INTRODUCTION

Anxiety disorders are highly prevalent and debilitating psychiatric diseases with a moderate to high degree of heritability (Hettema et al, 2001). Understanding the underlying molecular genetic pathomechanisms and their associated neurobiological abnormalities is a major goal of current research efforts. Neuropeptides have been suggested to have a crucial role in the pathogenesis of stress, arousal, and anxiety. Particularly, the recently discovered neuropeptide-S (NPS) and agonists at its receptor (NPSR) were reported to elicit anxiolytic effects in several studies using rodent models (Xu et al, 2004; Rizzi et al, 2008; Leonard et al, 2008; Vitale et al, 2008), and thus constitute promising candidates for research on fear processing, anxiety, and anxiety disorders (Reinscheid et al, 2005a; Okamura and Reinscheid, 2007; Pape et al, 2010).

Whereas NPSR precursor mRNA is strongly expressed in the locus coeruleus as the major source of noradrenergic transmission in the brain, NPSR mRNA is widely expressed throughout the central nervous system, including the amygdaloid complex, as investigated in rodent models (cf. Reinscheid et al, 2005a). For example, Jüngling et al (2008) described a cell-specific localization of NPSR expression in the projection neurons of the amygdala, particularly in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) principal neurons. Furthermore, BLA activity was reported to be dependent on a NPS-responsive circuitry (Meis et al, 2008). Consequently, intra-amygdala injections of NPS were shown to modulate fear-potentiated startle (Fendt et al, 2010, lateral amygdala) and anxiety-related behavior (Jüngling et al, 2008, BLA). The BLA is the primary input site of the amygdaloid complex and receives wide projections from thalamic nuclei and sensory association cortices. Whereas the central nucleus seems to be essential for the basic species-specific defensive responses associated with fear, the BLA is apparently associated with encoding the threat value of a stimulus (Davis and Whalen, 2001). Taken together, the NPS system appears to mediate specific effects on synaptic transmission to and within the BLA, which is important for processing of fear (Pape et al, 2010).

In the NPSR gene, a single-nucleotide polymorphism has recently been discovered (rs324981 A/T) that leads to an Asn/Ile exchange at position 107 (Asn107Ile), with the T-allele (107Ile) increasing NPSR expression and NPS efficacy at NPSR about 10-fold (Reinscheid et al, 2005b; Bernier et al, 2006). In two previous studies, the NPSR T-allele has consistently been found to be associated with panic disorder, elevated anxiety sensitivity, and increased autonomic arousal (Okamura et al, 2007; Domschke et al, 2010b).

The neurobiological underpinnings of anxiety reaction in healthy subjects and anxiety disorders point to a pivotal role of the amygdala, and amygdala hyper-excitability is regarded as the main neural substrate in the etiology of these illnesses, as an overwhelming majority of neuroimaging studies reported that patients suffering from various anxiety disorders show potentiated amygdala responsiveness to aversive, fear-relevant stimuli (Etkin and Wager, 2007; Sehlmeyer et al, 2009; Domschke and Dannlowski, 2010). Also in healthy controls, several studies reported associations between amygdala responsiveness and (non-pathological) anxiety-related traits (Fakra et al, 2009; Sehlmeyer et al, 2010; Baeken et al, 2010), particularly in the basolateral parts of the amygdala (Etkin et al, 2004).

Given the recent evidence regarding NPS in the etiology of anxiety disorders in humans, and the modulation of (basolateral) amygdala responsiveness already shown in animal models, we sought to investigate whether NPSR rs324981 A/T also impacts human amygdala responsiveness to anxiety-relevant (fearful and angry) facial expressions as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). We hypothesized that the risk (T) allele is associated with increased amygdala responsiveness to fear-relevant stimuli (angry and fearful faces) presented in a robust paradigm, already used in several previous imaging genetics studies.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Seventy-nine right-handed healthy subjects of European ancestry participated in the present study as part of an ongoing project investigating the neurogenetics of emotion processing. All subjects were free from any life-time history of psychiatric disorders according to the DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), diagnosed with the SCID-I interview (Wittchen et al, 1997). Further exclusion criteria were any neurological abnormalities, history of seizures, head trauma or unconsciousness, intake of any psychotropic medication, and the usual MRI contraindications. One male subject had to be excluded for anatomical abnormalities discovered in the structural MRI images (hydrocephalus). Six subjects were excluded for excessive head movement (>2 mm or >2°). The TPQ harm avoidance scale was administered (Cloninger, 1987) and trait anxiety was assessed using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, trait version) (Laux et al, 1981). The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck and Steer, 1987) was used to measure current depression level. Table 1 lists the sociodemographic and questionnaire data of the study participants dependent on the NPSR rs324981 genotype group. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Münster. After a complete description of the study to the participants, written informed consent was obtained. The participants received a financial compensation of 30 €.

Table 1 Task Performance, Sociodemographic, and Affective Characteristics of Study Participants Dependent on NPSR rs324981 A/T Genotype

Genotyping

Participants were genotyped for the functional NPSR rs324981 A/T (Asn107Ile) polymorphism according to published protocols (Domschke et al, 2010b). DNA isolated from venous blood samples was amplified by PCR using the primers F: 5′-GAAGGAAAAAAATTAAAAATGAACCTCCCCAGGATTTCAT and R: 5′-TTCTACCCAGGAGAAAGCGGGCAGTTTGATGCA, resulting in an amplicon size of 353 bp. Standard PCR was performed in a 20-μl volume containing 45–60 ng of genomic DNA, 10 pmol of each primer, 200 mM dNTPs, 0.4 U Taq DNA Polymerase (Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany), 50 mM KCl, 1.5 mM MgCl2, and 10 mM Tris–HCl (pH 8.4). After a 5-min denaturation, 35 cycles were conducted consisting of 30 s at 94 °C, 30 s at 66 °C, and 60 s at 72 °C, followed by a final extension step of 10 min at 72 °C. The amplicons were digested with TasI (Fermentas, St Leon-Rot, Germany) (1 U), separated for 2 h on 15% polyacrylamide gel, and visualized by silver staining. The Hardy–Weinberg criterion, as calculated by using the online program DeFinetti (http://ihg.gsf.de/cgi-bin/hw/hwa1.pl; Wienker TF and Strom TM), was fulfilled (exact test; p=0.48).

fMRI Methods

The experimental fMRI paradigm was a frequently used, robust paradigm for eliciting amygdala responsiveness, which has been used in several previous imaging genetics studies (Hariri et al, 2002, 2005; Pezawas et al, 2005; Meyer-Lindenberg et al, 2006; Zhou et al, 2008; Fakra et al, 2009). The paradigm consisted of four blocks of a face-processing task alternating with five blocks of a sensorimotor control task. During the face-processing task, participants viewed a trio of faces (all expressing either anger or fear) from the Ekman and Friesen (Ekman and Friesen, 1976) stimulus set and selected 1 of 2 faces (bottom) that was identical to the target face (top). Each face-processing block consisted of six images, balanced for target gender and emotion (angry or fearful). During the sensorimotor control blocks, the participants viewed a trio of geometric shapes (circles and ellipses) and selected 1 of 2 shapes (bottom) that was identical to the target shape (top). Each sensorimotor control block consisted of six different shape trios. All blocks were preceded by an instruction (‘Match faces’ or ‘Match shapes’ in German) that lasted 2 s. In the face-processing blocks, each of the six face trios was presented for 4 s with a variable inter-stimulus interval of 2–6 s (mean, 4 s), for a total block length of 48 s. In the sensorimotor control blocks, each of the six shape trios was presented for 4 s with a fixed inter-stimulus interval of 2 s, for a total block length of 36 s. The total task time was 390 s. Participant performance (accuracy and reaction time) was recorded.

The participants held a fiber-optic response pad with two buttons in their right hand. Images were projected to the rear end of the scanner (Sharp XG-PC10XE with additional HF shielding). T2* functional data were acquired using a 3T scanner (Gyroscan Intera 3T, Philips Medical Systems, Best, NL), using a single-shot echoplanar sequence, with parameters selected to minimize distortion in the region of central interest, while retaining adequate a signal-to-noise ratio (S/N) and T2* sensitivity. Volumes consisting of 34 slices were acquired (matrix 642, resolution 3.6 × 3.6 × 3.6 mm; TR=2.1 s, TE=30 ms, FA=90°). The slices were tilted 25° from the AC/PC line in order to minimize drop-out artifacts in the orbitofrontal and mediotemporal region.

The functional imaging data were realigned and unwarped, spatially normalized to standard MNI space (Montreal Neurological Institute), and smoothed (Gaussian kernel, 6 mm FWHM) using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Six subjects had to be excluded due to excessive head movement (exclusion criterion >2 mm and/or 2°). The onsets and durations of the two experimental conditions (faces and shapes) were modeled using a canonical hemodynamic response function in the context of the general linear model (GLM) and the model was corrected for serial correlations. For each participant, one contrast image was generated in each individual fixed-effects first-level analysis comparing activation in response to fear-relevant faces with the shapes baseline. The resulting contrast images were then entered into second-level random-effects group analyses.

We first analyzed whether the amygdalae were significantly activated by the task. Therefore, we used a small-volume correction approach with a threshold of p<0.05, family-wise error (FWE)-corrected for the amygdalae. The amygdala was defined according to Tzourio-Mazoyer et al (2002) and the amygdala mask was created by means of the WFU PickAtlas (Maldjian et al, 2003). In a second step, we tested our main hypothesis of amygdala modulation by NPSR genotype by regressing the number of risk (T) alleles on amygdala responsiveness to fear-relevant facial expressions using the same anatomical mask and statistical threshold.

Finally, mean contrast values from the cluster that was significantly modulated by genotype were extracted for each participant and further processed using SPSS 15.0. We correlated these extracted amygdala activation values with trait anxiety and harm avoidance scores, performed post-hoc comparisons of the three genotype groups, and tested for gender effects.

For exploratory reasons, a supplementary whole-brain analysis of the effects of NPSR rs324981 T-allele on brain responsiveness was conducted at p<0.001, uncorrected, with a cluster threshold of k=20 voxels (Table 2 for results). The anatomical labeling for the whole-brain data was performed by using the AAL-Toolbox (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al, 2002), and the Brodmann areas (BAs) were identified by using the Talairach Daemon atlas (Lancaster et al, 2000).

Table 2 Results of a Whole-Brain Regression Analysis of NPSR rs324981 T-Alleles on Neural Responsiveness to Fear-Related Facial Expressions Conducted at p<0.001, Uncorrected, k=20 Voxels

RESULTS

Behavioral Results

The genotype groups did not differ regarding behavioral data, any sociodemographic variables, or questionnaire measures (see Table 1).

fMRI Results

As in previous imaging genetics studies, the paradigm robustly activated the bilateral amygdala (right: x=28, y=−2, z=−24, Z>8, p(FWE-corrected) <0.001, cluster size k=178; left: x=−30, y=0, z=−24, Z=7.12, p(FWE-corrected) <0.001, cluster size k=114).

The regression analysis showed a strong positive association of NPSR rs324981 T-alleles and amygdala responsiveness to fearful/angry faces (x=36, y=−2, z=−24; Z=3.91, p(uncorrected)=0.00004; p(FWE-corrected)=0.01; r=0.44, cluster size k=6 (Figure 1)). To determine the amygdala sub-region, the SPM Anatomy toolbox version 1.5 (Eickhoff et al, 2005) was used. According to the implemented probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps (Amunts et al, 2005), the cluster was located in the BLA. Mean contrast values for this cluster were extracted for each subject and further processed using SPSS 15.0 in order to compare the three genotype groups among each other and for correlation analyses.

Figure 1
figure 1

NPSR rs324981 T-risk alleles are positively associated with right BLA responsiveness to fear-relevant faces. Left panel: Scatter plot depicting activity of the BLA cluster (middle panel) dependent on genotype. Middle panel: Coronal view (y=0) depicting amygdala responsiveness significantly modulated by genotype (at p<0.05, FEW-corrected for the bilateral amygdala). Right panel: Scatter plot depicting a positive correlation (r=0.37, p=0.004) of amygdala cluster responsiveness and TPQ harm avoidance scores.

PowerPoint slide

For the between-genotype group comparison, we conducted an ANOVA on the cluster contrast values using genotype group as the between-subjects factor. Paralleling the regression analysis, a strong group effect emerged (F(1,71)=8.05, p=0.001). According to post-hoc Scheffe tests, AT and TT carriers showed significantly increased amygdala responsiveness compared with AA carriers (p=0.01 and p=0.002, respectively), whereas no significant difference between AT and TT carriers occurred (p=0.33).

Responsiveness of the BLA cluster correlated significantly with harm avoidance (r=0.37, p=0.001) but not trait anxiety (r=0.13, p=0.27). A multiple regression analysis using BLA responsiveness and NPSR rs324981 T-alleles as predictors of harm avoidance confirmed the strong association of amygdala responsiveness and harm avoidance (β=0.36, p=0.005), with no direct effect of T-alleles (β=0.04, p=0.75), indicating an indirect effect on harm avoidance through amygdala responsiveness. Including the interaction term of amygdala responsiveness and NPSR rs324981 T-alleles into the model did not produce any significant results for the interaction term and almost no changes to the other predictors. Furthermore, there was no association of amygdala responsiveness with depression levels as measured using BDI scores (r=0.09, p=0.46).

Effects of Gender

Given (contradictory) gender-specific results in the categorical association studies with panic disorder (Okamura et al, 2007; Domschke et al, 2010b), we conducted an exploratory correlation of the extracted BLA contrast values and T-alleles within each gender group separately. Our analysis showed significant genotype effects for each gender, with a slightly higher association in women (men: r=0.36, p=0.045; women: r=0.49, p=0.001), in line with the female-dominant effects reported in our previous study (Domschke et al, 2010b).

The whole-brain analysis yielded other structures in which neural activity during fear processing was positively associated with NPSR T-alleles (see Table 2). There was no anatomical area showing a significant positive association with number of NPSR A-alleles.

DISCUSSION

In the present study, we showed a robust effect of a recently described variant in the NPSR gene (NPSR rs324981 A/T) on amygdala responsiveness to fear-relevant facial expressions, with the more active T-allele conferring increased right amygdala responsiveness to fearful/angry faces. This result is entirely in line with previous findings of the more active T-allele to be associated with panic disorder, anxiety sensitivity, and elevated autonomic arousal (Okamura and Reinscheid, 2007; Domschke et al, 2010b; Donner et al, 2010), whereas, quite counter-intuitively, in the rodent model NPS has been shown to act as a potent anxiolytic (see section Introduction). In general, it has to be stated that pharmacological interventions during adulthood do not readily mimic genetically driven alterations during ontogeny. As with the serotonin transporter paradox (see reference Sibille and Lewis, 2006), also for NPS it could be speculated that high NPS levels in early stages of development might have a detrimental effect, whereas at later stages in life NPS might be beneficial with respect to anxiety states. A more specific possible explanation of the apparently contradictive directions of allelic association might be that anxiety is to a great extent conferred through an increased level of arousal (Bouton et al, 2001; Blechert et al, 2007), which in animal models has explicitly been found to be driven by increased NPS activity (Xu et al, 2004; Reinscheid et al, 2005a; Leonard et al, 2008; Rizzi et al, 2008). Increased arousal as conferred by the more active T-allele would furthermore be supported by reports of NPS to cause a significant stimulation of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and concomitant increased arousal-like behavior in rats (Smith et al, 2006). Thus, the present finding of association of the more active T-allele with anxiety-relevant brain activation patterns could be due to its arousal-increasing properties as suggested before (cf. Domschke et al, 2010b).

Numerous researchers have argued that the amygdala has a central role in a neural circuit processing fear and anxiety, and mediating arousal (Ledoux, 2000; Davis and Whalen, 2001; Sehlmeyer et al, 2009). High amygdala responsivity to negative stimuli was shown to be associated with cognitive biases favoring processing of negative stimuli (Dannlowski et al, 2007a). The BLA has been argued to represent the central input structure of the amygdaloid complex and is critically involved in the generation of affect (Davis and Whalen, 2001). Also in humans, neuroimaging studies have identified the right BLA as being the main amygdala subarea associated with anxiety or hyperactivity in affective disorders (Etkin et al, 2004; Suslow et al, 2010). Interestingly, in the present study there was no association with depression level and amygdala responsiveness, potentially indicating an anxiety-specific effect and not a general effect of negative emotions. Again, also this is in accordance with animal studies where NPS agonists had no effect on immobility time in a tail suspension test (TST) suggesting no major antidepressant-like activity (Leonard et al, 2008).

Two recent studies have investigated the effects of NPSR rs324981 on brain activation patterns by fMRI: Domschke et al (2010b) reported significantly decreased activity in the anterior cingulate cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal, and orbitofrontal cortex in response to fearful faces in patients with panic disorder carrying the NPSR rs324981 T risk allele, potentially reflecting a distorted cortico-limbic interaction during emotional processing. Using a classic aversive conditioning paradigm in healthy participants, Raczka et al (2010) in turn reported the T-allele to be associated with increased activity in the rostral dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) evoked by the conditioned stimulus, an area that supports the explicit, conscious appraisal of threat stimuli. However, in both studies amygdala modulation by NPSR genotype has not been observed, potentially either due to a small sample size, a partially medicated patient sample, or a ceiling effect of amygdala activation in patient with panic disorder (Domschke et al, 2010b), or the use of a conditioning paradigm (Raczka et al, 2010), which was not specifically designed to activate the amygdala but rather other structures of the fear circuit. By contrast, our present study used a paradigm particularly designed for studying amygdala responsiveness to fear-relevant stimuli.

We detected an association of amygdala responsiveness and NPSR genotype only for the right amygdala. This might be due to the fact that overall there was also a somewhat stronger activation of the right amygdala by this specific task. Furthermore, at a more lenient threshold (p<0.05, uncorrected) there was a small cluster showing the same effect also within the left amygdala.

Additionally, we could show that the genetically modulated BLA responsiveness to fear-relevant stimuli was directly associated with the participants’ self-reported anxiety-related personality traits (harm avoidance), which is in line with previous neuroimaging studies (Etkin et al, 2004; Fakra et al, 2009; Sehlmeyer et al, 2010; however, see reference Baeken et al, 2010). Albeit we did not find a significant association of trait anxiety with the responsiveness of the investigated BLA cluster, it should be noted that at a more lenient threshold, such as a positive association, was also evident in our sample (x=34, y=6, z=−20, Z=1.89, p(uncorrected)=0.029).

Our supplemental whole-brain analysis yielded other anatomical areas modulated by NPSR genotype in the same direction as the amygdala. Albeit none of these clusters would survive a rigorous alpha correction for the whole brain, some areas are of particular interest due to their widely reported involvement in emotion processing. Particularly, in line with the findings of Domschke et al, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex clusters (DLPFCs) were observed at the uncorrected significance level, as well as right insular clusters, an orbitofrontal area, and activity within the dorsal ACC. The OFC has a role in the mediation of autonomic changes accompanying affective states produced in response to emotive stimuli or contexts, suggestive of a role for this region in the automatic regulation of emotional behavior (Phillips et al, 2003). The association of neural responses in these areas with T-alleles might indicate an increased subjective experience of emotions and autonomic arousal during the processing of the angry and fearful expressions. On the other hand, the DLPFC and dorsal ACC were shown to be involved in executive control and emotion regulation. This might reflect a compensatory engagement in (healthy) risk allele carriers who experience the necessity to regulate their emotions more strongly than subjects with lower amygdala responsiveness.

Some limitations must be acknowledged. We have not genotyped our subjects for other potentially relevant polymorphisms, which could modulate amygdala responsiveness, for example, 5-HTTLPR, 5-HT1A 1019C/G, or neuropeptide-Y (NPY) among others (Dannlowski et al, 2007b, 2008, 2009, 2010; Domschke et al, 2010a). The present effect size of r=0.44 (equivalent to d=0.99) is in the same range as the reported effects of neuropeptide-Y (Domschke et al, 2010a) and potentially even higher compared with 5-HTTLPR (Munafò et al, 2008). However, a direct comparison of different polymorphisms regarding effect sizes is not possible with our present data. The sample size was relatively small for a genetic association study, but on the other hand it was large for an imaging genetics study, exceeding recommended sample sizes for this kind of analysis (Munafò et al, 2008). Nonetheless, our sample size was particularly small for analyzing subsamples stratified for gender and therefore these results should be treated with care.

In sum, our study provides further support of a strong role of NPS and NPSR in the genetic and neural underpinnings of anxiety and anxiety disorders. It provides a missing link between the results of animal studies, human genetic association studies, and neuroimaging results, and integrates these findings. Taken together, these findings might stimulate future studies involving the exploration of therapeutic agents targeting the NPS system in anxiety disorders.