Introduction

Understanding the development of a healthy child comes from recognizing societal, environmental, and biological factors that support health and wellness, including physiological regulation. The autonomic nervous system (ANS) is a core physiological system that supports our capacity to navigate an ever-changing world and contributes to the modulation of energy expenditure and/or preservation.1,2 The ANS is co-regulated by two complementary inputs—the sympathetic excitatory system and the parasympathetic inhibitory system that work together to mobilize flexible stress reactivity (i.e., mobilization of energy to meet environmental demands) and recovery (i.e., restoration of homeostasis following mobilization). ANS regulation encompasses these adequate reactivity and/or recovery responses; however, responses may also be dysregulated, such as when reactivity and/or recovery processes may be inadequate to meet environmental demands or may meet immediate demands but increase potential risks for negative long-term physical or psychosocial outcomes. Cardiac measures of the ANS represent a moment-to-moment dynamic assessment of real-time regulation,3 and ANS regulation has been shown to predict a range of outcomes in infants and children, including attachment to parent, occurrence of behavior problems, and psychopathology.4,5 However, despite existing theories and studies of adult populations supporting the importance of ANS regulation in the development of health risks and the pathogenesis of disease,6,7 very few studies have examined these associations across early development, before health problems manifest.4

Bourgeoning research in adults suggests that ANS dysregulation may be one mechanism of disease development and pathological processes associated with immune problems, cardiovascular disease, cancer, and obesity.6,7,8 Of these, childhood obesity is an increasing concern for younger populations because it is highly prevalent in the United States and a risk factor for poor physical and psychosocial health into adulthood.9 Children with and without obesity exhibit differences in patterns of ANS regulation, but the majority of these studies are cross-sectional.10,11 Limited research has explored the development of these differences and whether early infant weight or adiposity gain may be associated with later ANS differences in early childhood.12,13,14

Although only a few reports have examined the development of the ANS throughout infancy, findings suggest that early environments influence both structural development and functional regulation of these systems.15,16 Prenatal and early life stress is an especially salient predictor of differences in physiological reactivity and has been most consistently examined in previous research.17,18 However, preliminary research also suggests that physical systems, such as rapid early weight gain, may also influence ANS development,19 as well as increase risk for future obesity.20,21 Further, to our knowledge, no studies of infant weight gain or adiposity have examined the joint functioning of both sympathetic and parasympathetic branches to assess ANS coordination within individuals, despite longstanding acknowledgement of their complementary, integrated function.

The current study sought to address these gaps in the literature by examining whether infant weight-for-length gain during the first 6 months is related to coordinated sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system reactivity across periods of challenge and recovery in a racially and ethnically diverse sample of low-income children at increased risk for obesity. Importantly, given the consistent evidence that early life stress “programs” physiological systems, particularly in this sample,22 we sought to examine whether weight-for-length gain would be associated with ANS regulation above and beyond the influence of early life stress.22 The study leveraged the Still Face Paradigm (SFP), a gold standard experimental infant stress protocol, to identify infants who demonstrate a “typical” ANS stress response (sympathetic nervous system activation and parasympathetic withdrawal during the stressor) compared to infants with a dysregulated or dampened ANS response. We hypothesized that more normative infant weight-for-length gain across the first 6 months would be associated with a profile of typical ANS regulation during stress and recovery (i.e., reactivity during still face and recovery following still face). Further, we hypothesized that greater weight-for-length gain in the first 6 months would be associated with dysregulated ANS responses (e.g., no reactivity during still face and reactivity during recovery periods) following the still face.

Methods

Participants

The current sample leveraged a longitudinal cohort designed to explore the effects of prenatal influences such as maternal stress and weight gain on child health and development.22 Participants were enrolled from a larger mindfulness intervention study of 215 pregnant women with obesity designed to prevent excessive weight gain during pregnancy.23 Criteria for inclusion from the pregnancy study included that all women were: (1) English speaking, (2) aged between 18 and 45 years, (3) 8–23 weeks pregnant with singleton, (4) have a pre-pregnancy BMI of 25–40 kg/m2, and (5) incomes <500% of the Federal Poverty Level, which is considered lower income given the cost of living in the area. Women were excluded if they (1) had medical conditions that were known to interfere with baseline body composition or gestational weight gain, (2) were currently taking medications related to weight loss, diabetes, antidepressants, antipsychotics, opiate drugs, or corticosteroids, and (3) have received gastric bypass surgery. All 202 mothers with live births were approached to participate in the subsequent longitudinal follow-up study and 162 mothers consented. The University of California San Francisco institutional review board approved all study protocols and written informed consent was collected from mothers before initiation of any data collection with mother or child.

Recruitment and further participant details for the original pregnancy study and the current infant study have been published previously.22,23 A total of 162 (80%) mothers and their infants were enrolled in the current study of children’s health and development. To retain the highest sample size, participants were given the choice to participate in the laboratory or in their home. Due to delays in funding, ANS measures were only collected in a subset of infants (N = 67), and a few infants (N = 7) did not tolerate the application of the electrodes or other aspects of the protocol and thus had no or incomplete ANS data.22 The current study sample included only infants who participated in data collection at 6 months (mean age = 6.4 months, SD = 0.51) with valid, complete ANS data (N = 60).

Infant weight-for-length gain

Infant birth weight was abstracted from the birth record. Weight at 6 months was measured by trained research assistants using a SECA scale (model 383) and length was measured using the Infant/Child Height-Length ShorrBoard. Measurements were repeated twice, and a third measurement was obtained if the first two measurements were incongruent (e.g., weight difference >0.2 kg or length difference >0.5 cm). Then an average of the congruent measurements was calculated for each infant. Weight measurements were used to derive age- and sex-specific weight according to the World Health Organization (WHO) 2006 growth charts.24 These comparisons yielded a z-score of their normed weight-for-length. Weight-for-length change was then determined as the difference between the normed birth weight-for-length and the normed 6-month weight-for-length scores, providing a final continuous score of standardized weight-for-length change over the first 6 months. A weight-for-length change score of 0 would indicate that the infant gained the average amount of expected weight-for-length, relative to the WHO sample, while a positive or negative change score would represent more than the normed average and less than the normed average weight-for-length gain, respectively.

Autonomic nervous system

ANS regulation was measured during a standardized SFP (see https://youtu.be/apzXGEbZht0 for example). The SFP is a widely used experimental paradigm for evaluating infant physiological, behavioral, and emotional regulation at 6 months of age, which has demonstrated validity25 and reliability26 across multiple samples.27 The SFP is designed to elicit infant regulation in response to periods of parental interaction where play is encouraged as normal and periods of disengagement when the mother stares straight ahead with a neutral and unchanging expression. The current study utilized a 10-min double SFP including five episodes: 2-min play (play 1), 2-min still face challenge (SF 1), 2-min play (reunion 1), 2-min still face challenge (SF 2), and 2-min play (reunion 2), which is detailed in prior publications (see Fig. 1).22 A subset of infants (N = 12) did not tolerate the second SF exposure; therefore, to maintain the largest possible sample size, SF reactivity and reunion scores were taken for the last completed SF and reunion episodes. A dichotomous variable indicating whether or not infants completed both SF challenges was included in statistical models to control for potential bias.

Fig. 1: Infant responses displayed in this figure are anticipated responses based on previous research utilizing the SFP.
figure 1

In practice, actual infant responses to the SFP will vary in direction and magnitude. ANS patterns of SNS activation and PNS withdrawal relate to increased heart rate while patterns of PNS activation and SNS withdrawal relate to decreased heart rate.

ANS regulation was collected continuously throughout the SFP using the BioNex hardware and BioLab acquisition software version 3.0 (Mindware Technologies, Ltd., www.mindwaretech.com) via spot electrodes placed on the infants. The full ANS collection and scoring methods protocol have been previously described.28 Parasympathetic nervous system reactivity was assessed using respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA; the naturally occurring variation in heart rate as a function of respiration). RSA was calculated using the interbeat intervals detected from electrocardiogram (ECG) readings, respiration rates detected from impedance waveforms (e.g., dZ/dt), and a bandwidth range of 0.15–1.04 Hz for infants of 6 months of age.29 Pre-ejection period (PEP), an indicator of sympathetic activity, is a systolic time interval representing the elapsed duration from the beginning of electrical stimulation until the ejection of blood from the left ventricle.30 PEP data were extracted and scored using impedance technologies where the ECG and impedance waveforms were used to obtain PEP measures quantified as the time interval in milliseconds from the onset of the ECG Q-wave to the B point of the dZ/dt wave. RSA and PEP data were filtered, extracted, and then scored in 30-s intervals and cleaned by examining for artifacts, checking all outliers (>3 SD), and deleting individual data files if >25% of the 30-s epochs were unscorable.

Separate PEP and RSA change scores were calculated for both the SFP challenge and the reunion episodes. “Reactivity” during the still face challenge was computed by subtracting the last completed still face by the baseline play 1 episode yielding two change variables, one for RSA and one for PEP. On the other hand, “recovery” during the reunion episode following the last still face was computed by subtracting the final reunion episode from the previous still face challenge yielding two change variables, one for RSA and one for PEP. Next, to assess joint ANS reactivity during still face, PEP and RSA change scores were combined. Previous research suggests that a classic reactivity response characterized by sympathetic activation (i.e., PEP shortening) and parasympathetic withdrawal (i.e., RSA decrease) is the most commonly studied response to a distressing challenge.31,32 Therefore, this study focused on understanding this response pattern only, and a dichotomous variable was calculated for those who demonstrated classic reactivity during the still face (i.e., negative PEP change score and negative RSA change score) versus all other types of responses. This process was repeated for recovery scores such that infants who had a paired ANS response of sympathetic activation and parasympathetic withdrawal during reunion compared to the last still face were classified as demonstrating a delayed “classic reactivity” response (placed in quotation marks to emphasize the difference compared to still face responses) versus all other types of dual-system responses.

Covariates

Gestational age, self-reported pre-pregnancy BMI (weight kg/height m2), birth weight, cigarette smoking during pregnancy, and infant sex were obtained via labor and delivery medical records. Participants reported total household income and household size at 6 months, which were used to calculate a continuous score of U.S. federal poverty level.33 At the 6-month interview, mothers reported on their breastfeeding behavior and a dichotomized score was created to determine whether infants were exclusively breastfed from birth to 6 months or not.34 During pregnancy and at the 6-month visit, mothers reported on their perceptions of stress in the previous month, using Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale.35 Postnatally, they also retrospectively reported on their experiences of objective stressful life events (SLE) that occurred during pregnancy.36 Stress measures were maintained as continuous scores, and further detail about these measurements are presented in prior publications.22 A variable indicating whether the infant completed the full SFP and another variable indicating whether or not mothers participated in the prenatal stress-management intervention conducted in the original study (which did not affect gestational weight gain but did reduce maternal perceptions of stress)23 were also included in all models.

Data analysis

Analyses were performed in SPSS version 26. Descriptive statistics were calculated for demographic characteristics of the sample. Preliminary independent samples t tests exploring potential bias were used to assess if individuals differed significantly based on whether they completed one or both still face challenge episodes. Primary analyses examining the association between continuous infant weight-for-length gain and the dichotomous grouping of ANS reactivity utilized multivariate logistic regression testing the probability of observing the classic reactivity response during the still face challenge and reunion recovery episodes separately. Because of previous findings that prenatal stress was associated with infant ANS at 6 months in this sample,22,27 comparisons of ANS regulation across the still face and reunion episodes included both prenatal and concurrent 6-month reports of stress. However, due to concerns about multicollinearity,22 separate models were conducted for the prenatal and concurrent maternal stress variables, resulting in two models evaluating associations with still face ANS and two models evaluating associations with reunion ANS. Original models included all covariates; however, to preserve the most parsimonious model, covariates that did not meaningfully influence coefficients with p > 0.25 were deleted listwise.37

Results

Figure 1 displays a conceptual example of anticipated infant responses across the SFP based on previous research. Table 1 presents demographic information of the whole sample and the sample split by classic reactivity or other reactivity. On average, the current sample had more weight-for-length gain over the first 6 months compared to global WHO averages, (M = 1.19, SD = 1.77). Further, 17 (28.3%) infants in the current sample met the clinical criteria for rapid infant weight gain.21 Preliminary analyses found significant differences in RSA and PEP recovery scores during reunion for infants who completed one versus both still face episodes, confirming the need to include this as a covariate in all regression models to control for potential bias. Mother’s pre-pregnancy BMI, birth weight, intervention assignment, SLE, and poverty were not significant covariates in any logistic models (p values = 0.42–0.87) and were therefore dropped from analyses. Final logistic models adjusted for the effects of either prenatal or concurrent perceived stress, breastfeeding, child sex, and whether children completed one or both still face challenges.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of demographics, family characteristics, and physiology split by classic reactivity versus other types of reactivity.

Adjusted logistic regression models across both the still face challenge and reunion episodes were each initially modeled twice to separately assess prenatal and concurrent stress. The beta coefficients across both models were similar, but prenatal stress was not statistically significant within its model, therefore, subsequent models controlled for only concurrent stress, and results for those models are shown in Table 2. The first logistic regression model showed that, for each one standard deviation of weight-for-length gain above the sample mean, infants were 0.52 times less likely to demonstrate classic reactivity to the still face challenge (odds ratio (OR) = 0.52; 95% confidence interval (CI) [0.27, 0.97]; p = 0.04). In other words, infants who gained on pace with WHO norms over the first 6 months were more likely to react to the still face with classic reactivity. Although not a main focus of the study, of note, higher levels of mother’s reported concurrent stress was associated with 0.38 lower odds of infants having a classic reactivity response to the still face (OR = 0.38; 95% CI [0.14, 0.99]; p = 0.04). No other covariates were significantly associated with classic reactivity.

Table 2 Adjusted logistic regression of weight-for-length change and covariate predictors of classic reactivity response at 6 months.

Second, examining the reunion episode following the still face challenge, infant weight-for-length gain was significantly associated with a “classic reactivity” response such that infants with gains of one standard deviation above the sample mean over the first 6 months were 2.35 times more likely to evidence sympathetic activation and parasympathetic withdrawal during the reunion episode (OR = 2.35; 95% CI [1.03, 5.36]; p = 0.03; Table 2). In other words, infants who gained more than expected based on WHO norms were more likely to react with “classic reactivity” during the recovery period. Figure 2 presents a conceptual comparison of the expected recovery response versus the delayed “classic reactivity” response. No covariates were significant predictors in this model.

Fig. 2: This figure presents a comparison of delayed classic reactivity versus the traditional recovery response following classic reactivity.
figure 2

Importantly, this is a conceptual figure and does not represent actual data.

Post-hoc explorations probing these two models revealed that only one infant who evidenced classic reactivity during the still face challenge also demonstrated “classic reactivity” during the reunion episode. This confirmed that, rather than prolonged classic reactivity (i.e., continuing to react in a classic reactivity pattern across both episodes), the infants who evidenced “classic reactivity” during the intended recovery period (i.e., reunion episode) were exhibiting delayed ANS activation in both the sympathetic and parasympathetic branches.

Discussion

The present study found that, after adjusting for effects of maternal stress and key covariates, infant weight-for-length gain from birth to 6 months was associated with patterns of joint sympathetic and parasympathetic reactivity at 6 months within a racially and ethnically diverse low-income sample of infants at increased risk for developing obesity.38 In line with our hypotheses, less weight-for-length gain was associated with classic reactivity during the still face challenge and greater weight-for-length gain was associated with a dysregulated response of classic reactivity during reunion. Thus results suggest that higher levels of weight-for-length gain in early life were associated with autonomic dysregulation at 6 months as measured using the gold standard SFP infant stress protocol. The current findings contribute new evidence supporting the view that early individual physical states,19 in addition to early adverse experiences,22 may influence developing ANS early in life in potentially impactful ways.

Importantly, the average weight-for-length change of the current sample is more than one standard deviation higher than global WHO averages and recommendations, putting those who gained more than one standard deviation into a clinically high-risk group for developing obesity.21 Therefore, infants in our specific sample who gained one standard deviation less than our sample mean were more likely to have gained normative amounts of weight-for-length during their first 6 months of life. Follow-up analyses (data not shown) confirmed that ANS differences were not associated with stunted linear length growth, further suggesting that the results shown here capture excessive weight gain. Given previous research that recognizes rapid and excessive infant weight gain as an especially salient predictor of later development of obesity,21 understanding the potential mechanism of ANS dysregulation is particularly important.

The SFP is a unique paradigm that enables the examination of behavioral, emotional, and physiological regulation in infants. In context of the parent–child relationship, unexpected disengagement from a primary caregiver causes distress that engages these regulatory systems and increases arousal. Classic reactivity is the autonomic response that is hypothesized to best support this type of infant distress,27 where infants show sympathetic activation and parasympathetic withdrawal, and has been associated with positive physical health outcomes in childhood.39 Our findings suggest that infants who had weight-for-length gain closer to global WHO averages over the first 6 months of life were more likely to demonstrate a classic reactivity response during the still face challenge, suggesting that average weight-for-length gain in the first 6 months may facilitate programming of adequate stress responsivity. Conversely, following disengagement, infants should begin to decrease in arousal as their primary caregiver returns to normative playing behavior during reunion episodes, which is intended to provide emotional and physiological recovery from the stress of the challenge task. Therefore, “classic reactivity” demonstrated during the reunion is potentially indicative of ANS dysregulation early in life. Our findings indicated that infants with greater than expected weight-for-length gain over the first 6 months were more likely to demonstrate this delayed or potentially displaced “classic reactivity” response during reunion. Importantly, the quality of the parent–child relationship may also influence reactivity during this socio-emotional challenge. Evidence from studies using this paradigm suggests that infants of nurturant and responsive caregivers are upset by their disengagement because it is novel, and infants are soothed by their positive interaction during reunion,27 whereas infants of a less responsive caregiver may exhibit more disorganized reactions because care and nurturing are not consistent and/or expected.27 Such differences in parent–child relationships or infant irritability could also influence the quality and quantity of feeding behavior, which may relate to relative weight-for-length gain in these infants.

Previous research examining the links between weight gain and ANS have done so primarily by examining correlates of group differences,10,11 and very few have examined the development of these differences.14 ANS dysregulation has been proposed as an explanatory mechanism of increased health risk and cardiovascular disease progression,40 and similar pathways may be evident in the development of obesity.41 Understanding how early life influences may disrupt the development of these mechanistic pathways are important to fully understanding disease progression and develop interventions focused on prevention. High weight gain early in life may “program” ANS regulatory processes in ways that contribute to potentially problematic adaptation to challenge and calibration of energy expenditure across development.14,42 Importantly, although the current findings suggest an association such that early gain contributes to ANS dysregulation, we were unable to test causation or possible multidirectionality of effects and recognize that the process is likely complex. This early dysregulation may subsequently contribute to maladaptive biological processes across systems that increase likelihood of obesity development. For example, studies in adulthood document links between the ANS and both plasticity of adipose tissue43 and metabolic dysfunction,44 which may illustrate additional mechanisms by which ANS can contribute to obesity.

Although we observed that weight-for-length gain over the first 6 months was associated with ANS dysregulation at 6 months, the single time point of data available for each of the measures precludes us from identifying the directionality of the relationships. Weight change could be driving these associations, but other explanations are possible, such that infants have different ANS reactivity at birth, which leads to this increased weight-for-length gain. However, there is no known and reliable method of assessing ANS reactivity and recovery at birth, which prevents ascertainment of causal directionality. Further, infant sympathetic nervous system function is not easily measured—the current study represents one of a few known studies with PEP reactivity data in infancy. This study focuses on one of the earliest periods when ANS regulation can be reliably measured and points to weight gain at a critical period early in life as associated with differences in ANS regulation. If these effects persist, it could set up a trajectory of higher risk for overeating and metabolic disease.

Limitations

Although the current study contributes new understanding of differences in ANS reactivity in early life that are relevant to young children’s health, there are a number of limitations that provide opportunities for future research. First, although our sample provides much needed data on low-income women of diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds, this may limit the generalizability of our findings to other populations and our small sample warrants replication. Second, our inability to adjust for infant postnatal exposure to household cigarette smoking is also a limitation, although maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy was very low in this sample (0.33%; consistent with low regional rates of smoking, in general), and its inclusion in analyses did not alter findings. Similarly, we do not have data on mother’s marijuana use prenatally or postnatally. Third, reports suggest that using z-score in examinations of children’s height can occlude the observation of catch-up growth that occurs following early rearing in a deprivation context;45 although not directly relevant to the current study, this may be an important consideration of future work on older children in high-stress contexts. Importantly, we chose to focus on the most well-understood ANS response of classic reactivity; however, by dichotomizing reactivity, we may be missing important individual differences captured in other ANS response patterns. Similarly, categorical split variables necessarily eliminate potentially important characteristics of ANS reactivity such as magnitude of response. Given our small sample size, this acts as a good first step in broadly understanding these associations but may be an oversimplification of the regulatory dynamics at play. Since we cannot definitively identify causal relationships between weight-for-length and ANS reactivity, this study is limited in its ability to predict ANS reactivity. Future studies need to replicate this study design and continue to follow infants into early childhood. Finally, ANS reactivity in infancy does not necessarily represent long-term reactivity responses since research suggests that regulatory systems are highly flexible during the first few years of life before stabilizing in later childhood.46 Future longitudinal research should track developmental trajectories of these relations over time as well as timing and effectiveness of various interventions.

Conclusion

Overall, the current study results show that weight-for-length changes from birth to 6 months of life are associated with infant ANS regulation at 6 months, above and beyond other empirically documented influences, such as maternal prenatal and postnatal stress and breastfeeding. If replicated, our findings may be especially relevant to policy and intervention work targeted at prevention of obesity in at-risk children. In addition to identifying infants of mothers with obesity, implementing screenings to detect early adversity, accelerated early weight-for-length gain, and dysregulated physiological responses may detect compounding risks of infants developing future obesity. Further, highlighting physiological regulatory factors that stem from above average early gain may provide additional points of intervention.47 Future research that employs longitudinal examinations from infancy through childhood is required to elucidate the directional effects of weight-for-length gain and ANS regulation as well as inform development of targeted interventions to support infants with potentially dysfunctional regulatory processes.