Introduction

The gaseous signalling molecule nitric oxide (NO) has been implicated in the regulation of a number of important neurophysiological processes such as neurogenesis, sleep–wake cycle, appetite, hormone release, and blood pressure1. Of particular interest is the discovery that NO plays a significant role in the early stages of memory formation in a variety of species from humans to invertebrates2,3,4,5. In the snail, Lymnaea stagnalis, a well-established model organism for learning and memory studies, there is an obligatory requirement for NO during the first 5 h of long-term memory (LTM) formation following single-trial associative food-reward conditioning6. On the other hand, NO is a highly reactive free radical with potential cytotoxic properties. Indeed, inappropriate changes in the level of NO contribute to the development of serious pathological conditions of the nervous system7,8. Therefore, NO production in the normal brain is tightly regulated through a variety of mechanisms. Long non-coding Natural Antisense Transcripts (NATs) appear to be one of the most intriguing additions to the list of such mechanisms.

Long NATs collectively refer to endogenous RNA molecules with lengths exceeding 200 nucleotides that are complementary to RNA transcripts of already established function. Depending on their origin, all long NATs can be grouped into two classes: cis-encoded and trans-encoded. Cis-encoded NATs are transcribed from the same loci as their sense counterparts whereas trans-encoded NATs are transcribed from different loci. Recent studies have shown that long NATs are abundant in eukaryotes and are particularly prevalent in the central nervous system9,10,11,12.

In our previous publications we reported on the discovery of two trans-encoded long NATs (antiNOS-1 and antiNOS-2), which are expressed in the brain of the pond-snail, Lymnaea stagnalis, and are complementary to the nitric oxide synthase (NOS)-encoding mRNA13,14,15,16. Both NATs are transcribed from a NOS pseudogene and are associated with the negative regulation of the production of gaseous neurotransmitter nitric oxide (NO) by NOS.

In this study we report on the characterization of a novel cis-encoded NAT, which is transcribed from the non-template strand of the Lym-NOS1 gene. Hereafter we will refer to this antisense RNA as Lym-NOS1AS. The Lym-NOS1AS is spliced, polyadenylated, does not contain ORFs larger than an arbitrary size and could therefore be assigned to a group of long non-coding RNAs (ncRNAs). We demonstrate that the Lym-NOS1AS is co-expressed with the Lym-NOS1 mRNA in the cerebral giant cell (CGC), a neuron with an established role in the conditioned feeding response17. Furthermore, we report on the timed and targeted differential regulation of Lym-NOS1AS in the brain by reward conditioning leading to LTM formation. Intriguingly, these learning-induced changes in the expression of Lym-NOS1AS correlate well with the previously discovered alteration of memory lapse and non-lapse periods18.

Results

A cis-encoded long NAT complementary to Lym-NOS1 mRNA is expressed in the brain

While screening a snail CNS cDNA library with a NOS-specific probe, we isolated a transcript of about 2500 nt in length. Although the transcript possesses some features of a typical mRNA, such as the polyadenylation signal and a poly(A) tail, it is unlikely that it can be translated because of the presence of multiple stop codons in all the reading frames. This indicated that we had cloned a long ncRNA. Another and rather unexpected feature of this novel ncRNA was the presence of 367 nt sequence, which is complementary to the 5′ end of the Lym-NOS1 mRNA (Fig. 1a). The degree of complementarity (100%) indicated strongly that this ncRNA is transcribed from the non-template strand of the Lym-NOS1 locus and belongs to a group of long cis-encoded NATs (Fig. 1b). Consequently, we named this NAT Lym-NOS1AS (accession number MW300420).

Figure 1
figure 1

Lym-NOS1AS is a long cis-encoded natural antisense transcript. (a) Nucleotide sequence of Lym-NOS1AS NAT (accession number MW300420). The antisense region (shown in red) is complementary to Lym-NOS1 mRNA (accession number AF012531). A putative polyadenylation signal is underlined. (b) A schematic diagram showing that Lym-NOS1AS is transcribed from the non-template strand of the Lym-NOS1 locus.

Of note, the central nervous system of Lymnaea contains 11 ganglia (paired cerebral, pedal, pleural, parietal, buccal ganglia and unpaired visceral ganglion, Fig. 2a). Our previous work established that the implicit memory trace resulting from single-trial food-reward classical conditioning is both acquired and stored in the same neural circuit located in the buccal and cerebral ganglia (hereafter ‘learning ganglia’)19. With this in mind, we decided to determine whether the Lym-NOS1AS is differentially expressed in the two parts of the learning ganglia. To achieve this, we extracted RNA from individual cerebral or buccal ganglia and the purified RNA samples were then subjected to real-time RT-PCR to estimate the level of Lym-NOS1AS expression. The results of the analysis show that the expression level of the NAT is almost 3 times higher in the cerebral ganglia than in the buccal ganglia (Fig. 2b).

Figure 2
figure 2

Lym-NOS1AS expression in ‘naïve’ ‘learning’ ganglia. (a) A diagram of the Lymnaea CNS. The ‘learning’ ganglia [cerebral ganglia (CG) + buccal ganglia (BG)] are highlighted in grey. The white dots in the cerebral ganglia indicate the position of the paired Cerebral Giant Cells (see Fig. 3). (b) Results of real-time RT-PCR performed on the cerebral (dark grey) and buccal (light grey) ganglia dissected from naïve snails (n = 5 samples, each sample contained material from 4 animals). The relative level of Lym-NOS1AS expression was calculated as 2−ΔΔCt. The asterisk indicates a significant difference between the two groups (two-tailed t-test for independent samples: t = −4.58, df = 8, p < 0.01). The data in this figure are shown as means ± SEM.

Lym-NOS1AS NAT and Lym-NOS1 mRNA are co-expressed in the CGC

In a previous work, we demonstrated that in rodents, a very similarly organized NOS-related cis-encoded NAT (Mm-antiNos1) acts as a negative post-transcriptional regulator of neuronal NOS gene expression20. This suggested to us that the Lym-NOS1AS can function in the same manner as the mouse ortholog regulating NO signaling in the brain. Apparently, however, it is only possible if both sense and antisense transcripts are co-expressed in a neuron. Hence, the question is: “Do such neurons exist in Lymnaea CNS?” Considering that Lym-NOS1 mRNA is expressed only in a small population of neurons21, this issue deserves special attention. Therefore, we relied on an important advantage of our model system, which is the presence of large easily identifiable neurons. A particularly promising candidate that can be used to address the question is the pair of CGCs. First, our in situ hybridization experiments show that the CGCs display much higher level of Lym-NOS1 mRNA in comparison to the other cell types located in the cerebral ganglia (Fig. 3ai, aii). Second, they are key neurons of the Lymnaea memory network17. Consequently, we identified and dissected 10 CGCs and RNA isolated from these cells was subjected to RT-PCR. The results of the analysis demonstrate clearly that the PCR products of exactly the expected sizes are detected when Lym-NOS1 (Fig. 3b) or Lym-NOS1AS (Fig. 3c) specific primers were used. Of note, the identity of the PCR products was confirmed by cloning and sequencing. Thus, we can conclude that the CGCs are an example of neurons, which express both Lym-NOS1 mRNA and Lym-NOS1AS.

Figure 3
figure 3

Lym-NOS1 mRNA and Lym-NOS1AS NAT are expressed in the CGC. (a) In situ hybridization shows a bilaterally symmetrical pair of large neuronal cell bodies in the left (ai) and right (aii) cerebral ganglia (see Fig. 2a for location of the cerebral ganglia in the brain) stained with the Lym-NOS1 mRNA specific probe. The cell bodies correspond in size and position to the identified CGCs (arrowed). Of note, there are several thousands of other types of neurons in the cerebral ganglia, but they are poorly stained, indicative of a very low level of the Lym-NOS1 mRNA. The dotted line boxes show the stained CGCs at a higher magnification. It is worth mentioning that the CGC has a very large nucleus that fills a significant part of the cell but only the cytoplasm shows significant hybridization, as expected. (b, c) The results of conventional RT-PCR experiments conducted on RNA extracted from isolated CGCs to detect Lym-NOS1 (b) and Lym-NOS1AS (c). The ‘RT+’ lanes show that the PCR products (arrowed) of the expected sizes (101 bp in case of Lym-NOS1 and 108 bp in case of Lym-NOS1AS) are detected indicating that the CGCs express both Lym-NOS1 and Lym-NOS1AS. The ‘RT−’ lanes represent the outcome of the control experiments in which reverse transcriptase was omitted. The absence of the amplification products in these lanes is indicative that the RNA preparation was free of DNA contamination.

Single trial conditioning differentially regulates the expression of Lym-NOS1AS in the brain

Our discovery that Lym-NOS1 mRNA and Lym-NOS1AS are co-expressed in the CGCs suggests that the Lym-NOS1AS is a part of the pathway involved in LTM formation. In order to test this hypothesis we conducted the following quantitative experiments.

First and foremost, in all our experiments in which the effects of single trial conditioning on gene expression were studied, a randomly chosen group of animals was retained, trained and tested for LTM formation 24 h after training. This was to confirm that LTM would have occurred in the experimental animals that were used in our quantitative assays. Notably, the mean feeding response to amyl acetate (CS) of the trained snails was always significantly higher than the response of unconditioned animals (Fig. 4a).

Figure 4
figure 4

Training-induced differential regulation of the Lym-NOS1AS. (a) The result of the behavioural test of LTM formation at 24 h after training. The mean feeding response to amyl acetate (the CS) of the classically conditioned animals (black bar) is significantly higher than the response of the unpaired control animals (white bar) (n = 15 animals per group, two-tailed t-test for independent samples: t = 6.94, df = 28, p < 0.0001). (b) Schematic representation of the experiment to investigate whether single-trial reward conditioning is associated with timed and targeted changes in the expression of Lym-NOS1AS. (c, d) Results of real-time RT-PCR performed on the cerebral and buccal ganglia, respectively. The relative level of Lym-NOS1AS expression in ganglia from conditioned (blue bars) and unpaired control animals (white bars) dissected at 1 h, 2 h, 4 h and 6 h after training was calculated as 2−ΔΔCt. All data in this figure are shown as means ± SEM. Asterisks indicate significant differences (p < 0.05) between the conditioned and unpaired data at the same time point (n = 20 animals per group, unpaired two-tailed t-tests with Welch’s correction).

To examine the effect of behavioral conditioning on temporal expression of the Lym-NOS1AS in different parts of the ‘learning ganglia’ we dissected individual cerebral and buccal ganglia at 1 h, 2 h, 4 h and 6 h after training (Fig. 4b). These time points were chosen because previous studies showed that (1) NO is only required for up to 5 h for successful consolidation of 24-h long-term memory after single-trial classical conditionin 6; (2) a period of memory lapse occurs at 2 h post-training, while at 1 h, 4 h post-training memory is fully expressed in response to the conditioned stimulus and thus these are regarded as non-lapse periods18.

RNA extracted from the dissected material was subjected to real-time RT-PCR in which the expression of the Lym-NOS1AS was analyzed using a calibrator-normalized relative quantification method. Notably, the results of the quantitative analysis showed no significant difference between the conditioned and unpaired groups in either the cerebral or the buccal ganglia at 6 h post-training, when memory formation is already not reliant on NO synthesis. Importantly, however, the same analysis revealed statistically significant down-regulation of the Lym-nNOS1AS in the cerebral ganglia from the 1 h and 4 h post-training conditioned groups (Fig. 4c), and upregulation in the cerebral ganglia from the 2 h post-training conditioned group. In contrast, no significant training related changes in the Lym-NOS1AS expression in the buccal ganglia in all tested groups of snails have been detected (Fig. 4d). Taken together these data show that LTM formation is associated with specific differential alterations in the expression of the Lym-NOS1AS, which are precisely timed and targeted to the cerebral ganglia.

Discussion

One very intriguing development in contemporary molecular neurobiology has been the discovery that long NATs are abundantly expressed in the CNS10,12,22. This suggests that these RNA molecules could be engaged in various aspects of brain function. Therefore, an especially important task now is to understand which particular neural processes depend on the expression of NATs.

The major focus of the current study is a novel NOS-related long natural antisense RNA, which is expressed in Lymnaea brain. We named this RNA Lym-NOS1AS. It is important to note that the Lym-NOS1AS shares many features with another NOS-related NAT (Mm-antiNos1) that we previously identified in the mouse brain20. Both molluscan and mammalian RNAs are cis-encoded, non-coding, spliced and polyadenylated. Also, their antisense regions have similar sizes and locations. This remarkable evolutionary conservation suggests strongly that the NOS-related NATs are of functional importance. And indeed, we have shown earlier that the Mm-antiNos1 RNA negatively regulates NO signaling in the brain and is likely to be involved in the regulation of neurogenesis20. Here we presented data that indicate that the molluscan ortholog can also participate in complex neural processes.

In the snail brain, two types of NOS-encoding mRNAs are expressed: Lym-nNOS1 and Lym-nNOS2. Notably, while the similarity between the Lym-NOS1 and Lym-NOS2 mRNAs within the open reading frames is remarkably high (89%), their untranslated regions are unique15. Furthermore, the genes, from which these two mRNAs are transcribed, respond dissimilarly to single-trial reward conditioning. The expression of Lym-nNOS1 is differentially regulated by training, whereas the expression of Lym-nNOS2 remains stable at all measured post-training time points15. These data, together with the fact that the Lym-NOS1AS is complementary exclusively to the Lym-nNOS1 mRNA raise the exciting possibility that this novel NAT, through the regulation of the Lym-nNOS1 expression, acts as an important component of the pathway regulating LTM formation.

To validate this idea further, we utilized some principal advantages of our model system. Among them are an opportunity to investigate molecular processes at the single cell level. Here, we exploited the existence of a pair of serotonergic cerebral giant cells, the CGCs in the Lymnaea brain. Importantly, these easily identifiable cells gate the conditioned feeding response and are an essential part of the neural network involved in LTM formation17. Also, we have shown that the CGCs express Lym-nNOS1 mRNA and that the expression of Lym-nNOS1 in these cells is regulated by learning15. But do the CGCs contain the Lym-NOS1AS? To answer this important question, we conducted RT-PCR on isolated CGCs and demonstrated that these neurons express both the Lym-nNOS1 mRNA and the Lym-NOS1AS. This result suggests that there is interaction between the sense and the antisense RNAs with a potential role in memory formation.

To further explore this possibility, we utilized the ability of Lymnaea to acquire LTM after a single appetitive conditioning trial, which allows to study conditioning-induced pathways in a precisely timed manner. For example, our previous relevant work established that there is an obligatory requirement for NO produced by NOS during the initial stages (up to 5 h following memory acquisition) of LTM formation6. The main goal of our present experiments was to establish whether Lym-NOS1AS is also regulated by learning. With this in mind, we launched a large-scale quantitative experiment, in which we measured the expression levels of the Lym-NOS1AS in the ‘learning’ ganglia at different time points after conditioning. Of note, the ‘learning’ ganglia contain neural circuits, which are essential for LTM formation19. Our quantitative studies culminated in two important observations. First, Lym-NOS1AS is differentially regulated by training within the learning ganglia. Namely, it is downregulated or upregulated in the cerebral ganglia at specific time points but shows no change in its expression in the buccal ganglia. Second, Lym-NOS1AS expression in the cerebral ganglia is transiently suppressed at 1 h and 4 h and transiently stimulated at 2 h after conditioning. Thus, the observed learning-induced changes in the Lym-NOS1AS ‘gene’ activity are targeted to the cerebral ganglia, where most of the NO-dependent information processing takes place during memory consolidation15. Furthermore, these changes are precisely timed and occur at a period when memory consolidation goes through a critical phase18 and NO is essential for LTM6.

It was demonstrated in a previous study that memory consolidation in the snail, just like in higher organisms including humans, develops through consecutive periods when the strength of the memory fluctuates resulting in intervals disrupting the continuous strengthening of the memory trace. During these intervals, the memory temporarily becomes weak and vulnerable to interference18. One such ‘lapse’ period was identified in Lymnaea, at 2 h post-training. In contrast, the 1 h and 4 h time points were identified as ‘non-lapse’ periods. Interestingly, the findings reported in the current paper reveal a correlation between the lapse/non-lapse periods and the level of Lym-NOS1AS expression. Specifically, the 1 h and 4 h non-lapse periods coincide with the downregulation of the NAT, whereas the 2 h lapse period coincides with the upregulation of the NAT. Thus, we can suggest that the observed suppression of the Lym-NOS1AS-dependent “NO break” at 1 h and 4 h post-training promotes NO production and provides a plausible explanation for the robustness of NO-dependent memory trace at these time periods. And the other way around, the detected activation of the break at 2 h post-training suppresses NO production and therefore can account for the observed temporary interruption of the NO-dependent phase of memory consolidation. Furthermore, the lack of difference in Lym-NOS1AS levels between conditioned and unpaired control animals at 6 h post-training also fits in perfectly with the previous conclusion that by this time the NO-dependent phase of memory consolidation is over6. Moreover, the cerebral ganglia are known to be involved in forming LTM. Therefore, our findings that the training-induced changes in the expression of Lym-NOS1AS are targeted to the cerebral ganglia further support our idea that this non-coding NAT is a component of the molecular pathway activated by one-trial conditioning.

Finally, both our everyday experience and the numerous behavioral studies show that not all learning culminates in the formation of long-lasting memories. This is due to the existence of inhibitory constraints that apply a continual brake on the molecular mechanisms the activation of which is required for LTM, such as the NO pathway. It is possible, though we currently have no direct evidence on this point, that Lym-NOS1AS represents an important example of such memory suppressors. Apparently, single-trial induced LTM requires this memory constraint to be absent or reduced at 1 h and 4 h following the learning event; time points representing non-lapse periods during memory consolidation. However, whether the revealed targeted and precisely timed removal of the brake provided by the Lym-NOS1AS is essential for or simply facilitates LTM formation has yet to be established.

Material and methods

Experimental animals

Specimens of Lymnaea stagnalis were raised in the breeding facility of the University of Sussex, where they were kept in 20–22 °C copper free water under 12 h light/dark cycle. They were fed on lettuce 3 times and a vegetable-based fish food twice a week as described previously6.

cDNA library screening

A Lymnaea CNS cDNA library was screened using a radioactively labeled fragment of Lym-nNOS1 cDNA21. A positive clone containing a cDNA insert of 2.5 kb was selected for further examination. Sequence analysis of the insert has shown that it contains a region complementary to the Lym-nNOS1 mRNA.

In situ hybridization

In situ hybridization of frozen sections of Lymnaea CNS was performed as previously described23. The labelled probe (5′-CACAGGA(AC)GGTATGGTGTTCT-3′) was prepared using the DIG Oligonucleotide Tailing Kit (Roche) according to the manufacturer’s protocol.

Reverse transcription-PCR on the cerebral giant cells

The cell bodies of CGCs were identified and individually dissected from the CNS of Lymnaea as described previously15. Total RNA extracted from the CGCs (n = 10) by means of the Absolutely RNA Nanoprep kit (Agilent Technologies) was subjected to reverse transcription. The reverse transcription reaction was carried out in a final volume of 5 μl using the iScript cDNA synthesis kit according to the manufacturer’s protocol (Bio-Rad). The resulted cDNAs were amplified by means of the HotStar Taq DNA polymerase according to the manufacturer’s protocol (QIAGEN). The following primers were used: 5′-AGTTTGAGGGATGAGAACCT-3′ and 5′-AAGGGACATTACACAGAGG-3′ for detection of Lym-NOS1 (accession number AF012531, the amplicon size is 101 bp), and 5′-GTAATAAGCGCATTTGCATAC-3′ and 5′-CCTGGTGTGAAGCTGATC-3′ for detection of Lym-NOS1AS (accession number MW300420, the amplicon size is 108 bp). The resulted PCR products were resolved in 2% agarose gel. The identity of the PCR products was confirmed by cloning and sequencing.

One-trial conditioning protocol and surgical procedures

Reward conditioning was carried out using a well-established single-trial classical conditioning protocol6,15,16,17,18,24,25. Snails were randomly assigned to experimental (paired) and control (unpaired) groups to be given a single conditioning and control trial, respectively. Experimental animals were exposed to a solution of amyl acetate (CS, 0.004% final concentration) and 30 s later to a sucrose solution (US, 0.67%) and stayed in the mixture of solutions for 2 min. Control animals were exposed to the CS and to the US, separated by an interval of 1 h. A randomly chosen sub-set of 20 animals from each group were retained and tested for LTM formation at 24 h after the paired and unpaired trials as previously described15. A third group of animals was kept under the same conditions and had the same feeding regime as experimental and unpaired control snails but was not exposed to either the CS or US. This group is referred to as naive animals. At different time points (1 h, 2 h, 4 h, and 6 h) after the treatment a randomly chosen sub-set of animals (20 snails per time point) were sacrificed and the cerebral and the buccal ganglia were removed, transferred immediately to crushed dry ice and then stored at − 80° C until use.

Quantitative real-time RT-PCR

The frozen (− 80 °C) cerebral and the buccal ganglia dissected from experimental (paired) and control snails (unpaired and naïve) (n = 20 in each group) were used to extract RNA by means of the Absolutely RNA miRNA kit (Agilent Technologies). The isolated RNA samples were treated with DNase TURBO (Ambion) to remove any traces of DNA and then quantified using the NanoDrop microvolume spectrophotometer. RNA integrity was confirmed by gel electrophoresis. The purified RNAs were copied into cDNAs using the iScript cDNA synthesis kit according to the manufacturer’s protocol (Bio-Rad). cDNAs were amplified and analyzed on the Mx3000P real-time cycler (Stratagene) using the Biotool SYBR Green qPCR Master mix (Stratech). Cycling parameters for the PCR were as follows: denaturation, 95 °C, 15 s; annealing, 52 °C, 20 s; extension, 60 °C, 20 s. We used primers 5′- GTAATAAGCGCATTTGCATAC-3′ and 5′- CCTGGTGTGAAGCTGATC-3′ for detection of Lym-NOS1AS (accession number MW300420, the amplicon size is 108 bp), and primers 5′-AAGGGACATTACACAGAGG-3′ and 5′-GTGTCAGTTGGAATCCTTG-3′ for detection of β-tubulin. The amount of Lym-NOS1AS NAT, normalized to an endogenous reference (β-tubulin mRNA) and relative to a calibrator, was calculated as 2−ΔΔCT.

Statistical analysis

In both the behavioural and molecular experiments comparisons between two independent groups (e.g., unpaired and paired) were carried out using unpaired two-tailed tests. Welch’s correction was used when the samples had unequal variances. All statistical analyses were carried out using Prism (GraphPad) software. The differences were considered statistically significant at p < 0.05.