Introduction

Sans social obligations, sleep–wake timings are determined by the interaction between an individual’s circadian clock and the timing of natural light exposure1,2. However, in most industrialized societies, organized work with its complex web of values and activities have insidiously and incrementally transformed our natural sleep patterns. Modern-day workers are likely to go to bed later, sleep less regularly and get exposed to greater stress than their predecessors3,4,5.

Recent lockdowns imposed around the world to contain the spread of COVID-19 resulted in massive disruption of daily routines surrounding work and face-to-face social interactions6,7,8. In theory, closure of workplaces and schools as well as sports, entertainment and social hubs should free up much time for neglected uses of time, including sleep. Work-from-home arrangements can afford individuals more latitude to adopt their preferred sleep–wake timings9. In turn, this could reduce variability in sleep timing that, when high, has been linked to poor sleep quality, impaired health and well-being as well as metabolic abnormalities10,11,12,13,14. On the other hand, being in a state of lockdown might cause anxiety and depression for many, reducing the amount of sleep obtained15. In addition, reduced morning light exposure and increased evening light exposure from screens could lead to progressive delays in chronotype16 and a loss of circadian rhythm entrainment17. Perhaps as a consequence of the aforesaid factors that push sleep in opposite directions, reports of sleep behavior during the lockdown suggest only modest gains in sleep time15,18,19,20,21,22,23 and are mixed depending on region and demographic surveyed as well as study design (e.g. cross-sectional vs. longitudinal) and instrumentation (e.g. single-item/multi-item questionnaire vs. sleep tracking device) used.

To date, studies documenting COVID-19 pandemic changes in sleep behavior and their potential health implications have largely been confined to observations within a country or a limited number of countries6,7,8,18,20,22,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32, with only a handful surveying from a wider geographic region21,33,34,35. As prevailing social norms and the severity of lockdowns differ across countries, the generalizability of such reports is unclear. Additionally, most studies have utilized questionnaires in which baseline data was inferred from participant recollection of past sleep habits and few studies have followed the temporal evolution of sleep through both lockdown and the lifting of restrictions. Finally, many of the existing reports also naturally take advantage of samples that the research group has ready access to and even ‘international surveys’ tend to be biased to persons who are educated and concerned enough to take part. This is a key reason why multi-country, objective, passively collected, longitudinal data is important to give a ‘bird’s eye view’ of what is occurring in the wider population.

To fill these gaps in our understanding of sleep behavior throughout this challenging period, we analyzed nocturnal sleep behavior and resting heart rate of ~ 113,000 users of the Oura ring sleep tracker from Jan to Jul 2020, using an equivalent period in 2019 as a baseline control. The Oura ring is a novel multisensor device that uses motion, heart-rate and temperature sensors to detect sleep/wake states (https://ouraring.com/). It has been validated in various population groups with sleep–wake detection performance comparable to that of research grade actigraphy and polysomnography36,37,38,39,40, and HR measurement comparable to that of ECG (R2 = 0.996 with a mean bias of − 0.63 bpm)41. This tracker allowed for large streams of longitudinal data to be collected during this period with minimal user effort, enabling analysis from a pre-pandemic baseline period, through lockdowns and lifting of restrictions in the 20 countries, spanning regions in North America, Europe, Asia and Oceania.

We sought to (a) assess how two critical sleep parameters were influenced by pandemic-related lockdown and subsequent partial lifting of restrictions, (b) determine the extent to which these sleep parameters were influenced by the severity of lockdown measures and (c) relate how alterations in sleep behavior affected resting heart rate, an indicator of cardiovascular health. To optimize clarity and utility of sleep data from a public health perspective, we focused on average midsleep time and sleep variability. The former provides a compact measure that is influenced by later bed and wake times linked to academic and health outcomes42,43, while the latter is an indicator of irregular sleep/wake patterns—also associated with negative health and cognitive outcomes independently of sleep duration10,12,44,45. Lockdown stringency was assessed using scores extracted from the Oxford Government Response Tracker46, which considers factors such as school and workplace closures, cancellations of public events and gatherings and closures of public transport.

Results

Regional and global trends in sleep and resting heart rate

Data was obtained from Oura users who had valid sleep and HR records across two equivalent periods in 2019 and 2020. Only countries with a minimum of 500 users were included in subsequent analyses. Sample size, age, BMI and proportion of male users by country are detailed in Table S1A while stringency indices by country alongside midsleep times, midsleep variability and resting heart rate values for each month are detailed in Table S1B–D. Users were predominantly male in all countries, with mean age between 35 and 45y. Lockdowns were strictest between the months of Apr and May in most countries surveyed, with the exception of Australia—which peaked in July.

Figure 1 shows plots of descriptives across 2019 and 2020 for the 3 indicator variables inspected: (a) midsleep time, (b) midsleep variability and (c) resting heart rate (interactive plots are provided in Supplementary Materials), while plots for sleep duration are shown in Figure S1A. Qualitatively, we observed that midsleep time as well as resting heart rate showed weekday-weekend differences whereby weekends (periodic 7-day peaks) presented later midsleep time as well as higher resting heart rate compared to weekdays (periodic 7-day troughs) (Fig. 1). Inspection of trends also suggest that when lockdowns were most severe across 20 countries (Table S1B–D), midsleep time shifted later, midsleep variability decreased and resting heart rate decreased. Later weekday midsleep times appeared to be contributed by delayed bedtimes and commensurately more delayed wake times (elevated troughs of orange relative to blue time courses in Figure S1B–C), decreasing the weekday-weekend difference in midsleep times and in turn reducing midsleep variability.

Figure 1
figure 1

Sleep patterns and resting heart rate measures by country. (a) midsleep time, (b) midsleep variability and (c) resting heart rate from Jan to Jul 2020 (orange curves), compared to Jan–Dec 2019 (purple curves). Dates in 2019 were shifted in order to ensure a matching by day of the week. Daylight savings time (social clocks shifted later by 1 h) began in March in select countries of the Northern Hemisphere, and ended in April in Australia and New Zealand (social clocks shifted earlier by 1 h), explaining sudden shifts in midsleep time and variability on these dates.

Lockdown stringency modulates changes in sleep and resting heart rate

Mirroring the heterogeneity in the severity of lockdowns, the shifts in midsleep time and sleep regularity differed widely across countries as evidenced by the high heterogeneity I2 statistic (> 75%). Forest plots (Fig. 2) depict country-level, month-by-month trends in the lockdown related shifts in midsleep time, midsleep variability and resting heart rate obtained by comparing monthly averages of data from comparable days in 2020 and 2019. Pooled effects derived from a random effects meta-analyses representing global changes by month are shown below each forest plot. In general, the largest shifts in midsleep time (+ 0.09 to + 0.58 h), midsleep variability (− 0.12 to − 0.26 h) and resting heart rate (− 0.35 to − 2.08 bpm) occurred in April and May when most countries imposed their strictest lockdown measures. Conversely the gradually lifting of restrictions from around June was accompanied by a return to patterns recorded in the previous year.

Figure 2
figure 2

Forest plots of the average change in midsleep time, midsleep variability and resting heart rate by month and country. The size of the colored squares is proportional to the sample size of each country while whiskers indicate the mean and 95% confidence interval of the estimated difference between 2020 and 2019 by month (Jan–Jul). The overall pooled effect across countries for each month is represented by the colored diamonds below each plot.

Multilevel modelling revealed that the extent to which sleep timings and resting heart rate shifted during the pandemic’s most pressing early months, could be largely explained by the severity of the stringency index (Fig. 3 and Tables S2–S4). Marginal R2 values increased from 0.02, 0.18 and 0.28 in baseline models to 0.56, 0.60 and 0.57 for midsleep time, midsleep variability and resting heart rate respectively when stringency index was included as a predictor in the model. For each unit increase in stringency index, midsleep time was delayed by 0.96 ± 0.15 min (mean ± CI; Model 2, Table S2) midsleep variability decreased by 0.46 ± 0.08 min (mean ± CI; Model 2, Table S3) and resting heart rate decreased by 0.06 ± 0.01 bpm (mean ± CI; Model 2, Table S4).

Figure 3
figure 3

Prediction of changes in sleep patterns and resting heart rate by lockdown stringency. Plots for fitted models (green curves) across 20 countries estimating changes in (a) midsleep time, (b) midsleep variability and (c) resting heart rate using lockdown stringency as a predictor. Observed mean differences between 2020 and 2019 (blue dots), lockdown stringency (black line) and predictions without (WO) lockdown stringency in the model (red dashed line) are also shown for comparison.

Changes in sleep patterns predict changes in resting heart rate

Finally, in models predicting 2020–2019 changes in resting heart rate associated with changes in midsleep time, midsleep variability and absolute sleep duration, we found that while each of the three variables significantly predicted resting heart rate in separate models, the model fit was significantly improved when all three predictors were included in the model (marginal R2 = 0.75, Table 1) compared to all previous models (all Ps < 0.001). In this model (Model 5), midsleep variability was also shown to be the strongest predictor of resting heart rate, wherein an hour increase in the standard deviation of midsleep variability predicted a 5.12 ± 1.07 increase in bpm, while an hour increase in midsleep time only predicted a 1.25 ± 0.55 decrease in bpm. Sleep duration in 2020 also no longer significantly predicted resting heart rate changes in Model 5 (Table 1). The final model fit using Model 5 was fitted to each country (Fig. 4). It was evident that the fitted curves mirrored the trends of changes in midsleep variability observed in each country.

Table 1 Model fit statistics predicting changes in resting heart rate.
Figure 4
figure 4

Prediction of changes in resting heart rate by changes in sleep patterns. Plots showing final fitted models (green curves) across 20 countries predicting changes in resting heart rate (RHR) (2020–2019) using changes in midsleep time, changes in midsleep variability, and sleep duration in 2020 as time-varying predictors in the same model. Observed mean differences between 2020 and 2019 (blue dots) and predictions without sleep variability in the model (blue dashed line) are also shown for comparison. Sleep variability was found to be the strongest predictor of resting heart rate and is shown here in orange solid line for visualization purposes.

Discussion

In all 20 countries across 4 continents, objective data obtained over successive years showed that pandemic-related lockdowns delayed sleep midpoint, reduced sleep variability and reduced resting heart rate as a function of the prevailing lockdown severity. Favorable resting heart rate decreases were most strongly related to reduction in sleep variability even after accounting for sleep duration. Although these changes were generalized across geographies and cultures, they unwound with easing of lockdowns.

Later timing of sleep has been associated with higher rates of metabolic dysfunction and cardiovascular disease47,48,49,50, while deviations from normal bedtimes have been shown to elevate resting heart rate51. There is also a risk that absence or attenuation of social cues together with reduced morning light exposure and increased evening light exposure from screens could lead to a loss of circadian rhythm entrainment17. However, freed from the usual obligations to wake up earlier than preferred during lockdown and removal of commuting time, the negative effects of later bedtimes were partially compensated by later wake times and reduced weekday-weekend sleep timing differences, with resultant gains in sleep regularity—results similarly echoed in prior work18,20,22. The latter can have positive effects on cardiometabolic health44 possibly reflected here in reduced resting heart rate. While a lower resting heart rate is most often associated with higher physical fitness, studies examining physical activity following lockdown have found declines of up to 40%52,53, making improved fitness an unlikely contributor in this context. Instead, our data indicated that at the country-level, sleep variability alone explained 70% of the variance in resting heart rate.

Our analysis also critically compared equivalent days across successive years. Raw measures in individual countries contain weekday-weekend, holiday and seasonality effects (e.g. in 2019) whereby weekends and holidays qualitatively show later sleep midpoints and higher resting heart rates. Conversely, summertime presented with later sleep midpoints, but with reduced sleep variability and lower resting heart rate. Seasonality effects are modulated by residential latitude and are phase-opposite in the Southern Hemisphere (Australia, New Zealand) compared to the Northern Hemisphere, while being practically absent in equatorial countries like Singapore54. Seasonality effects also appeared to be modulated during the lockdown, with an apparent advance in the onset of summertime in the Northern hemisphere and apparent prolongation of summertime in the South.

The benefits of improving sleep on health have economic impact and are increasingly recognized55. A large, time-use study suggested that interventions to increase sleep should concentrate on delaying morning start time for work and educational activities, increasing sleep opportunities and reducing commute times56. Creating greater opportunity for sleep by working from home and giving workers some flexibility in sleeping according to preferred schedules57 could yield benefits to both productivity and sleep health if properly implemented9,58. To realize sustained improvements in sleep behavior, a critical area to address is the erosion of boundaries between work and home life, and a growing expectation for workers to be ‘always on’ and reachable using information and communications technology59,60. For example, in France, there are laws governing the ‘right to disconnect’, to protect employees from having to engage in work-related electronic communications beyond working hours60.

The explosive expansion of video-conferencing has made functional home-based work and learning widely accessible—something unlikely had the current pandemic occurred just two decades ago. The precipitous growth in remote work and learning was reflected in a 10–60% rise in internet traffic within OECD countries during the early lockdown period in Mar–Apr 202061. The National Bureau of Economic Research found that for over 3 million users across 16 metropolitan areas, COVID-19 related mobility restrictions lengthened workdays by 48.5 min from the sending of emails outside regular working hours62. One network service provider reported an increase of 1–3 work hours per day in the US, UK, France, Spain, Canada and the Netherlands. In lieu to time saved from not having to commute, some started work earlier but ended at their habitual time63. Outside work, online gaming platforms and social media activity also increased64. Facebook reported increases of 100% on voice over internet calls and 50% in text messages on WhatsApp, Facebook Messenger and Instagram platforms during lockdown. For those with young children, adapting to home-based learning may also have displaced work time later. Some workers could also deliberately procrastinate sleep to regain a sense of control of personal time use. Together, these new daily routines could have contributed to progressively delayed midsleep timing. Future studies, supplemented by tools like ecological momentary assessments, would do well to understand heterogeneity in individual reactions to the blurring of work/non-work boundaries merits, and to include a wider demographic, particularly shift workers and lower income persons who live outside urban centers.

While this study highlights the strengths of being able to rapidly and remotely assess the impact of various intervention policies on sleep and resting heart rate, there are a few limitations to consider. (1) Only sleep periods between 4 and 12 h were analyzed. Shorter sleep periods could increase in frequency with work from home arrangements, but is in general challenging to detect and distinguish from other brief periods of sedentary behavior, e.g. sitting in bed reading a book or watching television. (2) Oura users typically come from middle to upper class households who could be more cushioned by the impact of COVID-19 and have flexible work arrangements. (3) Sleep quality measures were not obtained. These could be affected by anxiety over potential loss of jobs or contracting the disease. Results reported to date are mixed, and largely dependent on the demographic surveyed. Health professionals65, those with low levels of education65, shift workers15, individuals who showed symptoms of COVID-1915, those who usually lead active lifestyles66, females27,32, those who were laid off from jobs or experienced domestic conflicts35 and certain groups of students27,32 were amongst those more likely to report poorer quality during the lockdown. (4) As these data were extracted from a large wearable database, demographic variables (age, BMI, gender) were self-reported, and we were not able to obtain information about occupations, shift work status, presence of sleep disorders, medications, alcohol use, ethnic backgrounds, free versus work days or caregiving responsibilities of these users. Those with additional childcare responsibilities due to school closures or have had a member of the household fall ill might also have had to work late hours in order to catch up on work. Not-withstanding these limitations, our models based on stringency indices were able to capture > 50% of the variance in sleep and resting heart rate measures, indicating that it is a key predictor of sleep and resting heart rate trends during this period.

In sum, the use of large-scale wearable data revealed consistent and geographically widespread nature of the correlation between lockdown severity and shift to delayed but more regular sleep with reductions in resting heart rate measures. These findings should spur governments to consider the secondary health impact of various policies and interventions during this period and beyond.

Methods

Dataset

Data from 2019 was used as the reference year, representing a typical annual cycle and seasonal variation, for example, in terms of holidays, amount of light/daylength, and incidence of influenza-like illnesses. In order to ensure alignment by day of the week between 2019 and 2020, days were shifted before further computations were conducted. Due to the extra leap day in 2020, data was shifted by 1 day in Jan and Feb 2020 and by 2 days in Mar–Jul 2020. Local timestamps for each country and time zone were also utilized, which included shifts that reflect daylight savings start (Mar–Apr in the Northern Hemisphere, Sep–Oct in the Southern Hemisphere) and end points (Apr in the Southern Hemisphere, Sep–Nov in the Northern Hemisphere). Paired differences between matching days in 2019 and 2020 for users who had valid data in both timepoints were then computed and included in subsequent analyses.

Each valid sleep period was defined as the longest sleep episode for each day, with time in bed between 4 and 12 h. Three major variables were then extracted for each of these sleep periods: (1) Midsleep time was computed as the midpoint between bedtime and wake time, representing a proxy for circadian phase/chronotype67 (2) Midsleep variability was computed using a rolling 7-day standard deviation of midsleep times, representing a proxy for sleep time regularity, and (3) Resting heart rate was computed as an average of 5-min heart rate measures during the sleep period, across all sleep stages. Only sleep periods were considered in the assessment of RHR, because there are fewer confounding factors (e.g. food/alcohol/caffeine intake, prior physical activity and ambient temperature) during sleep as compared to daytime wake periods. Average resting heart rates < 30 bpm and > 100 bpm were removed as these were likely to represent physiologic or device anomalies. Due to an algorithm update in the spring of 2019 that affected computation of sleep duration by delaying bedtimes and advancing wake times, we could not reliably compare these changes between years. However, absolute sleep duration in 2020 was included as an additional variable in time-varying models predicting resting heart rate.

Age, BMI and gender information was self-reported by users upon app registration, and entered into models as potential covariates. This study was exempt from formal review by the National University of Singapore Institutional Review Board following their initial consideration of the protocol, as analysis involved the use of datasets stored without identifiers.

Computation of stringency index

Publicly available measures of restriction severity were extracted from the Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker46, focusing on 7 subscales believed to be most reflective of movement controls. These scales consisted of (1) school closures [0–3], (2) workplace closures [0–3], (3) cancellation of public events [0–2], (4) restrictions on public gatherings [0–4], (5) closures of public transport [0–2], (6) stay-at-home requirements [0–3] and (7) restrictions on internal movements [0–2]. These 7 subscales were summed up into a single stringency index [range: 0–19] and a mean value was computed for every month from January to July for each country.

Quantification of regional and global trends in sleep and resting heart rate

Changes in sleep and resting heart rate measures were derived for each month within each country separately, by first computing differences between equivalent days in Jan–Jul 2019 and 2020, and then averaging these differences by month. To estimate global (pooled) changes, separate random-effects meta-analyses by month were conducted for each predictor of interest—midsleep time, midsleep variability (standard deviation of the midsleep time over a 7-day rolling window) and resting heart rate. Meta-analyses were conducted using the R package ‘metafor’68. As there was evidence of high statistical heterogeneity between country estimates by month (Cochrane’s Q; P < 0.05, I2 > 75%), pooled estimates were weighted by the inverse variance of estimators for each country plus the estimated variance between countries. Note that values depict means ± 95% confidence intervals (CIs) unless explicitly stated otherwise.

Quantification of the effect of lockdown stringency on changes in sleep and resting heart rate

In order to quantify the effect of lockdown stringency on the heterogenous changes in sleep and resting heart rate patterns across countries, we ran multilevel growth curve models (MLMs) based on a sequential model-building approach. Multilevel models account for correlations between months within each country by allowing each country to have its own intercept. A null or baseline model is first constructed, and subsequent models consisting of the baseline model + additional explanatory variables were added sequentially to assess if the more complex model improved the overall model fit using a likelihood ratio test with degrees of freedom equal to the number of extra parameters. A significant likelihood ratio test indicates that the extra parameters improved the fit of the model to the data.

For each variable of interest (midsleep time, midsleep variability, resting heart rate), baseline MLMs (Model 1) were first estimated using country as a random intercept, month as a fixed effect, and a first-order autoregressive term. The latter was included to account for the nature of correlated time points in the repeated variable (month). Age, gender and BMI were entered as covariates, but were subsequently removed as they did not significantly improve the baseline model. Finally, in Model 2, we included the average lockdown stringency index by month as a time-varying factor to Model 1, in order to examine the overall effect of lockdown stringency on sleep and resting heart rate measures.

Quantification of the effect of changes to sleep patterns on changes to resting heart rate

To test our hypothesis that changes to sleep patterns (midsleep time and midsleep variability) would lead to associated changes in resting heart rate, we conducted further MLM analyses with changes in resting heart rate as the dependent variable and changes in midsleep time and midsleep variability as explanatory variables. Sleep duration in 2020 was also included as an additional variable of interest in this model. A baseline MLM (Model 1) with a random intercept, month as fixed effect, and first-order autoregressive structure was first constructed. Age, gender and BMI were entered as covariates, but were subsequently removed as they did not significantly improve the baseline model. Next, in Models 2–4, sleep duration in 2020, changes to midsleep time and changes to midsleep variability were added as time-varying predictors in separate models. Finally, in Model 5, all three sleep measures were entered in at the same time to assess the independent contributions of each predictor in the model.

All MLMs were estimated using the full information maximum likelihood method and performed using the nlme package in R (version 3.6.1). Significance level was determined at an alpha level of 0.05. Marginal and conditional R2 values for mixed models were calculated based on the method proposed by Nakagawa, Johnson and Schielzeth69. Notably, the marginal R2 only takes into account the variance of the fixed effects, while the conditional R2 takes both fixed and random effects into account.