INTRODUCTION

Having sexual desires carries important reproductive benefit, but when sexual urges become excessive, as in hyperactive sexual desire, this can result in sexual risk taking or sexually abusive behavior. Hyperactive sexual desire can cause marked personal distress and marital discord, and hypersexuality is a serious social problem when it develops into sexually abusive behavior. Very little is known about causes of hyperactive sexual desire, and empirically validated treatments are lacking. Insight into the underlying mechanisms of sexual motivation is essential to understand hypersexuality, and is needed to guide prevention as well as psychological and/or pharmacological treatment thereof.

According to incentive motivation models, sexual motivation is the result of the activation of a sexual response system by sexual stimuli (incentives) (Singer and Toates, 1987; Stewart, 1995; Agmo, 1999; Both et al, 2007). Sensitivity of the sexual system, moderated by neurotransmitters and hormone levels in the body and the brain, is necessary for sexual motivation to emerge. As incentive motivation models imply, sexual stimuli play an essential role in eliciting sexual desire and behavior. A crucial question is, especially in a society that is flooded with sexual cues, what is it that makes sexual stimuli lead to exaggerated wanting or craving in some but not others?

One important neurobiological link was made between aberrant sexual reward-seeking behavior and dopamine (DA) after the discovery that dopaminergic therapy in Parkinson's disease can result in hypersexuality, and in abnormal stereotypical behaviors like excessive shopping, punding, or gambling (Evans et al, 2009). The impulsive–compulsive behaviors in patients treated for Parkinson's disease are hypothesized to reflect dopaminergic sensitization to rewards, similar to that proposed for drug addiction (Robinson and Berridge, 1993). Conscious processing of both primary rewards such as food and sex, but also of secondary rewards such as money, is usually associated with enhanced activations in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), the central brain structure of the ‘reward system’. This system is driven by the release of DA into the NAcc (Kringelbach and Berridge, 2009), which is thought to be involved in tagging stimuli with an incentive salience, making these stimuli ‘wanted’ (Berridge and Robinson, 1998). The incentive sensitization theory posits that as a result of dopaminergic neural adaptations, the DA system becomes hyper sensitive to rewarding stimuli, which in turn become hypersalient, not by increased liking, but by increased ‘wanting’. Consistent with this theory, DA release in the ventral striatum following exposure to reward-related cues was higher in Parkinson patients with impulsive–compulsive behaviors, compared with Parkinson patients without impulsive–compulsive behaviors, which also suggests an individual vulnerability to becoming sensitized (O’Sullivan et al, 2011).

Dopaminergic modulation of conscious reward processing has been reported (Pessiglione et al, 2006; Pleger et al, 2009). ‘Wanting’, however, is not a consciously experienced wanting, but refers to the underlying implicit motivation process that drives behavior towards the rewarding target (Berridge and Robinson, 2003). Implicit reward processes can be assessed, and are of great informational value as they are unbiased by cognitive incentive, that is, subjective liking or disliking, desires, shame, positive, or negative outcome evaluations, which is especially relevant when presenting stimuli with sexual content. Typically, the reward system already responds to potentially rewarding sex- and drug-related stimuli that are presented outside awareness (Childress et al, 2008; Gillath and Canterberry, 2011). However, it is unknown whether DA modulates implicit motivation for sexual reward, at its earliest onset, outside awareness.

In this study, we therefore investigated whether DA modulates the response of the reward system to subconsciously processed sexual stimuli. We expected that increasing DA levels with a DA agonist would enhance, whereas inhibiting dopaminergic tone with a DA antagonist would decrease the activity in brain regions of the reward system, especially in the NAcc, caudate, insula, thalamus, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC) (Haber and Knutson, 2010).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Healthy, sexually active male volunteers from the general population were recruited by means of advertisements. The eligibility criteria were: no current (or history of) sexual complaints as determined by the International Index of Erectile Function (IIEF; Rosen et al, 1997) or psychiatric problems as determined by the Amsterdam Biographical interview (Wilde, 1963) and the MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al, 1998); a heterosexual orientation, and no history of sexual abuse; no medical illness (or medical history), indicating a risk in using haloperidol or levodopa (eg, cardiac illness, depression, thyroid disorders, glaucoma); no use of medication affecting sexual response; and no current or recent use (<12 weeks before participation) of psycho-pharmacological medication, psychotropic drugs, or medication that might interfere with haloperidol or levodopa (eg, cannabis or cocaine).

A total of 55 participants were included and randomly assigned to one of three experimental groups (L-dopa, haloperidol, or placebo) in a randomized, double-blind experimental design. Participants received a fixed dose of 100 mg levodopa combined with 25 mg of carbidopa (Sinemet, Tmax=45 min, half-time=1–2 h; Sagar and Smyth, 2000; Khor and Hsu, 2007), or haloperidol (3 mg, Tmax=3–6 h, half-time=14–36 h; Midha et al, 1989; Liem-Moolenaar et al, 2010), or placebo. A PET study in healthy volunteers demonstrated that a single dose of Sinemet changes DA levels in the putamen and caudate 1 h after intake (Kumakura et al, 2004). Similarly, for haloperidol, a PET study in healthy volunteers showed 60–70% D2 receptor occupancy 3 h after administration (Nordstrom et al, 1992).

All tablets were over-encapsulated by the hospital pharmacy to ensure that both participants and experimenters could not compare or identify the drugs. Randomization was carried out by the hospital pharmacy. Each participant gave signed informed consent in which confidentiality, anonymity, and the opportunity to withdraw without penalty were assured. The study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Leiden University Medical Center and carried out according to the standards of the Declaration of Helsinki (Declaration of Helsinki, 2000). One participant was excluded from the analyses because of severe movement during scanning, and one participant dropped out after entering the scanner room. The final sample thus consisted of 53 participants (see Table 1 for subject variables in each group on age, body mass index, scores on psychoneuroticism as assessed with the Symptom Checklist-90 (Arrindell and Ettema, 1986); behavioral inhibition as assessed with the Behavioral Inhibition Behavioral Activation Scale Score (Carver and White, 1994); scores on impulsiveness using the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (Patton et al, 1995); and sexual excitation and inhibition with Sexual Excitation and Sexual Inhibition Scales (Janssen et al, 2002); and finally, sexual arousal and anxiety using the Sexual Arousability Index (Hoon and Chambless, 1998)).

Table 1 Means (M) and Standard Deviations (SD) of Subject Variables and ANOVA Results

Materials

A backward-masking task was shown during fMRI scanning, which consisted of 160 trials with targets from four picture categories: sexual, emotionally negative, neutral, and fixation. Each trial started with a target picture with a duration of 26 ms. (A duration of 33 ms is generally used in this paradigm (eg, Carlson et al, 2010; Childress et al, 2008), and is considered ‘unseen’. However, we noticed in a pilot study that specifically sexual stimuli are still detectable at a 33 ms duration, whereas at 26 ms, emotionally negative, neutral, and sexual pictures were all detected at below chance level.) The target was immediately followed by a mask, which was always a neutral picture (duration=474 ms). There was an inter-trial interval showing a gray fixation cross with a random duration between 0.5 and 2 s for jitter (see Figure 1). Under these conditions, participants see the masks, but the masked targets escape visual recognition and remain ‘subconscious’. Neutral and emotionally negative pictures were selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Lang et al, 2001), based on nine-point Likert SAM scales ratings of valence and arousal (neutral targets, M±SD: valence=5.13±1.24; arousal=3.07±1.97; emotionally negative targets: valence=2.04±1.44; arousal=6.18±2.27; neutral masks: valence=5.04±1.30; arousal=3.03±1.86). Neutral targets depicted humans, whereas the neutral masks were non-human (eg, an office cupboard). Emotionally negative targets depicted partly naked or naked humans, for example, mutilated bodies. Sexual targets were selected from picture sets previously used in sexology research and depicted partly naked or naked humans in a heterosexual erotic context (Spiering et al, 2003; Both et al, 2004).

Figure 1
figure 1

Backward-masking task. Note: 160 trials were randomly presented containing 26-ms targets from four categories (sex, emotionally negative, neutral, depicting humans, and a fixation) that were masked by 474-ms neutral (inanimate) pictures. Under these conditions, the targets escape conscious perception. The figure depicts an example of a sex trial and a neutral trial.

PowerPoint slide

All trials were randomly presented, and targets and masks within each category were randomly presented. The duration of the entire task was approximately 6 min. Stimuli were presented in an 800 × 600 pixel resolution, back-projected on a screen located at the end of the scanner bore via an LCD projector located outside the scanner room. Subjects viewed stimuli on a screen through a mirror located on the head coil. Stimulus software (E-prime 1.2; Psychology Software Tools) was used for stimulus presentation.

To confirm that participants were not able to consciously detect the targets, a forced-choice category identification task was delivered after scanning. All trials were presented again, this time however, following each trial the participant had to indicate whether the target preceding the mask was a neutral, sexual, or emotionally negative picture.

Scan protocol

Imaging was carried out on a 3 T Philips Achieva MRI scanner (Philips, Best, The Netherlands), using an 8-channel SENSE head coil. A standard T1-weighted structural volume and a high-resolution gradient echo planar image (EPI) scan were acquired for registration purposes. For fMRI during the backward-masking task, T2*-weighted gradient EPI sensitive to BOLD contrast were obtained in the axial direction (echo time 30 ms, flip angle 80°, isotropic voxels of 2.75 mm, 0.25 mm slice gap, 38 slices, repetition time 2.2 s).

Procedure

Upon arrival, the details of the experimental procedure were given, and informed consent was obtained. Because levodopa reaches peak-plasma concentration within 1 h after intake, whereas haloperidol reaches its peak 4 h after ingestion, participants always ingested two capsules, the first 4 h and the second 1 h before fMRI scanning, to ensure the concurrence of peak-plasma concentrations of both drugs during scanning. If a participant was assigned to the levodopa group, the first capsule contained placebo, whereas the second capsule contained levodopa. In the haloperidol group, the first capsule contained haloperidol, whereas the second capsule contained placebo. In the placebo group, a placebo was given twice (see also Pessiglione et al (2006) and Pleger et al (2009) for this administration protocol).

After ingestion of the first capsule, participants filled out questionnaires (see Table 1). They were allowed to read during the waiting period until ingestion of the second capsule. Exactly 1 h after ingesting the second capsule, the scanning started. Participants were instructed to watch attentively, and keep their eyes focused on the middle of the screen. After scanning, participants made the forced-choice categorization task on a computer. Next, an exit interview was administered in which participants were asked about their sentiments with regard to the experimental procedure. Finally, participants were thanked and paid for their participation and advised to refrain from alcohol and drug use the next 24 h.

Data Processing and Analysis

FMRI data processing was carried out using FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool) Version 4.1, part of FSL (FMRIB's Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl. The following pre-statistics processing was applied: motion correction (Jenkinson et al, 2002); non-brain removal (Smith, 2002); spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 8 mm; grand-mean intensity normalization of the entire 4D data set by a single multiplicative factor; high-pass temporal filtering (Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight line fitting, with σ=50.0 s). Time-series statistical analysis was carried out with local autocorrelation correction (Woolrich et al, 2001). FMRI EPI data were registered to the high-resolution EPI scan of each participant, which was registered to the individual T1-weighted structural scan, which was registered to the MNI-152 standard space template (Jenkinson and Smith, 2001; Jenkinson et al, 2002). Four explanatory variables (EVs) were included in the general linear model, representing the four target categories: neutral (Neu), sexual (Sex), emotionally negative (Neg), and fixation (Fix), each time-locked to the target onset, until mask offset. Each EV was convolved with a double gamma hemodynamic response function to account for the hemodynamic response. Contrasts of interest were Sex vs Fix; Neg vs Fix; Sex vs Neu; and Neg vs Neu. For whole brain analysis, the images of contrasts of parameter estimates and corresponding variances were fed into a higher-level mixed-effects analysis, carried out with FLAME (FMRIB's Local Analysis of Mixed Effects) (Woolrich et al, 2004; Beckmann et al, 2003). To determine main task effects, irrespective of group assignment, a one-sample t-test was carried out. Whole brain Z (Gaussianized T) statistic images were thresholded by an initial cluster-forming threshold of Z>2.3 and a (corrected) cluster significance threshold of p=0.05. Then, the linear contrast (levodopa>placebo>haloperidol) was analyzed with independent ROI analyses, by masking the Z-stat images of the four task contrasts in the regions of interest before thresholding. Masks were binarized images of the NAcc, insula, dACC, thalamus, OFC, and caudate from the Harvard–Oxford Cortical and Subcortical Probability Atlas, set at a probability of 50%. For the dACC, the subgenual part of the ACC was removed at MNI coordinate y=32 (McCormick et al, 2006). Thresholding was performed using GRF-theory-based maximum height thresholding, with a corrected significance threshold of p=0.05 (Worsley, 2001).

RESULTS

There was no relation between the medication the participants had received and the percentage that correctly guessed what they had received (Fisher's exact test=8.29, p=0.16), suggesting that blinding was adequate. Most participants reported no side effects (n=41). Among the 12 participants who did have side effects, the most commonly reported ones were strange feeling in limbs, nausea, headache, dizziness, or strange vision. There were no differences in reported side effects across the three groups (Fisher's exact test=3.98, p=0.42).

The forced-choice category identification task delivered after scanning showed that mean forced-choice category identification was below chance level for all categories, indicating that participants were not able to discriminate the targets (see Table 2 for means and standard deviations). A repeated measures ANOVA was performed with Group (haloperidol vs placebo vs levodopa) as a between-subjects factor, Category (Neutral, Sex, Negative) as a within-subjects factor, and task response as the dependent variable. This showed that there were no significant differences in mean correct identification between the categories (F(1.77; 88.45)=0.29, p=0.72, Greenhouse–Geisser corrected), no significant differences between the groups (F(1, 50)=0.08, p=0.92), and no significant interactions (F(3.53; 88.45)=0.39, p=0.79, Greenhouse–Geisser corrected).

Table 2 Means (M) Percentage Correct Recognition and Standard Deviations (SD) on the Forced-Choice Category Identification Task

Whole Brain Analysis

See Figure 2 for the main effects of task in the contrast Sex vs Fix. In this contrast, several clusters were significantly activated with peak values in the insula, OFC, and paracingulate gyrus and in more posterior regions, such as the (bi)lateral occipital cortex (see Table 3 for significant clusters and local maxima). The largest cluster had its peak in the insula and extended into frontal operculum cortices, caudate, thalamus, and bilateral NAcc. A cluster with its peak in the paracingulate gyrus encompassed local maxima in the dACC. The contrast Sex vs Neu showed significant clusters (see Table 3) in OFC, inferior lateral occipital cortex, and inferior frontal gyrus, with local maxima in the insula and caudate encompassing NAcc. In the contrast Neg vs Fix, two significant clusters were found: one in the lingual gyrus and one that extended from frontal operculum cortex into the left insular cortex. No significant clusters were found in the contrast Neg vs Neu. The results from the latter two contrasts suggest that the (masked) sexual stimuli were specific in inducing activation in reward-related brain areas.

Figure 2
figure 2

Main effect of the contrast Sex>Fix. Note: (a) Coronal, (b) sagittal, and (c) axial view of clusters of voxels (Z>2.3, p=0.05, cluster-corrected) when contrasting Sex vs Fix (MNI coordinates, x, y, z=3, 9, 1). Intensity values in this thresholded zstat map range from 2.3 (red) to 5 (yellow). Voxel size=2 mm3 in standard space.

PowerPoint slide

Table 3 Cluster List of Significant Main Effects of Task

ROI Analysis

With independent ROI analyses, the linear contrast Group (levodopa>placebo>haloperidol) was analyzed, to test the hypothesis that subconscious stimulation of the reward system would be enhanced by DA activation and suppressed by DA inhibition. In Sex vs Fix, this contrast was significant (p<0.05, voxel-corrected) in both bilateral NAcc and dACC (see Figure 3). In Sex vs Neu, the linear contrast was significant in the right NAcc, but not in other ROIs. There were no significant differences in activation in the ROIs in the remaining contrasts.

Figure 3
figure 3

Contrasting Sex>Fix in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). Note: The thresholded zstat maps depict coronal view of voxels (voxel size=2 mm3 in standard space) significantly more active in the linear contrast levodopa>placebo>haloperidol (voxel-wise corrected, p<0.05) in the (a) NAcc and (b) dACC. Plots depict Z-values (mean group±SEM) from all groups extracted from these voxels.

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

This study was aimed at investigating the modulatory effects of DA on the processing of subconsciously perceived sexual stimuli. Our findings showed that DA stimulates activity in key brain areas of the reward system, the NAcc and dACC, in response to subconsciously perceived sexual stimuli. These results provide the first evidence for pharmacological modulation of implicit sexual reward processes, pointing at the possibility for DA to affect sexual motivation at its earliest onset, that is, outside awareness. These findings, again, emphasize the sensitivity of the brain to signals for sexual reward, even when these are not detected consciously, consistent with other reports showing activations in the ventral striatum (Childress et al, 2008), thalamus, and ACC (Gillath and Canterberry, 2011) during the subliminal presentation of erotic stimuli.

Most pronounced were the effects of DA in the NAcc and the dACC, where a linear association was found dependent on the administration of haloperidol, placebo, or levodopa. The NAcc plays a central role in the reward circuit and its activity has frequently been related to processing of secondary rewards, such as money (Assadi et al, 2009; Kelley, 2004; McClure et al, 2003; Pessiglione et al, 2006; Pleger et al, 2009), and also of sexual stimuli (Walter et al, 2008). The dACC, as a rule of thumb, has been associated with the more cognitive aspects of stimulus processing, and ventral ACC more with emotional processes; nonetheless, the dACC is a key contributor to emotional processing (Etkin et al, 2011). The roles that are attributed to the dACC are numerous, but the common denominator is its role in various aspects of apt decision-making, from initial sensory perception to motor preparation (Assadi et al, 2009). The dACC is connected to the NAcc and ventral putamen, which together with its DA system is suggested to be involved in the evaluation and execution of decision-making. The dACC participates in motivation (‘wanting’) through mobilization of resources, and initiation of goal-directed behaviors, through its projections to the motor area, and the periaquaductal gray matter, the latter also known for generating sexual behaviors (Assadi et al, 2009; Lonstein and Stern, 1998). dACC is associated with autonomic modulation of heart rate and pupil dilation (Critchley et al, 2003, 2005). Furthermore, dACC abnormalities have been found in obsessive–compulsive disorders, schizophrenia, and addiction (Yucel et al, 2007a, 2007b, 2007c). DA dysregulation in dACC–NAcc networks are suggested to impair accurate decision-making, for instance, to oversee the costs of pursuing deviant impulses and the inability to learn from previous mistakes (Assadi et al, 2009).

Interestingly, the responses to sexual stimuli in our task, regardless of group assignment, also showed significant activations in bilateral anterior insula, which together with the dACC form the ‘salience network’ during no task conditions (Menon and Uddin, 2010). According to the recent model by Menon and Uddin (2010), the insula functions as an integral hub that detects salient stimuli, switches between other large-scale networks to facilitate access to attention and working memory, it modulates autonomic responses to those stimuli, and facilitates rapid access to the motor system through its coupling with the ACC. The activation of the salience network only during processing of sexual cues indicates their facilitated processing, which may have prompted autonomic or motor responses. Implicit processing of erotic stimuli has shown to evoke early autonomic reactions and motor preparation in previous studies (Janssen et al, 2000; Both et al, 2008b).

DA neurons are also known to respond to aversive states and stimuli, although with excitation in some and inhibition in other DA neurons (Bromberg-Martin et al, 2010). Animal studies suggest that in response to aversive states, after a brief initial increase, DA is decreased and then released when aversive stimuli are removed (Budygin et al, 2012; Cabib and Puglisi-Allegra, 2012). However, in this study, no activation in the ventral striatum was detected in response to the masked emotionally negative stimuli, even when lowering the threshold to uncorrected activation levels. Surprisingly, we also did not find amygdala activation in response to the masked emotionally negative pictures. Childress et al (2008) also did not detect any significant differences in activation between negative and neutral pictures using similar aversive pictures in a similar backward masking paradigm. They suggested that the lack of effects might be due to large inter-subject variability related to other variables, such as anxiety proneness. Nonetheless, several studies using masked fearful faces did report amygdala activation (eg, Carlson et al, 2009). Consciously perceived faces appear to evoke somewhat stronger amygdala activation than complex aversive IAPS pictures, even though the latter are rated as more arousing (Britton et al, 2006). Faces are less complex than aversive pictures, which may facilitate masked presentations. However, the pictures that were selected for this study were similar in complexity to the sexual pictures. It could be speculated that the processing of masked emotional stimuli might be facilitated when it comes to emotionally positive stimuli compared with emotionally negative stimuli. If the general default tendency to approach is greater than the tendency to avoid (Cacioppo et al, 1997; Cacioppo and Gardner, 1999), the first might be more responsive to weak positive stimuli, such as masked sexual pictures, whereas the latter tendency might be more likely evoked by more intense aversive stimuli.

Regretfully, we did not investigate whether DA-dependent increased activations in regions thought to regulate incentive salience was related to increases in desire or wanting, which could, for instance, be assessed by behavioral approach tendencies. There is, however, evidence for enhanced tendencies to approach sexual stimuli following levodopa administration (Both et al, 2005). Future studies ideally should incorporate such measures when investigating the effects of subliminal rewarding stimuli. Furthermore, another component of reward, that is, associative learning is very relevant when studying hypersexuality (Klucken et al, 2009; Both et al, 2008a, 2008b). With DA modulation, it could be investigated how DA affects the incentive reward of neutral stimuli paired with rewarding ones. Possibly, individual differences in DA sensitivity (Ben Zion et al, 2006), in combination with frequent exposure to sexual cues and reinforcement processes, might explain the initiation of aberrant sexual desires.

The DA-dependent ‘running start’ of the reward system, well before the motivational state is consciously experienced as wanting, might explain the struggle to manage the pull of rewards, as evident in individuals with compulsive reward-seeking behaviors such as addictions and hypersexuality. These subconscious processes also might play a role in Parkinson's patients who develop an increased sexual preoccupation while using dopaminergic medication or in schizophrenic patients whose sexual desire decreases during antipsychotic therapy. Future investigations should target the influence of DA on decision-making subsequent to implicit sexual reward processing, and ideally should include clinical hypersexual populations.