INTRODUCTION

The dorsal (DH) and ventral (VH) regions of the hippocampus (HPC) are anatomically and functionally distinct areas (Fanselow and Dong, 2010). More specifically, the DH is mainly connected through differential connections with the neocortex, whereas VH is mainly connected with the amygdala and hypothalamus (Moser and Moser, 1998; Naber and Witter, 1998; Segal et al, 2010). On the other hand, the DH would be primarily involved in cognitive processes sustaining learning and spatial memory processes, whereas VH would be associated with motivational and emotional behaviors (Segal et al, 2010).

Glucocorticoids (GCs) primarily act through glucocorticoid receptors (GR) and mineralocorticoid receptors (MR). Both types of receptors are highly co-localized in limbic structures and particularly within the HPC (De Kloet et al, 1986; Van Eekelen et al, 1988).These two receptor types are, however, differentially distributed in the brain (Reul and de Kloet, 1985). Thus, MR are highly expressed in neurons of the hippocampal formation and the lateral septum and moderately expressed in subnuclei of the amygdala, the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus, and the locus coeruleus (Joëls et al, 2008). These regions define a circuit that is involved in the cognitive, emotional, and neuroendocrine processing of stressful events (McGaugh, 2004). GR are ubiquitously expressed in the brain, but mainly in the HPC, the lateral septum, and the paraventricular nucleus (Joëls and Baram, 2009). MR receptors have a high affinity for corticosteroids, so they are mostly occupied even when circulating corticosteroid levels are low, whereas GR have tenfold lower affinity and become more occupied as corticosteroid levels increase—for example, after stress (Joëls et al, 2008).

From a functional point of view, steroids can rapidly modulate neuronal activity through a non-genomic pathway via an activation of membrane receptors (Borski, 2000; Falkenstein et al, 2000; Makara and Haller, 2001; Dallman, 2005; Tasker et al, 2006; Groc et al, 2008; Conboy and Sandi, 2010) and synaptic excitatory transmission (Pfaff et al, 1971; Chaouloff and Groc, 2011). Steroids also increase the release of excitatory amino acids (Venero and Borrell, 1999). In addition to rapid membrane mechanisms, corticosteroid receptors could also translocate to the nucleus, where they act as regulators of gene transcription (Lu et al, 2006; Revest et al, 2010). Therefore, steroid effects on neuronal function usually require at least 1 h to develop and last hours to days. In the HPC, MR activation is a prerequisite for maintaining the ongoing information flow, whereas activation of GR—for example, after stress—causes a delayed suppression of neuronal excitability and synaptic plasticity (Joëls et al, 2007; Kim and Diamond, 2002), thus providing ‘negative-feedback regulation’ of behavioral aspects of the stress response (see Joëls and Baram, 2009).

From a cognitive point of view, it has been shown that GC receptors modulate spatial and non-spatial memory (De Quervain et al, 1998; Kim and Diamond, 2002; Roozendaal, 2003; Ferguson and Sapolsky, 2007). It has been proposed that GR receptors would sustain delayed effects on memory via transcriptional factors (McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995; De Kloet et al, 1999; Sapolsky et al, 2000; Lupien and Lepage, 2001; McGaugh and Roozendaal, 2002; Donley et al, 2005; Joëls et al, 2006; Joëls and Baram, 2009), whereas MR would be primarily involved in the rapid cognitive effects of stress. In particular, it has been shown that MR mediate the corticosterone-induced impairment of memory retrieval (Khaksari et al, 2007). However, the rapid effects of corticosterone on behavior and cognitive processes remain seldom documented (Orchinik et al, 1991; Sandi et al, 1996; Breuner et al, 1998; Sajadi et al, 2006).

We previously showed that acute stress induced a rapid (15 min) corticosterone rise in the DH associated to memory-retrieval deficits (Chauveau et al, 2010), which specifically implicated MR but not GR receptors via non-genomic membrane mechanisms (Dorey et al, 2011). However, as in our previous study, the memory deficit lasted at least 60 min after stress, and because one cannot exclude that stress could activate simultaneously both the VH and DH, we now intend to determine the relative involvement of the DH and VH and the GC receptor types in the rapid and delayed effects of stress on memory retrieval.

To these aims, experiment 1 was designed to measure simultaneously in both the DH and VH the stress-induced time-course evolution of corticosterone rise after acute stress. In a further step, experiment 2 was designed to determine the involvement of the GC receptors (MR and/or GR) on memory impairments associated with the stress-induced time-course evolution of corticosterone rise in the DH and/or VH.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Experimental Design

Experiment 1 was designed (i) to test the impact of an acute stress on the time-course evolution of corticosterone rises both in the DH and VH, measured in the same mouse by microdialysis and (ii) to evaluate the impact of stress on memory retrieval in independent groups of mice at different delays after stress delivery, according to data drawn from the microdialysis experiment.

Experiment 2 was designed to determine the type of GC receptors involved in the delayed (15, 60, and 105 min) stress-induced memory impairments. These delays were chosen according to the time-course evolution of corticosterone levels observed in the DH and VH after stress. The GR antagonist RU-38486 or the MR antagonist RU-28318 were injected into the DH or VH 15 min before stress delivery and memory was assessed at either 15-, 60-, or 105-min post-stress delay intervals.

Animals

The subjects were 6-month-old male mice of the C57BL/6 inbred strain obtained from Charles Rivers (France). At the time of the experiments, mice weighed between 28 and 32 g. They were housed individually with free access to water on a 12-h light–dark cycle in a temperature-controlled and ventilated room. Tests were conducted during the light phase of the cycle between 0800 and 1200 hrs. The number of animals in each group is given in the Results section.

Experiment 1

Surgery

Mice were anesthetized with a ketamine (1 mg/kg body weight)-xylazine (10 mg/kg body weight) solution and placed on a stereotaxic frame. Two microdialysis guide-cannulae (for CMA/7 microdialysis probes, CMA Microdialysis, Sweden) were implanted at the following coordinates from the bregma (Paxinos and Franklin, 2001): for the DH: AP=−2000 μm, L=±1300 μm, and V=−1000 μm; for the VH: AP=−3000 μm, L=±2900 μm, and V=−3000 μm). The laterality of implantation in the DH and VH was randomized as half of the animals were implanted in the right side for the DH and left side for the VH, whereas the other half were implanted in the opposite way.

Guide-cannulae were fixed with dental cement and three micro screws attached to the skull. All operated mice were allowed to recover in the animals room.

Microdialysis

Microdialysis was performed in freely moving animals to measure corticosterone levels in the DH and VH after acute stress. Indeed, in vivo microdialysis is an extremely powerful method to study the highly dynamic neurotransmitter responses in the field of stress physiology and behavior (Linthorst and Reul, 2008; Dorey et al, 2011).

On the day before the experiment, microdialysis probes (CMA/7, membrane length 1 mm; CMA Microdialysis, Sweden) were inserted through the guide-cannulae so that their membranes were lowered 1 mm below guide-cannulae into the DH or VH. Probes were continuously perfused with sterile, filtered Dulbecco’s solution (mock CSF) at a rate of 0.1 μl/min. Equilibration of extracellular concentrations lasted 12 h, i.e., in the night before the test day. After the equilibration phase, baseline dialysates (15-min samples) were collected in the morning with a flow rate of 1 μl/min during 2 h. Then, the acute stress was applied using a footshocks delivery system located in the dialysis cage. The acute stress was constituted by three successive unavoidable electric footshocks (0.9 mA; 10 ms) in keeping with our previous studies (Chauveau et al, 2010; Dorey et al, 2011). The dialysates were collected during 3 additional hours after stress (flow rate: 1 μl/min; sampling delay: 15 min). Samples were stored at −80 °C before analysis. Free corticosterone levels measured in the dialysates were expressed as the percentage of the averaged baseline values.

At the end of the microdialysis experiment, mice were anesthetized and then transcardially perfused in the left ventricle with saline solution (NaCl 0.9%) followed by formaldehyde (4%). Brains were then post-fixed in a 4% formaldehyde solution for 10 days, and then in a saccharose-formaldehyde solution (30–40% v/v) for 2 days. All the brains were coronally sectioned (50 μm thickness). A cresyl violet stain was used to check the exactness of the microdialysis probes implantation.

Intra-hippocampal corticosterone assay

An Enzyme Immunoassay commercial kit (Correlate-EIA, Assay Designs, Ann Arbor, USA) was used to measure HPC corticosterone concentrations in the microdialysates. The sensitivity of the assay was 18.6 pg/ml. Therefore, baseline sample concentration was more than 10-fold above the sensitivity threshold.

Plasma corticosterone assay

Independent groups of mice were used (29 mice as a whole). Mice were placed in the footshocks delivery system located in the microdialysis cage, for at least 1 h before being administered with the electric footschocks used in the microdialysis experiment. After stress, they remained in the microdialysis cage and were decapitated to collect trunk blood after either 15 min (N=4), 30 min (N=4), 60 min (N=4), 90 min (N=4), 105 min (N=4), or 120 min (N=4) post-stress delays. Control animals were submitted to the same experimental conditions but did not receive footshocks (N=5). They were killed 1 h after being placed in the shock delivery system included in the microdialysis cage. Thus, these animals constitute a ‘time 0’ control group. After centrifugation at 3000 r.p.m. for 10 min, the supernatant was stored at −80 °C until ELISA assay (Correlate-EIA, Assay Designs, Ann Arbor, USA).

Acute stress delivery

The same acute stress (0.9 mA; 10 ms) was applied for behavioral experiments. Stress was delivered in a room different from the one used for behavioral testing. Mice remained for 1 min in a stress delivery cage. Non-stressed mice were placed in the cage in the same conditions except that they did not receive footshocks.

Behavioral task: delayed alternation procedure

The procedure and methods have been extensively described in a previous study (see Dorey et al, 2011 for detailed information). Memory was evaluated by delayed alternation rate in a T-maze used as an index of memory performance (Figure 1). Testing was conducted between 0800 and 1200 hrs, in order to minimize the circadian variations of corticosterone.

Figure 1
figure 1

Memory testing procedure: during a session, animals were first submitted to an acquisition phase in which they were forced to enter twice the same arm of the maze (two forced trials). Then, after a 24-h delay interval, they were submitted to the test phase in which they can freely enter either in the right or in the left arm of the maze. An alternation was scored when the mouse entered the arm opposite to the one visited in the acquisition phase. Treatments (stress and antagonists injections in the DH and VH) were delivered before the test phase, according to the experimental schedule described in the Materials and Methods section.

PowerPoint slide

In the acquisition phase, the subjects were forced to enter twice the same arm of the maze. The two forced trials were separated by a 30-s interval. The acquisition phase was followed by a test phase implemented 24 h later. During the test trial, animals remained 30 s in the start box. The door was then opened, and animals were free to enter into one of the arms of the maze. The correct choice is to enter the arm opposite to that entered the day before. In all the experiments, animals were automatically confined for 30 s in the chosen arm by the closing of a slide door. Thus, this confinement period was imposed on the animals. The closing of the slide door was automatically triggered by the crossing of photoelectric cells placed at the middle of the arm, requiring that the subject completely entered the chosen arm. Thus, the closing of the door determines the criterion to score the choice of the mouse. The mouse remained 30 s into the chosen arm before being replaced for 30 s in the start-box for a subsequent free choice trial. This short-term trial was aimed at determining whether the intrinsic psychomotor ability and motivation of mice to alternate were spared by the treatments, so that any deficits observed at the previous 24 h test trial cannot be ascribed to either psychomotor or motivational impairments.

Mice were submitted to two forced trials sessions in the same experimental conditions. The blocked arm of the second session was opposite to the one blocked in the first session. Each session was separated by a 1-week period. Thus, each mouse served as its own control to avoid place preference bias in the alternation performance during the test sessions.

The test sessions took place 15, 60, or 105 min after the acute stress administration.

Experiment 2

Surgery

Mice were anesthetized with a ketamine (1 mg/kg body weigh)-xylazine (10 mg/kg body weight) solution. Stereotaxic coordinates are referenced in μm from the bregma (Paxinos and Franklin, 2001). For pharmacological administration in the DH, two stainless-steel guide cannulae (26 guage, 8 mm length) were implanted bilaterally 1 mm above the surface of the DH (AP=−2000 μm; L=±1300 μm; and V=−1000 μm). For administration in the VH, two stainless-steel guide-cannulae (26 guage, 8 mm length) were implanted bilaterally at the following coordinates (AP=−3000 μm; L=±2900 μm; and V=−3000 μm). Guide-cannulae were fixed in place with dental cement and three micro screws attached to the skull. All operated mice were allowed to recover in the animal room for at least 7 days before behavioral experiments.

Antagonists were injected through two 32-guage stainless-steel cannulae (9 mm length) attached to a microsyringe with polyethylene catheter tubing. The cannulae were inserted into the guide-cannulae. The syringes were placed in a constant flow rate pump (0.4 μl/min). Corticosterone receptors antagonists (RU-38486 and RU-28318; Tocris, USA) at the concentration of 20 mg/ml or vehicle (mock CSF additioned with a small amount of absolute ethanol) were bilaterally injected 15 min before stress delivery in either the DH or VH. The antagonists were injected in a volume of 0.40 μl per side (0.05 μl/min during 8 min). The choice of these doses was already validated according to the behavioral data described in a previous paper (Dorey et al, 2011). Moreover, we already showed that these MR and GR antagonists at the very same doses have no effects on memory in the same task in non-stressed mice (Dorey et al, 2011).

Experimental animals (i.e. animals receiving the MR or GR antagonists injections) were compared with animals which were implanted with cannulae and received the vehicle solution only, either in stress or non-stress conditions (stressed+vehicle or non-stressed+vehicle groups, respectively).

Behavioral schedule

The behavioral procedure was similar to that of Experiment 1. Behavioral testing occurred 15 min after stress. Independent groups of mice were tested for three post-stress delays according to the results obtained in Experiment 1 i.e. 15, 60, and 105 min. Mice were submitted to two forced trials sessions with the same antagonist dose and experimental conditions. The blocked arm of the second session was opposite to the one blocked during the first session. Each session was separated by a wash-out period of 1 week. Thus, each mouse served as its own control to avoid place preference biases. After behavioral testing, mice were killed, and their brains were removed and coronally sectioned (50 μm thickness). A cresyl violet stain was used to verify the exact probe location in the DH and VH.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using the Statview 5.0 software. In the behavioral experiments, data drawn from the two test sessions were averaged and expressed as means±SEM. The data were analyzed using one-way or two-ways factorial analyses of variance (ANOVA) followed, when adequate, by post-hoc comparisons (Bonferroni/Dunnett test). Comparisons of retrieval performances with chance level were calculated with one sample Student’s t-test (with hypothesized mean=chance level of 50%). Microdialysis data were analyzed using one- or two-ways repeated-measure ANOVA as appropriate, followed when adequate by post-hoc testing (Bonferroni/Dunnett test).

One animal out of 10 in Experiment 1 and 5 animals out of 70 in Experiment 2 were excluded a posteriori from statistical analyses because of inaccurate implantation of cannulae for microdialysis or pharmacological injections.

RESULTS

1st Experiment: Effects of Acute Stress on Hippocampal and Plasma Corticosterone Concentrations and Delayed Alternation rates.

Stress-induced intra-HPC corticosterone rise

The absolute concentrations of corticosterone in baseline condition are not significantly different between the DH and VH (251.4±14.1 ng/l vs 275.0±14.5 ng/l respectively; F(1,18)=1.35; p=0.26, NS). Figure 2a represents corticosterone levels in the HPC areas, DH (n=10) and VH (n=10), in which results are expressed in relative concentrations (i.e. as percentage of variation of baseline). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA performed on corticosterone kinetic evidenced a significant interaction between the HPC areas and time (F(14,252)=2.93; p≤0.0004). Thus, the time-course evolutions of corticosterone in the DH and VH are overall significantly different.

Figure 2
figure 2

(a) Time-course evolutions of stress-induced corticosterone in the dorsal (DH) and ventral (VH) hippocampus measured by microdialysis in the same animal. In the DH, the corticosterone rise is significant at the 15-min post-stress delay. The maximum corticosterone concentration occurred at 60 min post-stress delay with a return to baseline level 105 min after stress delivery. In VH, a significant corticosterone rise is observed at the 90-min post-stress delay, and the maximum corticosterone concentration is measured 105 min after stress delivery with a return to baseline at 120 min. Results are expressed in relative concentrations. Comparisons with baseline: for the DH: *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; for the VH: +p<0.05; +++p<0.001; between group comparisons: o- p<0.05; (b) Plasma corticosterone concentrations (ng/ml) at different post-stress delays were measured in independent groups of mice. Plasma corticosterone concentrations are significantly increased from 15 to 105 min after stress with a maximum corticosterone concentration at the 60-min post-stress delay. *p<0.05; **p<0.01 as compared with the non-stressed animals. (c): Effect of stress on delayed alternation performance at several post-stress delays. Stress significantly decreases alternation rates from 15 to 105 min after delivery, whereas no deficit was observed at the 120-min post-stress delay. Comparison with non-stressed group: *p<0.05; **p<0.01.

PowerPoint slide

Intergroup comparisons showed that corticosterone levels in the DH and VH significantly differed for 60 min (197.4±24.7 vs 110.9±7.5, respectively; p<0.05) and 105 min (103.0±10.5 vs 165.0±19.2%, respectively; p<0.05). Thus, our data show that rise in corticosterone in the DH appears to be earlier than in the VH.

Dorsal Hippocampus

As compared with the last pre-stress sample (97.1±6.7%; ‘time=0’), stress induced a progressive and significant increase in corticosterone levels from 15 min (137.6±20.2%; t=2.30; p≤0.05) to 90 min (139.1±24.1%; t=2.32; p≤0.05). Furthermore, the highest difference was observed 60 min after stress administration (197.4±24.7; t=3.57; p≤0.01).

Ventral Hippocampus

As compared with the last pre-stress sample (97.3±4.9%; ‘time=0’), stress induced a significant increase in corticosterone levels for 90 min (129.4.±19.6%; t=2.49; p≤0.05) and 105 min (165.0±19.2%; t=3.82; p≤0.01). Furthermore, the highest difference was observed 105 min after stress administration.

Stress-induced plasma corticosterone rise

ANOVA showed a significant global between-groups difference (F(6,22)=8.83; p<0.001; see Figure 2b). More specifically, stress induced a significant increase of plasma corticosterone level at the post-stress delays of 15 min (53.8±2.4 ng/ml; p<0.05), 30 min (66.6±7.0 ng/ml; p<0.05), 60 min (92.2±19.7 ng/ml; p<0.01), 90 min (78.5±7.6 ng/ml; p<0.01), and at 105 min (50.3±0.65 ng/ml; p<0.05) as compared with non-stressed animals (30.3±3.6 ng/ml). By contrast, the increase in corticosterone at the 120 min (46.2±7.6 ng/ml; NS) post-stress delay was not significantly different from non-stressed mice.

Effects of acute stress delivered 15, 60, 105, and 120 min before memory testing on alternation performance

Data are represented in Figure 2c. The data revealed a significant between-group difference (F(4,90)=6.2; p=0.017). More precisely, non-stressed mice (n=19) exhibited alternation rates (79.1±9.8%) significantly above those observed in stressed ones tested 15 min after stress (n=20; 44.4±8.9;%; p<0.05), 60 min after stress (n=18; 46.2± 13.3; p<0.05), and 105 min after stress (n=20; 35.1±10.7%; p<0.01). By contrast, stressed mice tested 120 min after stress exhibit performance similar to those of non-stressed ones (n=18; 79.1±7.6%; NS). Only the non-stressed and stressed 120 min groups differed significantly from chance level (50%; p<0.01 in each comparison.)

All the groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the delayed test session (92.0±5.5%, 85.6±6.4, 90.7±6.8, 92.1±4.9, and 87.4±5.8 for the non-stressed and stressed groups at 15, 60, 105, and 120 min, respectively; F<1.0).

Second Experiment: Effects of Intra-DH or VH Antagonists Injections on Delayed Alternation Rates

Effects of MR and GR antagonists injections in DH at 15 min post-stress delay

Data are presented in Figure 3. The data revealed a significant between-group difference (F(3,68)=6.3; p<0.01). More precisely, the non-stressed+vehicle group exhibited significant higher alternation rates (n=19; 78.8±10.0%) as compared with the performance of the stressed+vehicle group (n=18; 44.7±8.8%; p<0.05) and as compared with that of the stressed animals receiving the GR antagonist (n=19; 43.5±8.8%; p<0.01). By contrast, they did not differe from the stressed animals receiving the MR antagonist (n=16; 84.1±5.9%; NS). Only the non-stressed+vehicle and stressed+MR antagonist groups differed significantly from chance level (50%; p<0.01 and 0.001, respectively).

Figure 3
figure 3

Effect of GR antagonist (RU-38486 or mifepristone) and MR antagonist (RU-28318 or oxprenoate) injections into the DH on delayed alternation performance in stressed mice tested 15 min after stress delivery. The doses of antagonists were selected according to a previous study (Dorey et al, 2011). The antagonists were bilaterally injected 15 min before stress and 30 min before the test session. Control groups received the vehicle solution. As can be observed, the MR but not GR antagonist reversed the stress-induced decrease of alternation rates. Comparisons with non-stressed vehicles; +p<0.05; ++p<0.01; Comparisons with chance level (50%): **p<0.01; ***p<0.001.

PowerPoint slide

All groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the 24 h delayed test session (82.0±5.3%, 79.0±4.7%, 85.2±7.1, and 84.3±6.8%, respectively, for the non-stressed+vehicle, stressed+vehicle, stressed+MR antagonist, and stressed+GR antagonist groups, respectively; NS in all comparisons).

Effects of MR and GR Antagonists Injections in the DH and VH at 60 min Post-stress Delay

Dorsal hippocampus

Data are presented in Figure 4a. The data revealed a significant between-group difference (F(3,72)=7.1; p<0.01). More precisely, the non-stressed+vehicle group exhibited significant higher alternation rates (n=18; 77.9±9.6%) as compared with the performance of stressed+vehicle group (n=18; 45.9±11.0%; p<0.05) and as compared with the stressed animals receiving the MR antagonist (n=20; 39.2±11.0%; p<0.05). By contrast, they did not differ from the stressed animals receiving the GR antagonist (n=20; 73.2±9.6%; NS). Only the non-stressed+vehicle and stressed+GR antagonist groups differed significantly as compared with chance level (50%; p<0.01 and 0.05, respectively).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of GR antagonist (RU-38486 or mifepristone) and MR antagonist (RU-28318 or oxprenoate) injections into the DH or VH on delayed alternation rates at 60 and 105 min post-stress delays. In all experiments described in Figure 4a-d, both MR and GR antagonists were bilaterally injected at the dose of 20 mg/ml 15 min before stress and 30 min before behavioral testing. (a) Injections into the DH, test at 60 min post-stress: control groups’ animals received the vehicle solution. The administration of GR but not of MR antagonist reversed the stress-induced alternation deficit as compared to vehicle. (b) Injections into the VH, test at 60 min post-stress: the same injections and doses of MR and GR antagonists did not reverse the stress-induced memory impairments as compared with vehicle. (c) Injections into the DH, test at 105 min post-stress: both MR and GR antagonists did not reverse the stress-induced memory impairments as compared with vehicles; (d) injections into the VH, test at 105 min post-stress. The administration of GR but not of MR antagonist reversed the stress-induced alternation deficit as compared with vehicle. (a–d) Comparisons with non-stressed vehicle: +p<0.05; ++p<0.01; comparisons with chance level (50%): *p<0.05; **p<0.01.

PowerPoint slide

All groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the 24 h delayed test session (82.5±6.3%, 81.0.0±8.5%, 77.5±10.8%, and 86.7±7.3%, respectively, for the non-stressed+vehicle, stressed+vehicle, stressed+MR antagonist, and stressed+GR antagonist groups, respectively; NS in all comparisons).

Ventral hippocampus

Data are presented in Figure 4b. The data show that the injection of either the MR or GR antagonist was unable to restore memory performance in stressed animals.

The data revealed a significant between-group difference (F(3,66)=5.7; p<0.001). More precisely, the non-stressed+vehicle group exhibited significant higher alternation rates (n=17; 85.3±5.6%) as compared with the performance of the stressed+vehicle group (n=15; 46.6±9.0%; p<0.01) and as compared with the stressed animals receiving the MR antagonist (n=17; 55.8±8.4%; p<0.05) or the GR antagonist (n=21; 52.3±5.4%; p<0.01). Only the non-stressed+vehicle group differed significantly as compared with chance level (50%; p<0.01).

All groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the 24 h delayed test session (90.8±4.3%, 87.6.0±8.1%, 83.2±6.9%, and 89.7±5.9%, respectively, for the non-stressed+vehicle, stressed+vehicle, stressed+MR antagonist, and stressed+GR antagonist groups, respectively; NS in all comparisons).

Effects of MR and GR Antagonists Injections in the DH and VH at 105 min Post-stress Delay

Dorsal Hippocampus

Data are presented in Figure 4c. The data revealed a significant between-group difference (F(3,72)=6.2; p<0.05). More precisely, the non-stressed+vehicle group exhibited significant higher alternation rates (n=20; 79.0±10.2%) as compared with the performance of the stressed+vehicle group (n=19; 35.0±10.8%; p<0.01) and as compared with the stressed animals receiving the MR antagonist (n=18; 46.5±12.7%; p<0.05) and also as compared with the stressed animals receiving the GR antagonist (n=18; 49.7±10.3%; p<0.05). Only the non-stressed+vehicle group differed significantly as compared with chance level (50%; p<0.01).

All groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the 24 h delayed test session (91.5±6.2%, 79.6±5.6%, 76.6±9.7%, and 87.3±6.1%, respectively, for the non-stressed+vehicle, stressed+vehicle, stressed+MR antagonist, and stressed+GR antagonist groups, respectively; NS in all comparisons).

Ventral Hippocampus

Data are presented in Figure 4d. The data revealed a significant between-group difference (F(3,72)= 9.2; p<0.01). More precisely, the non-stressed+vehicle group exhibited significant higher alternation rates (n=19; 77.1±8.3%) as compared with the performance of the stressed+vehicle group (n=20; 40.8±6.8%; p<0.01) and as compared with the stressed animals receiving the MR antagonist (n=18; 39.1±11.8%; p<0.05) but not as compared with the stressed animals receiving the GR antagonist (n=18; 72.3±10.9%; NS). Only the non-stressed+vehicle group and the stressed+GR antagonist group differed significantly as compared with chance level (50%; p<0.01 and p<0.05).

All the four groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the 24 h delayed test session (F(3,73)<1.0; NS).

Histological Analyses

Figures 5 and 6 show the anterograde extent and the localization of the cannulae tips in the DH and VH of mice submitted either to the microdialysis (Figure 5) or to the pharmacological (Figure 6) experiments. Gray zones: main implantation sites; black hashed areas: antero-retrograde extent of the cannulae tips implantation.

Figure 5
figure 5

Representative localization of the microdialysis probes into the dorsal and ventral hippocampus in mice of Experiment 1. (a and b) Reconstruction of the antero-posterior extent of probe locations (gray area: main sites; hashed areas: antero-posterior extent from the main site). Stereotaxic rostro-caudal coordinates are expressed in mm from bregma. Photomicrographs illustrate the location of the microdialysis probes in the dorsal and ventral hippocampus.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 6
figure 6

Representative localization of the cannulae into the dorsal and ventral hippocampus in mice of Experiment 2. (a and b) Reconstruction of the antero-posterior extent of the sites (gray area: main sites; hashed areas: antero-posterior extent from the main sites). Stereotaxic rostro-caudal coordinates are expressed in mm from bregma. Photomicrographs illustrate the location of the cannulae in the dorsal and ventral hippocampus.

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

The main findings of the study are summarized in Figure 7.

Figure 7
figure 7

Synoptic representation of the main findings of the study. This figure points out a triple dissociation as regards the time-course involvement of the hippocampus regions, corticosterone rises and glucocorticoid receptor types in relation with memory retrieval impairments after acute stress.

PowerPoint slide

In the first experiment, we evidenced that an acute stress (electric footshocks) induced in the DH then in the VH a corticosterone rise measured by microdialysis. Indeed, we showed that the maximum corticosterone concentration in VH occurred 45 min later (105 min post-stress) as compared with the DH (60 min post-stress). On the other hand, we also observed a concomitant memory retrieval impairment in a non-rewarded delayed alternation task. Indeed, we found that stress-induced memory deficits occurred between 15 and 105 min, but were no longer observed at the 120-min post-stress delay. Thus, one of the main results of the study is that, at least up to the 60-min post-stress delay interval, the memory deficit does not depend on corticosterone rise into the VH. In the second experiment, we showed that the stress-induced memory deficit observed 15 min after stress was antagonized by injections of MR but not of GR antagonists (RU-28318 and RU-38486, respectively) in the DH. By contrast, with a 60-min post-stress delay, the GR but not the MR antagonist alleviated the memory impairment. However, both antagonists injected into the DH failed to restore memory performance at the 105-min post-stress delay. In addition, injections of the GR but not the MR antagonist into the VH blocked the stress-induced memory impairment observed at the 105-min post-stress delay.

From these data, two main questions arise: (i) what is the relative contribution of the time-dependent regional involvement of the DH and VH in the stress-induced memory deficits? (ii) What are the mechanisms underlying the sequential involvement of MR and GR within the HPC?

As regards the first issue, behavioral results obtained in the first experiment show that the time-course evolution of the memory impairments is concomitant with the stress-induced corticosterone rise within the DH and VH. Indeed, the findings drawn from microdialysis are congruent with our prvious studies showing that memory retrieval impairments are associated to the DH corticosterone rise 15 but not 120 min after acute stress delivery (Chauveau et al, 2010; Tronche et al, 2010; Dorey et al, 2011). Surprisingly, however, data of the present study show that the stress-induced memory deficit observed from 15 to 60 min post-stress delays does not depend on the VH as corticosterone concentrations in the VH are not different from baseline for this time-window. By contrast, the memory deficit observed at the 105-min post-stress delay seems to depend on the maximum corticosterone level observed in the VH, insofar as corticosterone concentrations in the DH are not different from baseline at this delay. Thus, the different time-course evolutions of corticosterone in the DH and VH could reflect a sequential involvement of both the HPC regions in the observed time-dependent stress-induced memory deficits. Such a view is also strengthened by the observations that GR antagonist administered in the DH is efficient at the 60-min post-stress delay, whereas inefficient when administered in the VH at the same delay. On the contrary, the GR antagonist administered in the VH is efficient at the 105-min post-stress delay, whereas being without effect when administered in the DH at the same delay.

Finally, from a functional point of view, our present data point out a temporal and spatial dissociation on stress-induced corticosterone rise and retrieval memory impairments.

It remains, however, to understand the mechanisms sustaining the emergence of the different time-course evolutions of corticosterone in the DH and VH after stress. Previous data from our team already evidenced the peripheral origin of corticosterone in the DH after stress. Indeed, we showed that systemic administration of metyrapone (an inhibitor of corticosterone synthesis) blocked the stress-induced rise of corticosterone in the DH (Chauveau et al, 2010) and that mice deprived of corticosterone-binding globuline (CBG/ KO mice) did not show any stress-induced corticosterone rise in the DH (Minni et al, 2012). In the present study, plasma and HPC corticosterone were measured in independent groups to avoid blood sampling stress, which could induce biases on HPC corticosterone quantification. We found that the maximum plasma corticosterone value occurring at 60 min after stress is in phase with the one observed in the DH, but not with the one observed in the VH (105 min post-stress). Given that, we could formulate the hypothesis of a diffusion of corticosterone from the DH to VH via a mechanism remaining to be determined. For this purpose, we intend to lesion or inactivate the DH to investigate its role in the delayed corticosterone rise within the VH after stress.

From a functional point of view, it has been proposed that an acute stress would induce a disconnection between the DH and VH and so far could spare or even increase the functional activity of the VH and its main brain targets involved in emotional processes (amygdala and hypothalamus) (Segal et al, 2010). Our data are in agreement with this hypothesis, as the maximum corticosterone concentrations emerged at different post-stress delays in the DH and VH. This suggests a different time-dependent involvement of both the HPC regions in the stress-induced memory deficits. According to our data, however, memory functions sustained by the DH are first affected by the acute stress. Indeed, in the present study, delayed alternation deficits are related to the earlier stress-induced corticosterone rise within the DH, then in the VH. This result is in agreement with previous studies from our group showing that an acute stress delivered 15 min before testing produced memory retrieval impairments in a serial discrimination task. Indeed, we found that the deficit was observed specifically for the information sustained by the DH, whereas the information sustained by the BLA-frontal cortex pathway (heavily connected with the VH) was unaffected (Chauveau et al, 2008, 2009, 2010). Furthermore, the presently observed late involvement of the VH following acute stress is in agreement with relevant studies indicating that the DH would be much more involved in processing cognitive information, whereas the VH has a key role in late consolidation of emotional memory and/or anterograde memory processes (Segal et al, 2010; Fanselow and Dong, 2010).

Even though speculative, we suggest that the late corticosterone rise into the VH could have a major role on anterograde memory processes and more particularly in the consolidation of emotional and/or traumatic memory. Indeed, according to our data, one can hypothesize that the delayed VH activation via GR receptors could be of importance for the establishment of long-term memory or even that excessive VH activation contributes to the development of PTSD. Thus, GR antagonists could be a relevant pharmacological perspective to avoid consolidation or reconsolidation of traumatic events (see Bonne et al, 2004). So far, in further studies using animal models of PTSD, we intend to test such drugs in the VH-dependent emotional memory tasks, i.e. fear conditioning (McEown and Treit, 2009).

As regards the second issue, i.e. the mechanisms underlying the sequential involvement of MR and GR within the HPC, we show that the rapid (15 min) deleterious effects of stress on memory involved first the MR but not GR receptors in the DH. This result confirms our previous finding (Dorey et al, 2011). In addition, lengthening the post-stress delay from 15 to 60 min involves GR but no longer MR in so far as only the GR antagonist is efficient to suppress memory impairments. The lack of memory effects of the MR antagonist at the 60- and 105-min delays cannot be explained by the fact that the MR antagonist is no longer bound to their receptors. Indeed, Kim et al (1998) already reported that the in vivo MR receptor occupancy by RU 28318 was still observed in the rat HPC up to 2 h after systemic injection.

The sequential activation of MR then of GR is in agreement with the view that the rapid stress response involves first MR (Dorey et al, 2011) and excitatory neurotransmitter receptors trafficking through GC action (Groc et al, 2008; Chaouloff and Groc 2011) which contribute to the rapid effect of GCs on memory. GC–glutamatergic interactions have been proposed to explain many of the diverse actions of GCs on cognition (Conboy and Sandi, 2010; Sandi, 2011). Furthermore, it has been suggested that NMDA receptors would be at the interface between cognition and emotion in both the hippocampal zones (Barkus et al, 2010).

For longer delays, GCs act via a classic genomic action sustained by GR (Joëls, 2008). Moreover, given the high and low affinity of MR and GR for GCs, respectively, the sequential activation of these receptors could depend on the hormone concentration at the time of memory testing after stress. However, electrophysiological data, indicating that stress can also activate simultaneously GR and MR in different parts of the HPC, have challenged the widely accepted model, which suggests a time- and dose-dependent effect of steroid hormones on cellular functions in the HPC. Indeed, as shown by relevant studies on the interaction between LTP and LTD regulation, corticosteroid modulation of synaptic plasticity in the HPC is a complex process involving differential activation of MR and GR within the DH and VH (see in Segal et al, 2010; Maggio and Segal, 2012). Nevertheless, our present data agreed overall with the view developed by Maggio and Segal (2012) suggesting that steroid hormones act as molecular switches: by changing the strength of synaptic connectivity in the HPC following stress, they regulate the routes by which the HPC is functionally linked to the rest of the brain. Indeed, our present data show that along with synaptic and molecular mechanisms induced by stress, the different time-course evolutions of corticosterone rises after stress in the DH and VH contribute to the functional switch from the DH to the VH. Indeed, MR are first involved at the 15-min post-stress delay when corticosterone concentration is low in the DH, whereas GR are activated when corticosterone concentration is higher i.e. at 60 min post-stress delay first in the DH and then at 105 min post-stress delay in the VH.

In conclusion, our study using a dynamic approach of the time-course evolution of corticosterone levels evidenced that stress impairs HPC -dependent memory retrieval via a mechanism dependent on post-stress delays (from 15 to 105 min), areas (DH vs VH), and of GC receptor types (MR vs GR). Such a triple dissociation emphasizes the complexity of the stress–memory interaction, particularly as regards the relationships between the DH and VH.