Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Intermittent collective motion in sheep results from alternating the role of leader and follower

Abstract

Flocking behaviour is often presented as an example of a self-organized process, where individuals continuously negotiate on the direction of travel and compromise by moving along a local average velocity until the group reaches a consensus. Such a collective behaviour does not take advantage of the benefits of hierarchical organizational strategies that confer the leader of the group full control over it with a reduced information flow overhead. Here we study the spontaneous behaviour of small sheep flocks and find that sheep exhibit a collective behaviour that consists of a series of collective motion episodes interrupted by grazing phases. Each motion episode has a temporal leader that guides the group in line formation. Combining experiments and a data-driven model, we provide evidence that group coordination in these episodes results from the propagation of positional information of the temporal leader to all group members through a strongly hierarchical, directed interaction network. Furthermore, we show that group members alternate the role of leader and follower by a random process, which is independent of the navigation mechanism that regulates collective motion episodes. Our analysis suggests that it is possible to conceive intermittent collective strategies that take advantage of both hierarchical and democratic organizational schemes.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Intermittent collective motion and leader selection.
Fig. 2: Inconsistency with previous models.
Fig. 3: Guiding mechanism and interaction network (IN) during a CMP.
Fig. 4: Efficient group guidance and information processing.
Fig. 5: Generalization of the model to larger groups.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The raw data in this work are available in the public repository Zenodo (https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6905807). Data that support the plots within this paper and other findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. Source data are provided with this paper.

Code availability

The custom codes written in Matlab (Matlab 2019a) to analyse the data presented in this paper are available from the corresponding author upon request.

References

  1. Krause, J. & Ruxton, G. D. Living in Groups (Oxford Univ. Press, 2002).

  2. Ioannou, C. C., Guttal, V. & Couzin, I. D. Predatory fish select for coordinated collective motion in virtual prey. Science 337, 1212–1215 (2012).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  3. Berdahl, A., Torney, C. J., Ioannou, C. C., Faria, J. J. & Couzin, I. D. Emergent sensing of complex environments by mobile animal groups. Science 339, 574–576 (2013).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  4. Flack, A., Nagy, M., Fiedler, W., Couzin, I. D. & Wikelski, M. From local collective behavior to global migratory patterns in white storks. Science 360, 911–914 (2018).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  5. Pillot, M.-H., Gautrais, J., Arrufat, P., Couzin, I. D., Bon, R. & Deneubourg, J.-L. Scalable rules for coherent group motion in a gregarious vertebrate. PLoS ONE 6, e14487 (2011).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  6. Strandburg-Peshkin, A., Farine, D. R., Couzin, I. D. & Crofoot, M. C. Shared decision-making drives collective movement in wild baboons. Science 348, 1358–1361 (2015).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  7. Ginelli, F. et al. Intermittent collective dynamics emerge from conflicting imperatives in sheep herds. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 12729–12734 (2015).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  8. Toulet, S., Gautrais, J., Bon, R. & Peruani, F. Imitation combined with a characteristic stimulus duration results in robust collective decision-making. PLoS ONE 10, e0140188 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Viscido, S. V., Parrish, J. K. & Grünbaum, D. Individual behavior and emergent properties of fish schools: a comparison of observation and theory. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 273, 239–249 (2004).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  10. Ballerini, M. et al. Interaction ruling animal collective behavior depends on topological rather than metric distance: evidence from a field study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 1232–1237 (2008).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  11. Lukeman, R., Li, Y.-X. & Edelstein-Keshet, L. Inferring individual rules from collective behavior. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 12576–12580 (2010).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  12. Vicsek, T., Czirók, A., Ben-Jacob, E., Cohen, I. & Shochet, O. Novel type of phase transition in a system of self-driven particles. Phys. Rev. Lett. 75, 1226–1229 (1995).

    Article  MathSciNet  ADS  Google Scholar 

  13. Couzin, I. D., Krause, J., James, R., Ruxton, G. D. & Franks, N. R. Collective memory and spatial sorting in animal groups. J. Theor. Biol. 218, 1–11 (2002).

    Article  MathSciNet  ADS  Google Scholar 

  14. Vicsek, T. & Zafeiris, A. Collective motion. Phys. Rep. 517, 71–140 (2012).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  15. Marchetti, M. C. et al. Hydrodynamics of soft active matter. Rev. Mod. Phys. 85, 1143–1189 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Couzin, I. D., Krause, J., Franks, N. R. & Levin, S. A. Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature 433, 513–516 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Guttal, V. & Couzin, I. D. Social interactions, information use and the evolution of collective migration. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 16172–16177 (2010).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  18. Katz, Y., Tunstrøm, K., Ioannou, C. C., Huepe, C. & Couzin, I. D. Inferring the structure and dynamics of interactions in schooling fish. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 18720–18725 (2011).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  19. Herbert-Read, J. E. et al. Inferring the rules of interaction of shoaling fish. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 18726–18731 (2011).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  20. Nagy, M., Akos, Z., Biro, D. & Vicsek, T. Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks. Nature 464, 890–893 (2010).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  21. Nagy, M. et al. Context-dependent hierarchies in pigeons. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 13049–13054 (2013).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  22. Nagy, M., Couzin, I. D., Fiedler, W., Wikelski, M. & Flack, A. Synchronization, coordination and collective sensing during thermalling flight of freely migrating white storks. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 373, 20170011 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. King, A. J. & Cowlishaw, G. Leaders, followers and group decision-making. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2, 147–150 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Tyler, S. J. The behaviour and social organization of the new forest ponies. Anim. Behav. Monogr. 5, 87–196 (1972).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Klingel, H. Soziale organisation und verhaltensweisen von Hartmann-und Bergzebras (Equus zebra hartmannae und e. z. zebra). Z. Tierpsychol. 25, 76–88 (1968).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Krueger, K., Flauger, B., Farmer, K. & Hemelrijk, C. Movement initiation in groups of feral horses. Behav. Process. 103, 91–101 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Portugal, S. J. et al. Upwash exploitation and downwash avoidance by flap phasing in ibis formation flight. Nature 505, 399–402 (2014).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  28. Fruchart, M., Hanai, R., Littlewood, P. B. & Vitelli, V. Non-reciprocal phase transitions. Nature 592, 363–369 (2021).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  29. Dadhichi, L. P., Kethapelli, J., Chajwa, R., Ramaswamy, S. & Maitra, A. Nonmutual torques and the unimportance of motility for long-range order in two-dimensional flocks. Phys. Rev. E 101, 052601 (2020).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  30. Xue, T., Li, X., Grassberger, P. & Chen, L. Swarming transitions in hierarchical societies. Phys. Rev. Res. 2, 042017 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Stein, J. C. Information production and capital allocation: decentralized versus hierarchical firms. J. Financ. 57, 1891–1921 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Mookherjee, D. Decentralization, hierarchies and incentives: a mechanism design perspective. J. Econ. Lit. 44, 367–390 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Hennart, J. F. in Organization Theory and the Multinational Corporation 157–181 (eds Ghoshal, S. & Westney, D. E.) (Palgrave Macmillan, 1993).

  34. Bode, N. W., Faria, J. J., Franks, D. W., Krause, J. & Wood, A. J. How perceived threat increases synchronization in collectively moving animal groups. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 277, 3065–3070 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Bode, N. W., Franks, D. W. & Wood, A. J. Limited interactions in flocks: relating model simulations to empirical data. J. R. Soc. Interface 8, 301–304 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Huepe, C. & Aldana, M. Intermittency and clustering in a system of self-driven particles. Phys. Rev. Lett. 92, 168701 (2004).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  37. Tunström, K. et al. Collective states, multistability and transitional behavior in schooling fish. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1002915 (2013).

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  38. Trillmich, J., Fichtel, C. & Kappeler, P. M. Coordination of group movements in wild Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi). Behaviour 141, 1103–1120 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Ariel, G. & Ayali, A. Locust collective motion and its modeling. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11, e1004522 (2015).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  40. Kramer, D. L. & McLaughlin, R. L. The behavioral ecology of intermittent locomotion. Am. Zool. 41, 137–153 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  41. Nathan, R. et al. A movement ecology paradigm for unifying organismal movement research. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 19052–19059 (2008).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  42. Strandburg-Peshkin, A., Papageorgiou, D., Crofoot, M. C. & Farine, D. R. Inferring influence and leadership in moving animal groups. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 373, 20170006 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Leca, J.-B., Gunst, N., Thierry, B. & Petit, O. Distributed leadership in semifree-ranging white-faced capuchin monkeys. Anim. Behav. 66, 1045–1052 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Gascuel, H.-M., Peruani, F. & Bon, R. Identifying interaction neighbours in animal groups. Anim. Behav. 174, 97–104 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Conradt, L. & Roper, T. J. Consensus decision making in animals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 449–456 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Strömbom, D. Collective motion from local attraction. J. Theor. Biol. 283, 145–151 (2011).

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  ADS  Google Scholar 

  47. Ferrante, E., Turgut, A. E., Dorigo, M. & Huepe, C. Elasticity-based mechanism for the collective motion of self-propelled particles with springlike interactions: a model system for natural and artificial swarms. Phys. Rev. Lett. 111, 268302 (2013).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  48. Pearce, D. J., Miller, A. M., Rowlands, G. & Turner, M. S. Role of projection in the control of bird flocks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 10422–10426 (2014).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  49. Barberis, L. & Peruani, F. Large-scale patterns in a minimal cognitive flocking model: incidental leaders, nematic patterns and aggregates. Phys. Rev. Lett. 117, 248001 (2016).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  50. Herbert-Read, J. E. Understanding how animal groups achieve coordinated movement. J. Exp. Biol. 219, 2971–2983 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Lavergne, F. A., Wendehenne, H., Bäuerle, T. & Bechinger, C. Group formation and cohesion of active particles with visual perception-dependent motility. Science 364, 70–74 (2019).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  52. Charlesworth, H. J. & Turner, M. S. Intrinsically motivated collective motion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 15362–15367 (2019).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  53. Strömbom, D., Hassan, T., Hunter Greis, W. & Antia, A. Asynchrony induces polarization in attraction-based models of collective motion. R. Soc. Open Sci. 6, 190381 (2019).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  54. Romanczuk, P., Couzin, I. D. & Schimansky-Geier, L. Collective motion due to individual escape and pursuit response. Phys. Rev. Lett. 102, 010602 (2009).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  55. Strömbom, D. et al. Bistability and switching behavior in moving animal groups. Northeast J. Complex Syst. 4, 1 (2022).

    Google Scholar 

  56. Strandburg-Peshkin, A. et al. Visual sensory networks and effective information transfer in animal groups. Curr. Biol. 23, R709–R711 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Agudo-Canalejo, J. & Golestanian, R. Active phase separation in mixtures of chemically interacting particles. Phys. Rev. Lett. 123, 018101 (2019).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  58. Gastil, J. A definition and illustration of democratic leadership. Hum. Relat. 47, 953–975 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Galton, F. Vox populi (the wisdom of crowds). Nature 75, 450–451 (1907).

    Article  MATH  ADS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank A. Dusstour, A. Kacelnik and A. Perna for discussions and comments on the text. We also thank Montpellier Supagro Research Station (Domaine du Merle) and M.-H. Pillot for technical support in herd management and data acquisition. L.G.-N. acknowledges support by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) under Germany’s Excellence Strategy—EXC 2002/1 ‘Science of Intelligence’—project 390523135. F.P. acknowledges financial support from CY Initiative of Excellence (grant ‘Investissements d’Avenir’ ANR-16-IDEX-0008), INEX 2021 Ambition project ‘Collective Intelligence’ (CollInt) [2021-008, CYIn-AAP2021-AmbEm-0000000031] and Labex MME-DII, projects 2021-258 and 2021-297.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

L.G.-N., R.B. and F.P. designed the study, performed statistical analysis of the data and derived the mathematical models used to interpret the data.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Fernando Peruani.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Physics thanks Cristian Huepe and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Experimental setup and characteristic times.

a, Scheme of the experimental setup, consisting of 4 adjacent arenas (area of 80 × 80 meters each one), with a 7-meter-high tower placed in the middle. The tower was used to acquire the videos, that would later be used to get the tracking of the individuals. b, Probability distribution of the duration times tCMP of the Collective Motion Phases (CMPs) observed in the experiments. c, Probability distribution of the duration times tGP of the Grazing Phases (GPs) observed in the experiments.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Quantifying the speed of individuals during CMPs.

a, Probability distribution P(v) of the individual velocities. This distribution allows us to define a threshold velocity vth = 0.45 m/s, which is the local minimum of the fit, depicted with a solid black line. b, Instantaneous velocities vi(t) of all the individuals of the group during one CMP. c, Average individual velocity 〈v〉CMP for all CMPs. The red line is the mean value over all CMPs, and the light red region represents the standard deviation. Figures ac correspond to experiments with groups of N = 4 individuals, but are representative for all group sizes.

Extended Data Fig. 3 Characterization of CMPs.

ac, Probability of finding a neighbour in a given direction as function of the rank of individuals within the line for group sizes N = 2, 3 and 4. di, Quantification of the orientational and spatial order by the average order parameter values 〈P〉 and 〈ψ〉 for all group sizes and for all CMPs. Data are presented as mean values (blue circles) ± SD computed over the whole duration of the corresponding CMP for each group size N = 2, 3 and 4. The red line depicts the mean value of the order parameters -〈P〉 and 〈ψ〉 - across all CMPs.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Quantification of leadership over many CMPs.

Probability distributions of playing the role of leader during a CMP, using m = 4. The blue bars represent the experimental results, and the red dots correspond to the binomial distribution B(n, m) that is associated to the null hypothesis that assumes that all individuals are equally probable to become CMP leader.

Extended Data Fig. 5 Quantification of leadership based on the statistics of the individual that acted as CMP leader most times.

The red histograms correspond to the χ2 distributions obtained from numerical simulations. The blue vertical line corresponds to the experimental measured χ2 value.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Exit-times distributions.

Histograms of the exit times of uninformed individuals obtained with the two models. a, The results for model P and IN3 are shown. b, The results for model V and IN3 are shown. c, Comparison between the average exit times obtained by using model P and three different interaction networks, namely IN1, IN2 and IN3.The results between IN3 and the other networks (IN1 and IN2) are significantly different. We used the Welch’s two-sided unequal variances t-test to quantify this hypothesis. In both comparisons (between IN3 and IN1, and between IN3 and IN2), the resulting p-value resulted to be p < 0.005. This confirms that the results between IN3 and the other two interaction networks - IN1 and IN2 - are significantly different. This is highlighted with the label ’***’. No adjustments were made for multiple comparisons. Data are presented as mean values ± SD.

Extended Data Fig. 7 Quantification of navigation accuracy.

Scheme of a sequence of CMPs of a group of N individuals. The light-grey trajectory is meant to follow the centre of mass of the group xg. The target is set on the x-axis at a distant position from the origin. The positions labelled as xg(tn) are the positions in space of the centre of mass of the group after n CMPs. Let us recall that each CMP possesses a leader.

Extended Data Fig. 8 Effect of the interaction network for multiple target scenario.

The figure shows the superposition of individual trajectories of 300 simulations using interaction network IN1, IN2, and IN3 and model P.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

The Supplementary Information contains mainly details on the experimental set-up, data acquisition and fitting of the experimental data using the models explained in the main text (model P, model V and model V + P).

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Video 1

Tracking of a sequence of four CMPs extracted from a trial with a group of size N = 4, where each individual adopts the leading position one time.

Supplementary Video 2

Alternating collective motion phases (CMPs) and grazing phases (GPs) observed in the experiments (raw data) for all group sizes (N = 2, 3 and 4).

Supplementary Video 3

Tracking of CMPs for group size N = 4.

Supplementary Video 4

Numerical simulations of the exploration of the maze shown in Fig. 4c in the main text for a group of size N = 4 using model P.

Supplementary Video 5

Numerical simulations of the exploration of the maze shown in Fig. 4c in the main text for a group of size N = 4 using model V.

Supplementary Video 6

Video taken by Richard Bon (Université Paul Sabatier, CRCA, Toulouse, France) of a large group of moving sheep. The video was taken in the Alps.

Supplementary Video 7

Numerical simulations implemented for the generalized model using a generalized network IN3 and a group of N = 40 individuals. The probability of following a neighbour from the back is p0 = 0.8 (Methods).

Supplementary Video 8

Numerical simulations implemented using our model (model P + IN3) and a group of N = 4 individuals. For aesthetic reasons, we included a more detailed implementation for the transition of the individuals between CMP and GP. For the realization of the video, we used a constant time interval between transitions of the individuals in such a way that the leader (rank K = 1) makes the transition at a given time t1, then the first follower (rank K = 2) makes the transition at time t2 = t1 + dt, the third follower (K = 3) makes the transition at time t3 = t2 + dt and so on. This implementation was used for both transitions: CMP → GP and GP → CMP. For the video, we used dt = 5 time units.

Supplementary Video 9

Numerical simulations implemented using a generalization of the model presented in Barberis et al.5, which is a generic model that considers restricted vision angles of the particles.

Source data

Source Data Fig. 1

Data used for the plots in Figure 1b–e.

Source Data Fig. 2

Data used for the plots in Figure 2c,d.

Source Data Fig. 4

Data used for the plots in Figure 4b,d,e,g.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gómez-Nava, L., Bon, R. & Peruani, F. Intermittent collective motion in sheep results from alternating the role of leader and follower. Nat. Phys. 18, 1494–1501 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41567-022-01769-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41567-022-01769-8

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing