References
Bates, E. & MacWhinney, B
1982Functionalist approaches to grammar. In Language Acquisition - The State of the Art, E. Wanner & L.R. Gleitman (eds), 173–218. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Bassano, D
2000Early development of nouns and verbs in French: Exploring the interface between lexicon and grammar. Journal of Child Language 27(3): 521–559. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernal, S., Lidz, J., Millotte, S. & Christophe, A
2007Syntax constrains the acquisition of verb meaning. Language Learning and Development 3(4): 325–341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bird, H., Lambon, R.M.A., Patterson, K. & Hodges, J
2000The rise and fall of frequency and imageability: Noun and verb production in semantic dementia. Brain and Language 73(1): 17–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Black, M. & Chiat, S
2003Noun-verb dissociations: A multi-faceted phenomenon. Journal of Neurolinguistics 16(2–3): 231–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L
1970Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
1991Language Development from Two to Three. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P
1984Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgement of Taste. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Braine, M.D.S
1976Children’s first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 41(1): 98–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Breedin, S., Saffran, E.M. & Coslett, H.B
1994Reversal of the concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 11(6): 617–660. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R.W
1973A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Budwig, N
1995A Developmental-functionalist Approach to Child Language. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Casasola, M., Bhagwat, J. & Ferguson, K
2006Precursors to verb learning: Infant’s understanding of motion events. In Action Meets Word: How Children Learn Verbs, K. Hirsh-Pasek & R.M. Golinkoff (eds), 160–190. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chapman, R.S
2000Children’s language learning: An interactionist perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 41(1): 33–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V
1982The young word-maker: A case study of innovation in the child’s lexicon. In Language Acquisition: The State of the Art, E. Wanner & L.R. Gleitman (eds), 390–425. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
1987The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In Mechanisms of Language Acquisition, B. MacWhinney, (ed.), 1–33. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Croft, W
2001Radical Construction Grammar: Syntactic Theory in Typological Perspective. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, W. & Cruse, D.A
2004Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Desagulier, G
In press. Visualizing distances in a set of near synonyms: Rather, quite, fairly, and pretty. In Polysemy and Synonymy: Corpus Methods and Applications in Cognitive Linguistics [Human Cognitive Processing 43], D. Glynn & J. Robinson (eds) Amsterdam John Benjamins DOI logo
Divjak, D. & Gries, S.T
2006Ways of trying in Russian: Clustering behavioral profiles. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 2(1): 23–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, N. & Levinson, S.C
2009The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32(05): 429–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D
1982Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In Language Development, S.A. Kuczak (ed.), 22: 67–88. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gelman, S.A. & Gottfried, G.M
1996Children’s causal explanations of animate and inanimate motion. Child Development 67:1970–1987. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D
2006Why verbs are hard to learn. In Action Meets Word: How Children Learn Verbs, K. Hirsch-Pasek & R. Golinkoff (eds), 544–564. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L.R
1990The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition 1: 3–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A.E
1995Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
2003Constructions: A new theorical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(5): 219–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2006Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R.M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Mervis, C.B., Frawley, W. & Parillo, M
1995Lexical principles can be extended to the acquisition of verbs. In Beyond Names for Things: Young Children's Acquisition of Verbs, M. Tomasello & W. Merriman (eds), 185–222. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Hockett, C.F
1960The origin of speech. Scientific American 203: 88–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Husson, F., Lê, S. & Pagès, J
2011Exploratory Multivariate Analysis by Example Using R. Boca Raton FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R
1987The Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Vol. I: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G
1987Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lyons, J
1968Introduction to Theoretical Linguistics. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Macnamara, J
1984Names for Things: A Study in Human Learning. Cambridge MA: Bradford.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B
1999The Emergence of Language. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M.P
1976The Use of Definite and Indefinite Reference in Young Children: An Experimental Study of Semantic Acquisition. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A. & Parisse, C
2012The Paris Corpus. Journal of French Language Studies 22: 7–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K
1973Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38(1–2, Serial No. 149). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ninio, A
2006Kernel vocabulary and Zipf’s law in maternal input to syntactic development. BUCLD 30: Proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia & C. Zaller (eds), 423–431. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B
1984Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories. In Culture Theory: Mind, Self, and Emotion, R. Shweder & R. LeVine (eds), 276–320. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Olguin, R. & Tomasello, M
1993Twenty-five-month-old children do not have a grammatical category of verb. Cognitive Development 8(3): 245–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Payne, T.E
1997Describing Morphosyntax: A Guide for Field Linguists. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Pinker, S
1984Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
1994How could a child use verb syntax to learn verb semantics? Lingua 92: 377–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rakison, D.H
2005Developing knowledge of motion properties in infancy. Cognition 96: 183–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rossi, C. & Parisse, C
2012Categories in the making: Assessing the role of semantics in the acquisition of noun and verb categories. Journal of French Language Studies 22: 37–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slater, A
1989Visual memory and perception in early infancy. In Infant Development, A. Slater & G. Bremner (eds), 43–71. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Slobin, D.I
2001Form-function relations: How do children find out what they are? In Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, M. Bowerman & S. Levinson (eds), 406–449. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M
2000Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74: 209–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2003Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Boston MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
2004What kind of evidence could refute the UG hypothesis? Studies in Language 28: 642–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Olguin, R
1993Twenty-three-month-old children have a grammatical category of noun. Cognitive Development 8(4): 451–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E
2003The emergence of noun and verb categories in the acquisition of French. Psychology of Language and Communication 7(1): 23–36.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E., Parisse, C. & Delacour, A
2010The early comprehension of noun-verb distinction in French. An experimental method. Paper presened at the International Conference on Infant Studies , Baltimore, USA, March.
Veneziano, E. & Parisse, C.
in press. Retrieving meaning from noun and verb grammatical contexts: Interindividual variation among 2- to 4-year-old French-speaking children In Sources of Variation in First Language Acquisition: Languages, Contexts, and Learners, M. Hickmann, E. Veneziano & H. Jisa (eds) Amsterdam John Benjamins
Veneziano, E. & Sinclair, H
2000The changing status of “filler syllables” on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language 27(3): 461–500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E., Sinclair, H. & Berthoud-Papandropoulou, I
1990From one word to two words: repetition patterns on the way to structured speech. Journal of Child Language 17: 633–650. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D.P., Lewis, W. & Garrett, M.F
2004Representing the meanings of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology 48(4): 422–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zingeser, L.B. & Berndt, R.S
1990Retrieval of nouns and verbs in agrammatism and anomia. Brain and Language 39(1): 14–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar