Abstract

Behavioral ecologists interested in comparative cognition have struggled to design tasks that are both ecologically relevant and experimentally rigorous. In experimental psychology, standardized tests of reversal learning, set-shifting and self-control have long been used to measure aspects of flexible behavior especially with regards to determining the neural mechanisms that enable animals and humans to rapidly and efficiently adapt to different situations. More recently, behavioral ecologists have used the term “behavioral flexibility” more broadly to explain differences in traits such as personality and innovation. Here, we argue that the term behavioral flexibility designates too many non-equivalent traits, and that this can lead to misconceptions about the nature of cognitive abilities.

INTRODUCTION

The terms “behavioral flexibility” and “cognitive flexibility”, used interchangeably, have been employed for decades in the field of experimental psychology to label a form of cognition that enables animals and humans to adapt their behavior to changing environmental contingencies (e.g. Grattan and Eslinger 1989; Ragozzino et al. 1999; Floresco et al. 2009). In experimental psychology, commonly used tests of behavioral flexibility include reversal learning, set-shifting and self-control. In recent years, the term has featured prominently in behavioral ecology, where it is sometimes applied in the same manner as in psychology via tests of reversal learning, but more often in the context of innovation (Sol et al. 2002; Reader and Laland 2003) and problem-solving (Leal and Powell 2012). However, there is increasing evidence that innovative problem-solving and reversal learning are distinct, if not opposite, abilities (e.g. Griffin et al. 2013), while within psychology, different tests of flexibility may well be measuring different traits (Griffin and Guez 2014). If we add to this the many unrelated behaviors that the term has been applied to (e.g. animal personality: van Overveld and Matthysen 2013); defense mechanisms: Stoekl et al. 2015); division of labor: Kwapich and Tschinkel 2016), there is a clear risk that behavioral flexibility as a concept, let alone a term, will completely lose its significance.

Here, we briefly survey the ways in which behavioral flexibility has been assessed and conclude that even if the different assays used in behavioral ecology are conceptually linked, there is little empirical evidence that they are related. We argue that referring to such a large number of potentially non-equivalent and non-related skills with a single term is not useful, often misleading and should be avoided.

BEHAVIORAL FLEXIBILITY IN PSYCHOLOGY

In experimental psychology, the concept of behavioral flexibility emerges from principles of animal learning (for reviews, see Sutherland and Mackintosh 1971; Dickinson 1981) in which an animal makes a decision or choice that is largely influenced by various schedules of reward and future reward outcomes (see Clarke et al. 2004; Chudasama 2011). One commonly used scenario, and one that has been readily adopted in comparative studies, is the reversal learning paradigm, where a dominant response must be overridden due to changes in reward contingencies (see e.g. Jones and Mishkin 1972; Rolls 2000). First, the animal associates one rewarded conditional stimulus (CS+) with a response leading to a reward in the presence of a second, unrewarded stimulus (CS−). This process may continue over several hundred trials, encouraging the formation of a dominant response. Then, unknown to the animal, the stimulus-reward contingency is reversed and the animal must now change its response and use the previously unrewarded stimulus as a cue. There is some response persistence to the initially rewarded stimulus, as would be expected, before the animal works out the new rule. In some cases, however, the response persistence may be exaggerated. This is the case, for example, of rats or monkeys (marmosets and macaques) with orbitofrontal damage, indicating that this structure is involved in reversal performance (Dias et al. 1996; Chudasama and Robbins 2003; Schoenbaum et al. 2003; Izquierdo et al. 2004; Kim and Ragozzino 2005; Jang et al. 2015).

Related to reversal learning is the set-shifting paradigm, where the animal’s attention is solicited by different stimulus dimensions and the animal must alternate between strategies, rules, and attentional sets (Roberts et al. 1988). The cues can be olfactory, tactile, visual and spatial at the same time. The subject needs to first focus on one type of stimulus (for example, a rewarded and an unrewarded stimulus that differ in color) to get the reward as in a classic discrimination learning task, but then it must switch to another stimulus dimension (for example, spatial position or shape or texture) to distinguish the rewarded and unrewarded stimuli in the next phase, ignoring the previously rewarded color dimension (Dias et al. 1996; Oswald et al. 2001; McAlonan and Brown 2003; Brigman et al. 2005; Floresco et al. 2008). Therefore, in set-shifting, the rule is less tangible and the animal must form multi-dimensional attentional sets and shift between them to succeed. Although reversal learning and set shifting are related, they are anatomically dissociable: reversal learning, which involves adapting behavior in accordance with changes in stimulus-reward contingencies, requires an intact orbital prefrontal cortex in mammals, whereas switching attention between perceptual dimensions as in set-shifting relies on the lateral prefrontal cortex in primates or medial prefrontal cortex in rats (Chudasama and Robbins 2006; Nilsson et al. 2015). In sum, set-shifting tasks are designed to measure the subject’s ability to switch strategies, rather than simply learn a new association by reversing a previous one, and this is reflected in the different neural circuits that are involved in the two tasks.

Self-control is considered to be another aspect of behavioral flexibility both by experimental psychologists (see review by Coutlee and Huettel 2012) and behavioral ecologists (e.g. Amici et al. 2008; Boogert et al. 2011). Sometimes also referred to as “cognitive control”, self-control is defined as the extent to which an animal is able to withhold or inhibit its action in the face of a more immediate apparent reward. One way of testing for spatial self-control is the detour-reaching task, commonly used in comparative studies, which requires the animal to inhibit direct attempts to reach a visible food reward in a transparent apparatus, and to instead make a detour around the transparent obstacle to retrieve the food (Diamond 1990; Wallis et al. 2001). Self-control probably involves different brain areas than do reversal learning and set-shifting, at least in humans (see Aron et al. 2014). Although this is still a matter of debate, the right inferior frontal cortex seems to be one of the main areas responsible for self-control (Aron et al. 2014, but see Swick et al. 2008). In short, self-control tasks assess a subject’s ability to inhibit its initial response of using the simplest route or strategy to focus on an indirect, but more efficient approach, an ability that appears to be neurologically distinct from reversal and set-shifting tasks’ proficiencies.

Behavioral ecologists that look to psychology for standardized, well documented assays of animal cognition should thus be aware that, whatever the conceptual similarities between the tasks described above, there are thus clear differences in the traits that they measure (reversal of an association, attention to different cue dimensions, inhibition or impulse control), as well as their neural substrates (e.g. lateral prefrontal, orbitofrontal or right inferior frontal cortex; Wallis et al. 2001; Chudasama et al. 2003; Chudasama and Robbins 2003; Rudebeck et al. 2006; Kühn et al. 2009; Sharp et al. 2010; Aron et al. 2014). This heterogeneity needs to be taken into account when transposing tasks and their interpretation to the more naturalistic situations that behavioral ecologists usually focus on. For example, although detour-reaching and reversal learning are both said to measure flexibility, a study on wild-caught song sparrows found that the two tasks had “opposite” relationships with song repertoire: repertoire size had a positive relationship with detour reaching performance, but a negative one with reversal learning (Boogert et al. 2011).

BEHAVIORAL FLEXIBILITY IN ECOLOGY

Behavioral ecologists sometimes use the same tasks as experimental psychologists (e.g. reversal learning: Bond et al. 2007, detour reaching: Boogert et al. 2011), but they also often think of behavioral flexibility in terms of innovation and problem solving (e.g. Reader and Laland 2002; Sol et al. 2002; Tebbich et al. 2010; Wright et al. 2010; Huebner and Fichtel 2015). Innovation is defined in non-humans as a solution to a novel problem or a novel solution to an old problem (Kummer and Goodall 1985). Extractive foraging problems requiring obstacle removal have become a classic experimental test for innovation, following decades of studies on the origin and spread of the oldest (1921) animal innovation in the literature, the opening of milk bottles by tits (Fisher and Hinde 1949). While there is still a debate about the relative roles of persistence, motor diversity and cognition in the solving process (Griffin et al. 2014; Quinn et al. 2014; Rowe and Healy 2014; Thornton et al. 2014; Morand-Ferron et al. 2015; Cauchoix and Chaine 2016; Diquelou et al. 2016; Pritchard et al. 2016), there is some agreement that obstacle removal problems are a good way of assessing innovative foraging in experimental tests (reviewed in Griffin and Guez 2014). Studies on birds are the most numerous in this field (but see Thornton and Samson 2012; Benson-Amram et al. 2016). Overall, the studies suggest a negative (interindividual: Griffin et al. 2013, interpopulational: Tebbich and Teschke 2014, interspecific: Tebbich et al. 2010; Tebbich et al. 2012) or zero (interindividual: Boogert et al. 2011; Isden et al. 2013; Shaw et al. 2015; Logan 2016, interpopulational: Audet et al. 2016) relation between reversal learning and problem-solving performance. Likewise, problem-solving and detour reaching performance are often uncorrelated in birds (Boogert et al. 2011), but also interspecifically in great apes: orangutans are by far the best of the four great apes in detour-reaching (Vlamings et al. 2010), but the worst in an extractive problem requiring repeated innovation (Manrique et al. 2013).

By their very nature, reversal learning tasks might measure very different processes than the ones measured by innovation and extractive foraging problems. In a reversal task, there is a sudden change in the relationship between two cues and a reward, such that the cue that repeatedly predicted the reward in preceding trials is no longer predictive, and the cue that never predicted reward becomes highly predictive. In serial reversals, previously correct cues repeatedly become suddenly incorrect and previously incorrect cues repeatedly become correct. Persistence leads to errors in such tasks (see review by Nilsson et al. 2015), but persistence is on the contrary a strong facilitator of success in innovative problem-solving (Gajdon et al. 2006; Tebbich et al. 2010; Overington et al. 2011; Benson-Amram and Holekamp 2012; Cole and Quinn 2012; Thornton and Samson 2012; Cauchard et al. 2013; Huebner and Fichtel 2015; Griffin and Guez 2016). Chow et al. (2016) have recently tested the effects of persistence and flexibility on problem-solving efficiency in grey squirrels. They found that “flexibility”, measured as the rate of switching between tactics, was not linked to problem-solving performance, whereas persistence was a strong predictor of success. In addition, the sudden and often repeated changes in cue value in reversal and set-shifting tasks characterize neither extractive foraging problems in captivity or innovation cases in the wild. In fact, the problems that are solved in the wild are often very similar to captive extractive foraging problem-solving tasks but, to our knowledge, do not resemble reversal learning tasks.

While neural substrates of innovative problem-solving are still poorly understood, a few studies on laboratory rodents points to specific areas of the prefrontal cortex. In mice, inactivation of the medial prefrontal cortex causes deficits in an obstacle removal problem (Ben Abdallah et al. 2011) and in set-shifting, but not in reversal learning (Floresco et al. 2008), while in rats, the beta-adrenergic antagonist propranolol negatively impacts obstacle removal but not set-shifting (Hecht et al. 2014). This neurobiological evidence, together with correlational data, suggests that innovative problem-solving and other behavioral flexibility measurements are distinct proficiencies. As most recent studies of innovative problem-solving are done on birds, it should be noted that multiple lines of evidence point to the nidopallium caudolaterale (NCL) as the equivalent of the mammalian prefrontal cortex, providing a clear candidate structure for the control of similar behaviors in the 2 classes (Mogensen and Divac 1993; Rose and Colombo 2005; Rose et al. 2010; Herold et al. 2011; Helduser and Güntürkün 2012; Shanahan et al. 2013; Veit and Nieder 2013; Lengersdorf et al. 2015).

Innovating in the flexible usage of the term

While reversal learning, innovation and problem-solving dominate the literature on flexibility in behavioral ecology, the term has recently come to be used to qualify a surprisingly broad range of behaviors, including variation in neophilia/neophobia in primates (Bergman and Kitchen 2009), exploratory behavior in birds (van Overveld and Matthysen 2013), vigilance level in birds (Couchoux and Cresswell 2012), tool-use in primates (Vale et al. 2016), nest site choice in turtles (Barsante Santos et al. 2016), division of labor among colony members in ants (Bernadou et al. 2015; Kwapich and Tschinkel 2016) or between parents in frogs (Ringler et al. 2015), daily activity allocation in fish (Fingerle et al. 2016), niche allocation in rats, fish and birds (Igulu et al. 2013; Hunt 2016; Loveridge et al. 2016), courtship timing in spiders (Bardier et al. 2015), adjustment of feeder use in birds (Herborn et al. 2014), social organization in primates (Kamilar and Baden 2014; Otani et al. 2014), trial-and-error (discrimination, not reversal) learning in bats (Zhang et al. 2014), diversity of material used for nests in bees (MacIvor and Moore 2013), intensity of chemical defense in wasps (Stoekl et al. 2015), foraging activity across trials in fish (Adriaenssens and Johnsson 2011), degree of soft tissue retraction in foraging snails (Edgell et al. 2009), and the adjustable choice of suction or compression to process food items in elasmobranches (Wilga et al. 2012). This rich diversity of behavioral investigations is useful, as it provides a detailed picture of how behaviors are modified under changing conditions. However, there is little chance that all these cases share a similar etiology. Therefore, referring to this huge diversity of traits under the same blanket term is problematic. Within certain limits, a concept can be multifaceted, but the number and nature of the different contexts in which “behavioral flexibility” has been applied seems excessive. Based on the actual flexible usage of the term, flexibility is attributed to such a wide array of behaviors that are likely to have very different underpinnings that the term is more confusing than useful, especially in cognitive ecology. In this field, experiments implicitly or explicitly aim to understand the cognitive, and eventually neural processes, behind the behaviors tested. Given the mechanistic implications of such experimental studies, the use of a blanket term is especially problematic, as studies within both psychology and behavioral ecology point to heterogeneity in the co-variation and neural underpinnings of the different assays. In large scale comparative analyses of innovation in the wild (e.g. Reader and Laland 2002; Sol et al. 2002), where the focus is not on mechanisms, but on a wide variety of manifestations that go from simple incorporation of new foods in the diet to more sophisticated technical skills (Overington et al. 2009; Ducatez et al. 2015; Navarrete et al. 2016), the problem is less acute, but still preoccupying.

CONCLUSION

What’s flexible in behavioral flexibility? In brief, ways of measuring it. Our review suggests that different assays of behavioral flexibility used in experimental psychology and behavioral ecology are not necessarily equivalent, do not co-vary and are controlled by different neural mechanisms. Some of these assays are even designed to assess opposite abilities, given the contrasting effect of persistence on the performance of each task. Consequently, referring to innovative problem-solving, reversal learning, set-shifting, self-control or other even more distant traits under the same general term of behavioral flexibility can lead to misconceptions about how behavior should be interpreted, especially when comparing cognitive mechanisms across species. Thus, we suggest that the term should be avoided, at least in behavioral ecology. The precise tasks used to assess flexibility in experimental studies, whether they use the standard tasks and model species of psychology or the more naturalistic context of behavioral ecology, should be specified, as we gain more and more detailed knowledge of the mechanisms and the neural events that regulate the different ways in which animals change their behavior in the face of environmental challenges.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank Simon Ducatez and Yogita Chudasama for helpful discussions, as well as three anonymous reviewers for comments.

REFERENCES

Adriaenssens
B
Johnsson
JI
.
2011
.
Shy trout grow faster: exploring links between personality and fitness-related traits in the wild
.
Behav Ecol
.
22
:
135
143
.

Amici
F
Aureli
F
Call
J
.
2008
.
Fission-fusion dynamics, behavioral flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates
.
Curr Biol
.
18
:
1415
1419
.

Aron
AR
Robbins
TW
Poldrack
RA
.
2014
.
Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
18
:
177
185
.

Audet
J-N
Ducatez
S
Lefebvre
L
.
2016
.
The town bird and the country bird: problem solving and immunocompetence vary with urbanization
.
Behav Ecol
.
27
:
637
644
.

Bardier
G
Aisenberg
A
Toscano-Gadea
CA
Costa
FG
.
2015
.
Wooing during day or night is not the Same: an experimental study in the wolf spider Schizocosa malitiosa
.
Ethology
.
121
:
958
965
.

Barsante Santos
AJ
Lima Neto
JX
Gil Vieira
DH
Dutra Neto
L
Bellini
C
Albuquerque
NDS
Corso
G
Soares
BL
.
2016
.
Individual nest site selection in Hawksbill turtles within and between nesting seasons
.
Chelonian Conserv Biol
.
15
:
109
114
.

Ben Abdallah
NM
Fuss
J
Trusel
M
Galsworthy
MJ
Bobsin
K
Colacicco
G
Deacon
RM
Riva
MA
Kellendonk
C
Sprengel
R
et al.  .
2011
.
The puzzle box as a simple and efficient behavioral test for exploring impairments of general cognition and executive functions in mouse models of schizophrenia
.
Exp Neurol
.
227
:
42
52
.

Benson-Amram
S
Dantzer
B
Stricker
G
Swanson
EM
Holekamp
KE
.
2016
.
Brain size predicts problem-solving ability in mammalian carnivores
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci
.
201505913
.

Benson-Amram
S
Holekamp
KE
.
2012
.
Innovative problem solving by wild spotted hyenas
.
Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
.
279
:
4087
4095
.

Bergman
TJ
Kitchen
DM
.
2009
.
Comparing responses to novel objects in wild baboons (Papio ursinus) and geladas (Theropithecus gelada)
.
Anim Cogn
.
12
:
63
73
.

Bernadou
A
Busch
J
Heinze
J
.
2015
.
Diversity in identity: behavioral flexibility, dominance, and age polyethism in a clonal ant
.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol
.
69
:
1365
1375
.

Bond
AB
Kamil
AC
Balda
RP
.
2007
.
Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica)
.
J Comp Psychol
.
121
:
372
379
.

Boogert
NJ
Anderson
RC
Peters
S
Searcy
WA
Nowicki
S
.
2011
.
Song repertoire size in male song sparrows correlates with detour reaching, but not with other cognitive measures
.
Anim Behav
.
81
:
1209
1216
.

Brigman
JL
Bussey
TJ
Saksida
LM
Rothblat
LA
.
2005
.
Discrimination of multidimensional visual stimuli by mice: intra- and extradimensional shifts
.
Behav Neurosci
.
119
:
839
842
.

Cauchard
L
Boogert
NJ
Lefebvre
L
Dubois
F
Doligez
B
.
2013
.
Problem-solving performance is correlated with reproductive success in a wild bird population
.
Anim Behav
.
85
:
19
26
.

Cauchoix
M
Chaine
AS
.
2016
.
How can we study the evolution of animal minds?
Front. Psychol
.
7
. [cited
2016 Aug 5
]. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4791388/.

Chow
PKY
Lea
SEG
Leaver
LA
.
2016
.
How practice makes perfect: the role of persistence, flexibility and learning in problem-solving efficiency
.
Anim Behav
.
112
:
273
283
.

Chudasama
Y
.
2011
.
Animal models of prefrontal-executive function
.
Behav Neurosci
.
125
:
327
343
.

Chudasama
Y
Passetti
F
Rhodes
SE
Lopian
D
Desai
A
Robbins
TW
.
2003
.
Dissociable aspects of performance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task following lesions of the dorsal anterior cingulate, infralimbic and orbitofrontal cortex in the rat: differential effects on selectivity, impulsivity and compulsivity
.
Behav Brain Res
.
146
:
105
119
.

Chudasama
Y
Robbins
TW
.
2003
.
Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to pavlovian autoshaping and discrimination reversal learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal cortex
.
J Neurosci
.
23
:
8771
8780
.

Chudasama
Y
Robbins
TW
.
2006
.
Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans
.
Biol Psychol
.
73
:
19
38
.

Clarke
HF
Dalley
JW
Crofts
HS
Robbins
TW
Roberts
AC
.
2004
.
Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion
.
Science
.
304
:
878
880
.

Cole
EF
Quinn
JL
.
2012
.
Personality and problem-solving performance explain competitive ability in the wild
.
Proc Biol Sci
.
279
:
1168
1175
.

Couchoux
C
Cresswell
W
.
2012
.
Personality constraints versus flexible antipredation behaviors: how important is boldness in risk management of redshanks (Tringa totanus) foraging in a natural system?
Behav. Ecol
.
23
:
290
301
.

Coutlee
CG
Huettel
SA
.
2012
.
The functional neuroanatomy of decision making: prefrontal control of thought and action
.
Brain Res
.
1428
:
3
12
.

Diamond
A
.
1990
.
Developmental time course in human infants and infant monkeys, and the neural bases of, inhibitory control in Reaching
.
Ann NY Acad Sci
.
608
:
637
676
.

Dias
R
Robbins
TW
Roberts
AC
.
1996
.
Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts
.
Nature
.
380
:
69
72
.

Dickinson
A
.
1981
.
Conditioning and associative learning
.
Br Med Bull
.
37
:
165
168
.

Diquelou
MC
Griffin
AS
Sol
D
.
2016
.
The role of motor diversity in foraging innovations: a cross-species comparison in urban birds
.
Behav Ecol
.
27
:
584
591
.

Ducatez
S
Clavel
J
Lefebvre
L
.
2015
.
Ecological generalism and behavioural innovation in birds: technical intelligence or the simple incorporation of new foods?
J Anim Ecol
.
84
:
79
89
.

Edgell
TC
Lynch
BR
Trussell
GC
Palmer
AR
.
2009
.
Experimental evidence for the rapid evolution of behavioral canalization in natural populations
.
Am Nat
.
174
:
434
440
.

Fingerle
A
Larranaga
N
Steingrímsson
.
2016
.
Density-dependent diel activity in stream-dwelling Arctic charr Salvelinus alpinus
.
Ecol Evol
.
6
:
3965
3976
.

Fisher
J
Hinde
R
.
1949
.
The opening of milk bottles by birds
.
British Birds
42
:
347
357
.

Floresco
SB
Block
AE
Tse
MT
.
2008
.
Inactivation of the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat impairs strategy set-shifting, but not reversal learning, using a novel, automated procedure
.
Behav Brain Res
.
190
:
85
96
.

Floresco
SB
Zhang
Y
Enomoto
T
.
2009
.
Neural circuits subserving behavioral flexibility and their relevance to schizophrenia
.
Behav Brain Res
.
204
:
396
409
.

Gajdon
GK
Fijn
N
Huber
L
.
2006
.
Limited spread of innovation in a wild parrot, the kea (Nestor notabilis)
.
Anim Cogn
.
9
:
173
181
.

Grattan
LM
Eslinger
PJ
.
1989
.
Higher Cognition and Social Behavior: Changes in Cognitive Flexibility and Empathy after Cerebral Lesions
.
Neuropsychology
3
:
175
185
.

Griffin
AS
Diquelou
M
Perea
M
.
2014
.
Innovative problem solving in birds: a key role of motor diversity
.
Anim Behav
.
92
:
221
227
.

Griffin
AS
Guez
D
.
2014
.
Innovation and problem solving: a review of common mechanisms
.
Behav Processes
.
109
(Pt B):
121
134
.

Griffin
AS
Guez
D
.
2016
.
Bridging the gap between cross-taxon and within-species analyses of behavioral innovations in birds: making sense of discrepant cognition – innovation relationships and the role of motor diversity
.
Advances in the study of behavior
.
Vol. 48
.
Academic Press
. p.
1
40
.

Griffin
AS
Guez
D
Lermite
F
Patience
M
.
2013
.
Tracking changing environments: innovators are fast, but not flexible learners
.
PLoS One
.
8
:
e84907
.

Hecht
PM
Will
MJ
Schachtman
TR
Welby
LM
Beversdorf
DQ
.
2014
.
Beta-adrenergic antagonist effects on a novel cognitive flexibility task in rodents
.
Behav Brain Res
.
260
:
148
154
.

Helduser
S
Güntürkün
O
.
2012
.
Neural substrates for serial reaction time tasks in pigeons
.
Behav Brain Res
.
230
:
132
143
.

Herborn
KA
Heidinger
BJ
Alexander
L
Arnold
KE
.
2014
.
Personality predicts behavioral flexibility in a fluctuating, natural environment
.
Behav Ecol
.
25
:
1374
1379
.

Herold
C
Palomero-Gallagher
N
Hellmann
B
Kröner
S
Theiss
C
Güntürkün
O
Zilles
K
.
2011
.
The receptor architecture of the pigeons’ nidopallium caudolaterale: an avian analogue to the mammalian prefrontal cortex
.
Brain Struct Funct
.
216
:
239
254
.

Huebner
F
Fichtel
C
.
2015
.
Innovation and behavioral flexibility in wild redfronted lemurs (Eulemur rufifrons)
.
Anim Cogn
.
18
:
777
787
.

Hunt
VM
.
2016
.
Reproductive success and habitat selection in black-crowned night-herons (Nycticorax nycticorax) in a city park
.
Am Midl Nat
.
175
:
168
182
.

Igulu
MM
Nagelkerken
I
van der Beek
M
Schippers
M
van Eck
R
Mgaya
YD
.
2013
.
Orientation from open water to settlement habitats by coral reef fish: behavioral flexibility in the use of multiple reliable cues
.
Mar Ecol Prog Ser
.
493
:
243
257
.

Isden
J
Panayi
C
Dingle
C
Madden
J
.
2013
.
Performance in cognitive and problem-solving tasks in male spotted bowerbirds does not correlate with mating success
.
Anim Behav
.
86
:
829
838
.

Izquierdo
A
Suda
RK
Murray
EA
.
2004
.
Bilateral orbital prefrontal cortex lesions in rhesus monkeys disrupt choices guided by both reward value and reward contingency
.
J Neurosci
.
24
:
7540
7548
.

Jang
AI
Costa
VD
Rudebeck
PH
Chudasama
Y
Murray
EA
Averbeck
BB
.
2015
.
the role of frontal cortical and medial-temporal lobe brain areas in learning a Bayesian prior belief on reversals
.
J Neurosci
.
35
:
11751
11760
.

Jones
B
Mishkin
M
.
1972
.
Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus–reinforcement associations
.
Exp Neurol
.
36
:
362
377
.

Kamilar
JM
Baden
AL
.
2014
.
What drives flexibility in primate social organization?
Behav Ecol Sociobiol
.
68
:
1677
1692
.

Kim
J
Ragozzino
ME
.
2005
.
The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in learning under changing task contingencies
.
Neurobiol Learn Mem
.
83
:
125
133
.

Kühn
S
Haggard
P
Brass
M
.
2009
.
Intentional inhibition: how the “veto-area” exerts control
.
Hum Brain Mapp
.
30
:
2834
2843
.

Kummer
H
Goodall
J
.
1985
.
Conditions of Innovative Behaviour in Primates
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
.
308
:
203
214
.

Kwapich
CL
Tschinkel
WR
.
2016
.
Limited flexibility and unusual longevity shape forager allocation in the Florida harvester ant (Pogonomyrmex badius)
.
Behav Ecol Sociobiol
.
70
:
1045
1045
.

Leal
M
Powell
BJ
.
2012
.
Behavioural flexibility and problem-solving in a tropical lizard
.
Biol Lett
.
8
:
28
30
.

Lengersdorf
D
Marks
D
Uengoer
M
Stüttgen
MC
Güntürkün
O
.
2015
.
Blocking NMDA-receptors in the pigeon’s ‘prefrontal’ caudal nidopallium impairs appetitive extinction learning in a sign-tracking paradigm
.
Front Behav Neurosci
.
85
.

Logan
CJ
.
2016
.
Behavioral flexibility and problem solving in an invasive bird
.
PeerJ
.
4
:
e1975
.

Loveridge
R
Wearn
OR
Vieira
M
Bernard
H
Ewers
RM
.
2016
.
Movement behavior of native and invasive small mammals shows logging may facilitate invasion in a tropical rain forest
.
Biotropica
.
48
:
373
380
.

MacIvor
JS
Moore
AE
.
2013
.
Bees collect polyurethane and polyethylene plastics as novel nest materials
.
Ecosphere
.
4
:
155
.

Manrique
HM
Völter
CJ
Call
J
.
2013
.
Repeated innovation in great apes
.
Anim Behav
.
85
:
195
202
.

McAlonan
K
Brown
VJ
.
2003
.
Orbital prefrontal cortex mediates reversal learning and not attentional set shifting in the rat
.
Behav Brain Res
.
146
:
97
103
.

Mogensen
J
Divac
I
.
1993
.
Behavioural effects of ablation of the pigeon-equivalent of the mammalian prefrontal cortex
.
Behav Brain Res
.
55
:
101
107
.

Morand-Ferron
J
Cole
EF
Quinn
JL
.
2015
.
Studying the evolutionary ecology of cognition in the wild: a review of practical and conceptual challenges
.
Biol Rev
.

Navarrete
AF
Reader
SM
Street
SE
Whalen
A
Laland
KN
.
2016
.
The coevolution of innovation and technical intelligence in primates
.
Phil Trans R Soc B
.
371
:
20150186
.

Nilsson
SR
Alsiö
J
Somerville
EM
Clifton
PG
.
2015
.
The rat’s not for turning: Dissociating the psychological components of cognitive inflexibility
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
56
:
1
14
.

Oswald
CJ
Yee
BK
Rawlins
JN
Bannerman
DB
Good
M
Honey
RC
.
2001
.
Involvement of the entorhinal cortex in a process of attentional modulation: evidence from a novel variant of an IDS/EDS procedure
.
Behav Neurosci
.
115
:
841
849
.

Otani
Y
Sawada
A
Hanya
G
.
2014
.
Short-term separation from groups by male Japanese macaques: costs and benefits in feeding behavior and social interaction
.
Am J Primatol
.
76
:
374
384
.

Overington
SE
Cauchard
L
Côté
KA
Lefebvre
L
.
2011
.
Innovative foraging behaviour in birds: what characterizes an innovator?
Behav Processes
.
87
:
274
285
.

Overington
SE
Morand-Ferron
J
Boogert
NJ
Lefebvre
L
.
2009
.
Technical innovations drive the relationship between innovativeness and residual brain size in birds
.
Anim Behav
.
78
:
1001
1010
.

van Overveld
T
Matthysen
E
.
2013
.
Personality and information gathering in free-ranging great tits
.
PLoS One
.
8
:
e54199
.

Pritchard
DJ
Hurly
TA
Tello-Ramos
MC
Healy
SD
.
2016
.
Why study cognition in the wild (and how to test it)?
J Exp Anal Behav
.
105
:
41
55
.

Quinn
JL
Cole
EF
Morand-Ferron
J
.
2014
.
Studying microevolutionary processes in cognitive traits: a comment on Rowe and Healy
.
Behav Ecol
.
aru141
.

Ragozzino
ME
Detrick
S
Kesner
RP
.
1999
.
Involvement of the prelimbic-infralimbic areas of the rodent prefrontal cortex in behavioral flexibility for place and response learning
.
J Neurosci
.
19
:
4585
4594
.

Reader
SM
Laland
KN
.
2002
.
Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
99
:
4436
4441
.

Reader
SM
Laland
KN
, editors.
2003
.
Animal Innovation
. 1st ed.
Oxford (NY)
:
Oxford University Press
.

Ringler
E
Pašukonis
A
Fitch
WT
Huber
L
Hödl
W
Ringler
M
.
2015
.
Flexible compensation of uniparental care: female poison frogs take over when males disappear
.
Behav Ecol
.
26
:
1219
1225
.

Roberts
AC
Robbins
TW
Everitt
BJ
.
1988
.
The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates
.
Q J Exp Psychol B
.
40
:
321
341
.

Rolls
ET
.
2000
.
The orbitofrontal cortex and reward
.
Cereb Cortex
.
10
:
284
294
.

Rose
J
Colombo
M
.
2005
.
Neural correlates of executive control in the avian brain
.
PLoS Biol
.
3
:
e190
.

Rose
J
Schiffer
AM
Dittrich
L
Güntürkün
O
.
2010
.
The role of dopamine in maintenance and distractability of attention in the “prefrontal cortex” of pigeons
.
Neuroscience
.
167
:
232
237
.

Rowe
C
Healy
SD
.
2014
.
Measuring variation in cognition
.
Behav Ecol
. [cited
2014 Aug 22
]. Available from: http://www.beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/doi/10.1093/beheco/aru090.

Rudebeck
PH
Walton
ME
Smyth
AN
Bannerman
DM
Rushworth
MF
.
2006
.
Separate neural pathways process different decision costs
.
Nat Neurosci
.
9
:
1161
1168
.

Schoenbaum
G
Setlow
B
Saddoris
MP
Gallagher
M
.
2003
.
Encoding predicted outcome and acquired value in orbitofrontal cortex during cue sampling depends upon input from basolateral amygdala
.
Neuron
.
39
:
855
867
.

Shanahan
M
Bingman
VP
Shimizu
T
Wild
M
Güntürkün
O
.
2013
.
Large-scale network organization in the avian forebrain: a connectivity matrix and theoretical analysis. Front
.
Comput. Neurosci
.
7
. [cited
2014 Aug 22
]. Available from: http://www.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fncom.2013.00089/full.

Sharp
DJ
Bonnelle
V
De Boissezon
X
Beckmann
CF
James
SG
Patel
MC
Mehta
MA
.
2010
.
Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
107
:
6106
6111
.

Shaw
RC
Boogert
NJ
Clayton
NS
Burns
KC
.
2015
.
Wild psychometrics: evidence for ‘general’ cognitive performance in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes
.
Anim Behav
.
109
:
101
111
.

Sol
D
Timmermans
S
Lefebvre
L
.
2002
.
Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds
.
Anim Behav
.
63
:
495
502
.

Stoekl
J
Machacek
Z
Ruther
J
.
2015
.
Behavioural flexibility of the chemical defence in the parasitoid wasp Leptopilina heterotoma
.
Sci Nat
.
102
:
67
.

Sutherland
NS
Mackintosh
NJ
.
1971
.
Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning
.
Academic Press
.

Swick
D
Ashley
V
Turken
AU
.
2008
.
Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition
.
BMC Neurosci
.
9
:
102
.

Tebbich
S
Stankewitz
S
Teschke
I
.
2012
.
The relationship between foraging, learning abilities and neophobia in two species of Darwin’s finches: The relationship between foraging and learning abilities in Darwin’s finches
.
Ethology
.
118
:
135
146
.

Tebbich
S
Sterelny
K
Teschke
I
.
2010
.
The tale of the finch: adaptive radiation and behavioural flexibility
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
.
365
:
1099
1109
.

Tebbich
S
Teschke
I
.
2014
.
Coping with uncertainty: woodpecker finches (Cactospiza pallida) from an unpredictable habitat are more flexible than birds from a stable habitat
.
PLoS One
.
9
:
e91718
.

Thornton
A
Isden
J
Madden
JR
.
2014
.
Toward wild psychometrics: linking individual cognitive differences to fitness
.
Behav Ecol
.
25
(
6
):
1299
1301
.

Thornton
A
Samson
J
.
2012
.
Innovative problem solving in wild meerkats
.
Anim Behav
.
83
:
1459
1468
.

Vale
GL
Flynn
EG
Pender
L
Price
E
Whiten
A
Lambeth
SP
Schapiro
SJ
Kendal
RL
.
2016
.
Robust retention and transfer of tool construction techniques in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
.
J Comp Psychol
.
130
:
24
35
.

Veit
L
Nieder
A
.
2013
.
Abstract rule neurons in the endbrain support intelligent behaviour in corvid songbirds
.
Nat Commun
.
4
. [cited
2014 Aug 22
]. Available from: http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/ncomms3878.

Vlamings
PH
Hare
B
Call
J
.
2010
.
Reaching around barriers: the performance of the great apes and 3-5-year-old children
.
Anim Cogn
.
13
:
273
285
.

Wallis
JD
Dias
R
Robbins
TW
Roberts
AC
.
2001
.
Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and lateral prefrontal cortex of the marmoset to performance on a detour reaching task
.
Eur J Neurosci
.
13
:
1797
1808
.

Wilga
CAD
Stoehr
AA
Duquette
DC
Allen
RM
.
2012
.
Functional ecology of feeding in elasmobranchs
.
Environ Biol Fishes
.
95
:
155
167
.

Wright
TF
Eberhard
JR
Hobson
EA
Avery
ML
Russello
MA
.
2010
.
Behavioral flexibility and species invasions: the adaptive flexibility hypothesis
.
Ethol Ecol Evol
.
22
:
393
404
.

Zhang
W
Zhu
G
Tan
L
Yang
J
Chen
Y
Liu
Q
Shen
Q
Chen
J
Zhang
L
.
2014
.
Role of olfaction in the foraging behavior and trial-and-error learning in short-nosed fruit bat, Cynopterus sphinx
.
Behav Processes
.
103
:
23
27
.

Author notes

Address correspondence to J.-N. Audet. E-mail: Jean-nicolas.audet@mail.mcgill.ca

Forum editor: Leigh Simmons