,
Jahn
T
,
Steiner
LA
, et al
.
The montreal cognitive assessment: normative data from a German-speaking cohort and comparison with international normative samples
.
J Alzheimers Dis
.
2018
;
64
(
2
):
643
55
.
27.
McCarrey
AC
,
Yang
AN
,
Kitner-Triolo
MH
,
Ferrucci
L
,
Resnick
SM
.
Sex differences in cognitive trajectories in clinically normal older adults
.
Psychol Aging
.
2016 Mar
;
31
(
2
):
166
75
.
28.
Bieri
J
,
Bradburn
WM
,
Galinsky
MD
.
Sex differences in perceptual behavior
.
J Pers
.
1958 Mar
;
26
(
26
):
1
12
.
29.
Little
BR
.
Sex differences and comparability of three measures of cognitive complexity
.
Psychol Rep
.
1969
;
24
(
2
):
607
9
.
30.
Finkel
D
,
Reynolds
CA
,
McArdle
JJ
,
Gatz
M
,
Pedersen
NL
.
Latent growth curve analyses of accelerating decline in cognitive abilities in late adulthood
.
Dev Psychol
.
2003 May
;
39
(
39
):
535
50
.
31.
de Frias
CM
,
Nilsson
LG
,
Herlitz
A
.
Sex differences in cognition are stable over a 10-year period in adulthood and old age
.
Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn
.
2006
;
13
(
3–4
):
574
87
.
32.
Ferreira
L
,
Ferreira Santos-Galduróz
R
,
Ferri
CP
,
Fernandes Galduróz
JC
.
Rate of cognitive decline in relation to sex after 60 years-of-age: a systematic review
.
Geriatr Gerontol Int
.
2014 Jan
;
14
(
14
):
23
31
.
33.
Karlsson
P
,
Thorvaldsson
V
,
Skoog
I
,
Gudmundsson
P
,
Johansson
B
.
Birth cohort differences in fluid cognition in old age: comparisons of trends in levels and change trajectories over 30 years in three population-based samples
.
Psychol Aging
.
2015 Mar
;
30
(
1
):
83
94
.
34.
Mitchell
AJ
,
Beaumont
H
,
Ferguson
D
,
Yadegarfar
M
,
Stubbs
B
.
Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: meta-analysis
.
Acta Psychiatr Scand
.
2014 Dec
;
130
(
130
):
439
51
.
35.
Mendonça
MD
,
Alves
L
,
Bugalho
P
.
From subjective cognitive complaints to dementia: who is at risk?: a systematic review
.
Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen
.
2016
;
31
(
2
):
105
14
.
36.
Slavin
MJ
,
Sachdev
PS
,
Kochan
NA
,
Woolf
C
,
Crawford
JD
,
Giskes
K
, et al
.
Predicting cognitive, functional, and diagnostic change over 4 years using baseline subjective cognitive complaints in the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study
.
Am J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2015 Sep
;
23
(
23
):
906
14
.
37.
Sousa
M
,
Pereira
A
,
Costa
R
.
Subjective memory complaint and depressive symptoms among older adults in Portugal
.
Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res
.
2015
;
2015
:
296581
.
38.
Barbosa
RP
,
Mendonça
MD
,
Caetano
AP
,
Lampreia
TM
,
Miguel
R
,
Bugalho
PM
.
Cognitive complaints in Parkinson’s disease patients: from subjective cognitive complaints to dementia and affective disorders
.
J Neural Transm
.
2019
;
126
(
126
):
1329
35
.
39.
Michelet
M
,
Engedal
K
,
Selbæk
G
,
Lund
A
,
Bjørkløf
GH
,
Horndalsveen
PO
, et al
.
The validity of the Norwegian version of the cognitive function instrument
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
.
2018
;
46
(
46
):
217
28
.
40.
Jørgensen
K
,
Nielsen
TR
,
Nielsen
A
,
Waldorff
FB
,
Waldemar
G
.
Validation of the brief assessment of impaired cognition and the brief assessment of impaired cognition questionnaire for identification of mild cognitive impairment in a memory clinic setting
.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2020
;
35
(
35
):
907
15
.
41.
Numbers
K
,
Crawford
JD
,
Kochan
NA
,
Draper
B
,
Sachdev
PS
,
Brodaty
H
.
Participant and informant memory-specific cognitive complaints predict future decline and incident dementia: findings from the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study
.
PLoS One
.
2020 May 12
;
15
(
15
):
e0232961
.
42.
Zullo
L
,
Clark
C
,
Gholam
M
,
Castelao
E
,
von Gunten
A
,
Preisig
M
, et al
.
Factors associated with subjective cognitive decline in dementia-free older adults: a population-based study
.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry
.
2021 Feb 8
.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/gps.5509.
43.
van Wanrooij
LL
,
Richard
E
,
Jongstra
S
,
Moll van Charante
EP
,
van Gool
WA
.
Associations of subjective memory complaints and simple memory task scores with future dementia in the primary care setting
.
Ann Fam Med
.
2019 Sep
;
17
(
17
):
412
8
.
44.
Piauilino
DC
,
Bueno
OF
,
Tufik
S
,
Bittencourt
LR
,
Santos-Silva
R
,
Hachul
H
, et al
.
The prospective and retrospective memory questionnaire: a population-based random sampling study
.
Memory
.
2010 May
;
18
(
4
):
413
26
.
45.
Lubitz
AF
,
Eid
M
,
Niedeggen
M
.
Psychosocial and cognitive performance correlates of subjective cognitive complaints in help-seeking versus non-help-seeking community-dwelling adults
.
J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol
.
2020 Mar
;
33
(
33
):
93
102
.
46.
Morris
JC
,
Mohs
RC
,
Rogers
H
,
Fillenbaum
G
,
Heyman
A
.
Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease
.
Psychopharmacol Bull
.
1988
;
24
(
4
):
641
52
.
47.
Aartsen
MJ
,
Martin
M
,
Zimprich
D
;
Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam
.
Gender differences in level and change in cognitive functioning. Results from the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam
.
Gerontology
.
2004
;
50
(
1
):
35
8
.
48.
Sundermann
EE
,
Biegon
A
,
Rubin
LH
,
Lipton
RB
,
Landau
S
,
Maki
PM
;
Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
.
Does the female advantage in verbal memory contribute to underestimating Alzheimer’s disease pathology in women versus men
.
J Alzheimers Dis
.
2017
;
56
(
3
):
947
57
.
49.
Röhr
S
,
Pabst
A
,
Riedel-Heller
SG
,
Jessen
F
,
Turana
Y
,
Handajani
YS
, et al
.
Estimating prevalence of subjective cognitive decline in and across international cohort studies of aging: a COSMIC study
.
Alzheimers Res Ther
.
2020 Dec 18
;
12
(
1
):
167
.
50.
Gjøra
L
,
Strand
BH
,
Bergh
S
,
Borza
T
,
Brækhus
A
,
Engedal
K
, et al
.
Current and future prevalence estimates of mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and its subtypes in a population-based sample of people 70 years and older in Norway: the HUNT Study
.
J Alzheimers Dis
.
2021
;
79
(
3
):
1213
26
.
51.
Amariglio
RE
,
Donohue
MC
,
Marshall
GA
,
Rentz
DM
,
Salmon
DP
,
Ferris
SH
, et al
.
Tracking early decline in cognitive function in older individuals at risk for Alzheimer disease dementia: the Alzheimer’s disease cooperative study cognitive function instrument
.
JAMA Neurol
.
2015
;
72
(
4
):
446
54
. Erratum in: JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(5):608.
52.
Bartos
A
,
Fayette
D
.
Validation of the Czech montreal cognitive assessment for mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer disease and Czech norms in 1,552 elderly persons
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
.
2018
;
46
(
5–6
):
335
45
.
53.
Pinto
TCC
,
Machado
L
,
Costa
MLG
,
Santos
MSP
,
Bulgacov
TM
,
Rolim
APP
, et al
.
Accuracy and psychometric properties of the Brazilian version of the montreal cognitive assessment as a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease in the initial stages in the elderly
.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord
.
2019
;
47
(
47
):
366
74
.