Introduction

The statistical study of the distribution of human refractions has a long and distinguished history. Interest in refractive distributions stems in large part from the fact that human refraction appears to be very different to many other biological variables such as height or intelligence test results, which typically display a normal (ie Gaussian) or log-normal distribution.1, 2 It has been long known that human refraction has a distribution that, in statistical terms, is referred to as leptokurtotic and negatively skewed.3 This is demonstrated in Figure 1, which shows data from Sorsby’s 1953 study of young male army recruits.4 Compared with a Gaussian distribution of the same mean and standard deviation, the refractive data show a great excess of subjects clustered close to the mean and also a greater degree of variation at the extremes. In adult human refraction data there is also an excess of myopes, especially high myopes, which is the source of the negative skew.

Figure 1
figure 1

A representation of the refractive data from Sorsby et al4 and a prediction for the distribution of human refraction based on an uncorrelated combination of ocular components as measured by Steiger.5

What proved particularly intriguing to early researchers in this field was that the ocular parameters that contribute to final refraction such as corneal curvature, anterior chamber depth, lens thickness, and axial length were distributed in a more typically Gaussian manner. Steiger calculated the expected refractive distribution if the biometric components of refraction were randomly associated and this is shown in Figure 1 as the solid line.5 The excess of emmetropes (or perhaps more accurately low hyperopes and emmetropes) in human populations led to the suggestion that a mechanism exists to regulate eye growth so as to minimise refractive errors.6 For many years this process was largely hypothetical, but in recent years a number of longitudinal studies have provided direct evidence for such a mechanism in human infants.7, 8, 9 Animal studies have provided clear evidence of the mechanisms that might drive this process. It has now been demonstrated, in a wide range of species, that the retina is able to detect and use hyperopic and myopic defocus to control eye growth.10, 11, 12

As will be explored in this paper, the combination of a leptokurtotic and negatively skewed distribution of adult refractions arises from two apparently conflicting tendencies. First, the existence of a mechanism to control eye growth during infancy so as to bring refraction towards emmetropia/low hyperopia (ie emmetropisation) and second, the tendency of many human populations to develop myopia during later childhood and into adulthood. The distribution of refraction therefore changes with age. Although the process of emmetropisation does not appear to have changed in the past few decades, the prevalence of myopia has increased dramatically.13, 14, 15, 16 This had led to significant changes in the distribution of adult refractions over time and geographically. Analysis of the processes involved in shaping refractive development allows for the creation of a life-course model of refractive development, as presented below. Monte Carlo simulations based on this model can recreate the wide variation in refractive distributions seen from birth to adulthood and the impact of increasing myopia prevalence on refractive error distributions in Asia.

The development of human refraction errors over time

To understand the relevance of emmetropisation to the aetiology of human refractive errors it is necessary to first define how refraction develops from birth all the way into adulthood.

Development of refraction up to age 6

At birth neonates display a wide range of refractions, which are distributed in the typical Gaussian pattern of so many other biological variables.9, 17, 18 This distribution undergoes a shift in mean and a substantial reduction in standard deviation within the first year.9 During this phase of eye growth there are changes in all the major determinants of refractive power namely: corneal curvature,19 axial length,20 and lens power.21 The reduction in hyperopia is more than can be attributed to simple scaling effects (or passive emmetropisation)22 and appears to be attributable to modulation of axial growth. Like the reduction in spherical refractive error, there is also a marked reduction in astigmatism over the first few years23 that appears to be independent of the change in spherical refractive error.7

Figure 2 shows four distributions of refraction from two different studies9, 24 from 3 months of age to 3.5 years. During this time three separate processes can be observed. First, there is a progressive shift in mean refraction from +2 D to approximately +0.75 D. Second, there is a significant reduction in the standard deviation or variability of refraction. Finally, although at this age the population is still approximating a Gaussian distribution, the subjects falling outside the best-fit Gaussian are predominantly hyperopic (hatched in grey in Figure 2), leading to a positively skewed distribution. These higher hyperopes appear to have failed to emmetropise or to be doing so very slowly. Effectively these hyperopes have been ‘left behind’ as the rest of the population has been regulated towards low hyperopia/emmetropia.

Figure 2
figure 2

Four distributions of refraction from two different studies ((a) Mutti et al9 and (b) Ingram and Barr24) from 3 months of age to 3.5 years. (a) 3–9 months. (b) 1–3 years.

Emmetropisation continues at a slower rate after this early rapid phase and by 6 years of age most populations display a definitely leptokurtotic distribution, although unlike adult populations this remains positively skewed (ie an excess of hyperopes, as shown in Table 1). At this age, the rate of myopia is low even in countries such as Japan that display much higher rates of myopia in older children/adults than are seen in Australia or European countries. The mean refraction is hyperopic in all three studies but it is lowest (closest to emmetropia) in Japan. If emmetropisation is considered to be the process whereby human refractive errors are minimised, then this process would appear to be largely complete in most populations by this age in terms of spherical refractive error, astigmatism, and anisometropia.23, 25

Table 1 Distribution parameters of human myopia in three different countries at 6–7 years of age

Refractive development after 6 years of age

After the age of 6, refraction starts to display divergent patterns of refractive development. In some populations, such as Australia26 and the South Pacific island of Vanuatu,27 emmetropisation appears to continue and the population becomes even more leptokurtotic with a low incidence of myopia and hyperopia. In most populations that have been studied to date, an opposite pattern is observed with an increasing level of myopia leading to increased variance, reduced leptokurtosis, and a negative skew, as opposed to the positive skew observed in younger cohorts. This is most apparent in eastern populations with a high prevalence of myopia.

Figure 3 shows data for 12–13-year-olds from Australia, northern Ireland26, 28 and 11-year-olds from Japan. Whereas the Australian population has a highly leptokurtotic distribution with a low incidence of both hyperopes and myopes, the North Ireland population has retained a greater proportion of hyperopes and acquired more myopes, and thereby leading to a negatively skewed population with less kurtosis than was apparent at the age of 6 years. The Japanese population, although slightly younger, shows the highest proportion of myopia of these three groups. Figure 4 shows the changes in mean refraction and standard deviation of refraction for boys and girls in the CLEERE (Collaborative Longitudinal Evaluation of Ethnicity and Refractive Error) study from 6 to 14 years of age.29 Despite a relatively small shift in the mean refraction over this 8-year period, there is a very large increase in the variability of refraction despite the low rate of myopia development in this study compared to eastern populations.

Figure 3
figure 3

Histograms of the distribution of refraction in 11–13 years in Australia, Northern Ireland, and Japan.

Figure 4
figure 4

Mean and standard deviation of refraction from the ages of 6–14 in the CLEERE (Collaborative Longitudinal Evaluation of Ethnicity and Refractive Error) study (Zadnik et al29).

In the Far East where rates of myopia are rising fastest there is evidence that the shift towards myopia starts as early as 6 years of age.15, 30 What aspect of refractive development is driving the recent rises in myopia prevalence? A study of school children conducted over 13 years (from 1984 to 1996) in Japan provides a clear indication that the increasing levels of myopia observed during this period were not a reflection of any disruption of early emmetropisation.30 Among 17-year-olds the prevalence of myopia increased during the study period from 49.3 to 65.6%, but this divergence only appeared after the age of 5 years, as shown in Figure 5. Certain eastern urban populations now display markedly skewed distributions with a high prevalence of myopia (Figure 6), although the prevalence remains lower in rural populations.31, 32

Figure 5
figure 5

Prevalence of myopia in Japanese school children from ages 3–17 over a 13-year period (1984–1996 inclusive, Matsumura and Hirai30).

Figure 6
figure 6

Distribution of refraction in adult population in Japan with a myopia prevalence showing a highly skewed distribution that has lost the classical feature of leptokurtosis seen, for example, in Figure 1.

The onset of myopia after the age of 6 has been observed to be associated with a greatly increased rate of myopic shift after several years of relatively stable refraction or slowly declining refraction.33, 34 The early phase of progression follows an approximately linear course,35 but slows after several years and usually asymptotes towards a stable myopic refraction. This process has been found to be very well described by a double-exponent model that fits a range of different myopia onset and progression profiles.33 Despite the vast literature on the epidemiology of myopia, the triggers for the sudden initial acceleration around the time of myopia onset and the mechanisms responsible for arresting this process remain unknown.

The phase of myopia development and progression commences in childhood but persists well into adulthood.36 In studies of myopia progression it is apparent that the primary growth response is increasing axial length,37, 38, 39 even when the onset occurs in adults.40

In later life, refractive changes appear to primarily reflect changes in the optical power of the lens rather than axial length. The lens continues to grow during adult life and also undergoes changes in refractive index with time. The balance of these opposing factors gives rise to the lens paradox, whereby the overall refractive power of the eye remains relatively stable.41, 42 In later life the development of cataracts can lead to refractive shifts, typically in a myopic direction.43

If emmetropisation exists, how can we explain the existence of refractive errors?

When considering the aetiology of refractive errors it is important to appreciate that emmetropisation is only one of many homeostatic or disruptive processes affecting eye growth from conception to adulthood.44 By the age of 6 the two principal determinants of refraction are the refraction at birth and the degree of emmetropisation that has occurred in the intervening years. The presence of a significant refractive error at age 6 requires one of the following scenarios to apply: an initial refractive error too great to be corrected by emmetropisation, an initial refraction within the normal range but deficient emmetropisation, or a combination of both of these. Refractive errors that are present at age 6 can therefore be considered as primary failures of emmetropisation.44 The overall variation of refraction and the proportion of significant ametropes are lowest at this age; therefore, it can be concluded that primary failure of emmetropisation is not the dominant factor in the aetiology of refractive errors as a whole. It is however clear that hyperopes and myopes have a different life course. The positive skew of the refractive distribution at age 6 indicates that most hyperopia arises from the persistence of infantile hyperopia due a failure of emmetropisation. The low incidence of myopia at age 6 compared to older ages indicates that the vast majority of myopia develops in eyes that have successfully emmetropised earlier in life. Myopia is therefore most commonly due to a secondary failure of the emmetropisation mechanisms to maintain emmetropia/low hyperopia.

Another factor that might also contribute to the aetiology of refractive errors is stochastic influences on eye growth. Such influences are well described in biological systems and can be manifested at a phenotypic or genetic level.45 The existence of such stochastic factors can be inferred from the existence of anisometropia and, to a lesser extent, discordant monozygotic twins. The two eyes of an anisometrope share both the same environment and genome yet display different refractions. Early onset anisometropes may have an ocular development that is complicated by amblyopia but anisometropia often develops later and is associated with both hyperopia and myopia. Deng and Gwiazda demonstrated that the prevalence of anisometropia declines to a small extent from 6 months (1.96%) of age to 5 years (1.27%). Between 1 and 5 years of age many cases resolve spontaneously and almost as many arise.46 As the variation in refraction increases up to age 12–15, so too does the amount of anisometropia (5.77%).25 Anisometropia is also associated with the magnitude of refractive error, increasing in frequency with increasing myopia, hyperopia, and astigmatism.25, 47 Loss of well-regulated patterns of growth from age 5–6 years therefore manifests as increased variability between subjects and between the two eyes of a single subject.

A similar pattern is seen in monozygotic twins that show increased discordance with increasing refractive error.48, 49 Table 2 shows the variation between refractive error and intra-pair difference in monozygotic twins from Sorsby et al.48 There is a significant association between the refractive error and the degree of refractive discordance with the discordance increasing with absolute refractive error (Fisher’s exact test, two-tailed P=0.016).

Table 2 Variation between refractive error and intra-pair difference in monozygotic twins from Sorsby et al48

Clinical examples of a failure of emmetropisation

Large congenital refractive errors do exist but are rare50 and often associated with genetic disorders.51, 52 Examples of clearly genetic congenital refractive errors include the congenital and non-progressive myopia associated with Stickler’s syndrome53 and Leber’s amaurosis.54 In such cases there appears to be a strong genetic bias away from emmetropia and the large initial refractive errors remain largely unmodified by any emmetropisation mechanism.

Keeping in view the visually guided nature of emmetropisation, conditions that prevent clear vision from birth are associated with a lack of emmetropisation and a broad range of refractions. As is observed in visual deprivation studies in animals, the refraction in such children is shifted towards a myopic mean.55, 56 In contrast, visual deficits that are not congenital but develop in the first 3 years are associated with hyperopic errors.57 Visual deficits such as those associated with albinism and other causes of nystagmus are also associated with impaired emmetropisation and broad refractive distributions.58 Astigmatism is also greatly increased in albinism and one analysis suggests that the vertical refractive meridian, which is unaffected by the motion blur of horizontal nystagmus, may display some degree of emmetropisation.59

There is a poorly understood interaction between amblyopia and emmetropisation. Induced amblyopia in monkeys leads to hyperopia in the amblyopic eye which correlates with the density of the amblyopic deficit.60 The development of amblyopia leads to a failure in compensatory growth to imposed lenses.61 In humans the situation is less clear cut but studies have suggested that anisometropia may be a consequence of amblyopia as much as a cause.62, 63 Amblyopic eyes display different patterns of vitreous chamber growth to the fellow eye.64

Prematurity, even in the absence of retinopathy of prematurity, has been demonstrated to impair emmetropisation in at least a subset of children.65, 66 A more dramatic failure of emmetropisation can be observed in Down’s syndrome despite the good visual acuity usually observed in this condition.67, 68 It has been proposed that the apparent absence of emmetropisation in Down’s syndrome would reveal the underlying pattern of genetically determined eye growth.69 The patterns of refractive development in Down’s syndrome are instead highly variable and display the mathematical features of a random walk typical of a stochastic process.67, 68

A comprehensive model of the mechanisms involved in refractive development

This paper has reviewed the major influences on refractive development from birth to adulthood. These are the initial refraction at birth, the efficacy and duration of the emmetropisation process in the first few years of life, the poorly understood mechanisms of myopia onset and progression, stochastic influences on eye growth, and, more rarely, sources of major genetic bias towards myopia or hyperopia. This allows the creation of a model for the development of refractive errors from birth to adulthood. This model encapsulates each of these processes in a simple mathematical form. Equation (1) provides a mathematical description of this model and Figure 7 provides an annotated explanation of each component and parameter.

Figure 7
figure 7

An annotation of equation 1 describing the biological mechanisms associated with each component of the model (a) and an annotation of equation (1) describing the biological relevance of each component parameter (b).

where, R(t) is the refraction at time t; Ro is the refraction at birth; Eg is the gain of emmetropisation controller; Rs is the refractive set-point target; Et is the emmetropisation time constant; Rc is the myopic offset; a, is the myopic progression shape; to is the myopia onset time; Gn(t) is the growth associated biological noise; Gb(t) is the genetic bias

In essence, this model combines the four components described above. First, the starting point of refraction at birth is captured by the term Ro. Emmetropisation is captured in the simplest possible manner as an exponential model (with parameters for the gain of emmetropisation, the set-point towards which the eye grows, and a time-constant reflecting the time-limited nature of the process). The modified Gompertz formula developed by Thorn et al33 is included to capture the process of myopic progression as it describes this better than any other model to date. The Rc and a parameters of the original model are maintained for ease of comparison but the Rc parameter is omitted as the starting refraction is determined by the first two terms. The biological noise and bias terms (Gn(t) and Gb) are specified as generalised functions, although in general these factors only seem to be dominant in limited range of clinical scenarios.

Equation 1 has been specified to allow calculation of refraction at a given age depending on the value of the various parameters and does not allow for interaction between the different components. This is clearly a simplification but the first three factors are largely independent on the basis that the operative factors have little or no temporal overlap. A more complete mathematical description would specify all four processes as part of a differential equation incorporating stochastic components as this would provide for the interaction between each of the processes. Such a treatment is beyond the scope of the current paper.

This model allows the simulation of the distribution of human refraction from birth into adulthood and each of the parameters can be estimated from existing clinical studies. To model a population, each of these parameters can be subjected to random variation using either a Gaussian or Beta probability function, and once again existing studies provide a basis for estimating such parameters. The following Monte Carlo simulations are based on 20 000 subjects with the parameters given in Table 3 and performed using custom Matlab (Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA, USA) functions that are available from the author on request. With the exception of the variation in the gain of emmetropisation (Eg), the distributions chosen for the parameters listed in Table 3 were based on published data for such parameters33 or estimated from human distribution data from a variety of sources. As the stochastic and bias elements are dominant only in pathological situations, these elements have not been included in the following simulations.

Table 3 Parameters and distribution models used for Monte Carlo simulations of refractive developments shown in Figures 8 and 9

To create the behaviour observed in human populations it proved necessary to divide the population into a proportion with a high gain of emmetropisation (90–95% of the population) and a corresponding proportion with low gain. The variation between modern day Australian distributions (eg Figure 3), historic UK distributions (eg Figure 1), young Asian and older Asian distributions (eg Figures 3 and 6) could be created by varying the proportion of those who undergo later myopic progression (ie those who are both genetically sensitive and exposed to myopic environmental triggers). This proportion has minimal impact on refractive development in this model up to the age of 6 because of the observed age range of the to parameter (myopia onset parameter), but represents a dominant shape factor at older ages.

Figure 8 shows the results of Monte Carlo simulations from 3 months of age up to 6 years. The evolution of the refractive distribution from a normally distributed population with wide variation to a positively skewed leptokurtotic population closely mirrors that seen in population studies. Figure 9 extends these models from age 6 years up to age 24 years. The left-hand graphs on this figure have set 15% of the population to be susceptible to myopic progression and the right hand graphs 55%. Both in terms of increasing myopia prevalence (<−0.5 D) and the shape of the distribution these graphs also mirror the statistics of refractive distributions in low and high myopia prevalence populations.

Figure 8
figure 8

Results of Monte Carlo simulations of human refractive development from 3 months of age to 6 years.

Figure 9
figure 9

Results of Monte Carlo simulations of human refractive development from 8 years of age to 24 years. The left-hand panels model a population with a low tendency to develop myopia and the right-hand panels a population with a high tendency to develop myopia.

Discussion

This analysis of emmetropisation and the development of refractive errors are intended to provide a framework for a more rational discussion and approach to refractive errors. Since refractive error is the end result of a long, complex growth process over several decades, it is no longer valid to consider refraction in the same manner as a simple trait. Adult hyperopes and myopes errors have quite different life courses in terms of ocular development and would appear to be the result of fundamentally different processes. Furthermore, any given refractive error can be the result of a wide variety of influences. These influences include the following: where in the refractive distribution range of an eye starts at birth; the effectiveness (ie gain) and set-point of emmetropisation; the impact of stochastic factors when emmetropisation is deficient; the susceptibility to later myopiogenic factors; the exposure to such factors; and the regulation of the adolescent phase of ocular growth in axial length and lens power. Therefore, rather than accepting a single figure for the heritability of refraction we should be asking what aspects of this process are genetically determined, what aspects are essentially random (stochastic) and what aspects are influenced by visual experience or other environmental factors.

The model presented in this paper is based on well-defined, if not fully understood, phenomena within refractive development. That a single model can provide a mechanistic explanation of both the evolution of refractive distributions since birth through childhood into adulthood and the variations in refractive distribution shape seen in different adult populations is a testament to validity of the underlying concepts. The parameters also have clinical relevance and are, in the most part, measurable. The question of examining genetic and environmental contributions to refractive error may become more tangible if the different aspects of the refractive life-course encapsulated within this model and its parameters are considered in isolation.

Although the term emmetropisation is often used to describe the process where older hyperopes ‘grow out of their glasses’,70 it is clear from this review that true emmetropisation occurs early in life. The process of growing out of their glasses, that is observed in some but by no means all childhood hyperopes, occurs at the same age that myopia is starting to emerge. Is this late hyperopic ‘emmetropisation’ merely another manifestation of the processes driving myopia onset/progression or is it an entirely different process. This is an interesting and unanswered question. If it were true and the factors driving myopia onset and progression could be determined, then such factors could be used to develop novel management strategies for the hyperopia and accommodative esotropia.

There is clear evidence from myopia intervention studies that the growth of the older human eye is sensitive to optical defocus.71, 72, 73 If the human eye remains sensitive to defocus why does the later phase of eye growth lead to refractive errors, in particular myopia? An intriguing suggestion is that, as shown in tree shrews, the older eye loses the ability to respond to myopic defocus that might slow or halt eye growth but continues to be sensitive to hyperopic defocus that promotes axial elongation.69

It seems reasonable to assume that human infant emmetropisation reflects the optically guided growth mechanisms that have been identified in lens-rearing studies in animals. Both are most active early in life74 and human infants display other features that are predicted by an optically guided process.75, 76 Partial hyperopic correction in infants does not seem to impair the end result of emmetropisation though it does appear to slow the process.77 In keeping with the predictions of an optically guided control model, the rate of emmetropisation has been reported to be correlated with the magnitude of the initial refractive error.78 We therefore have a good animal model for emmetropisation but we do not have an animal that helps us understand how, in later childhood, eyes undergo a rapid refractive acceleration in the direction of myopia. Until we can define and understand the triggers and growth mechanisms mediating this initial acceleration and subsequent stabilisation, we cannot claim to explain the aetiology of the vast majority of myopia.

Conclusion

The bulk of emmetropisation occurs in early childhood and is largely complete by age 6. Therefore, refractive errors that exist at this age can be considered failures of emmetropisation. The commonest refractive error at age 6 is hyperopia with both anisometropia and myopia being far less common at this age. Since the prevalence of myopia shows a marked increase in later years, only a very small proportion of myopic refractive errors can be attributed to a primary failure of emmetropisation. Therefore, an understanding of how and why emmetropisation fails will be of particular importance in understanding hyperopia rather than myopia. Anisometropia remains the least understood refractive abnormality and a fuller understanding may require the addition of chance (ie stochastic factors) to the traditional pair of nature and nurture. When considering the aetiology of refractive errors it is no longer tenable to consider refraction as a trait without considering the developmental processes involved. It is hoped that the model presented in this paper may be of assistance in bringing together different aspects of eye growth. While myopia has public health implications in the adult population,79 within paediatric ophthalmology it is hyperopia and anisometropia that create the greatest morbidity. Far less attention has been devoted to understanding how these conditions may be manipulated biologically than what has been given to myopia treatments. This is an imbalance that merits redress.