Skip to main content
Original Communications

Using a Probe Detection Task to Assess the Timing of Intrinsic Pleasantness Appraisals

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.68.3.161

The occurrence and timing of emotion-antecedent appraisal checks are difficult to assess. We report an attempt to estimate the time window of the intrinsic pleasantness check using a dual-task probe paradigm. In three experiments, participants viewed negative and positive pictures. Their other task was speeded response on a probe superimposed on the pictures with different stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). Longer probe-reaction times were observed for negative than for positive pictures. This effect appeared at SOA 300 ms or 350 ms, suggesting that the intrinsic pleasantness appraisal check yields a differential behavioral outcome around 300 ms after stimulus onset, and seems to continue unless attention to picture content is inhibited. This paradigm might be successfully used for the mental chronography of appraisal processes.

References

  • Aftanas, L., Varlamov, A., Pavlov, S., Makhnev, V., Reva, N. (2001). Affective picture processing: Event related synchronization within individually defined human theta band is modulated by valence dimension. Neuroscience Letters, 303, 115–118. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aguado, L., Pierna, M., Saugar, C. (2005). Affective priming with associatively acquired valence. Psicológica, 26, 261–279. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Aue, T., Flykt, A., Scherer, K.R. (2007). First evidence for differential and sequential efferent effects of stimulus relevance and goal conduciveness appraisal. Biological Psychology, 74, 347–357. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J.T., Berntson, G.G. (1994). Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 401–423. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Danion, J.-M., Kauffmann-Muller, F., Grangé, D., Zimmerman, M.-A., Greth, P. (1995). Affective valence of words, explicit and implicit memory in clinical depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 34, 227–234. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dawson, M.E., Schell, A.M., Beers, J.R., Kelly, A. (1982). Allocation of cognitive processing capacity during human autonomic classical conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 273–295. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dolcos, F., Cabeza, R. (2002). Event-related potentials of emotional memory: Encoding pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral pictures. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 252–263. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dolcos, F., LaBar, K.S., Cabeza, R. (2004). Dissociable effects of arousal and valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and subsequent memory: An event-related fMFRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Suppl., 163. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fanselow, M.S., Lester, L.S. (1988). A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. In R.C. Bolles, M.D. Beecher (Eds.), Evolution and learning (pp. 185–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Flykt, A., Bjärtå, A. (2008). The time course of resource allocation in spider-fearful participants during fear reactions. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1381–1400. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flykt, A., Caldara, R. (2006). Tracking fear in snake and spider fearful participants during visual search: A multiresponse domain study. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 1075–1091. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Globisch, J., Hamm, A.O., Esteves, F., Öhman, A. (1999). Fear appears fast: Temporal course of startle reflex potentiation in animal fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 36, 66–75. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grandjean, D., Scherer, K.R. (2008). Unpacking the cognitive architecture of emotion processes. Emotion, 8, 341–351. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hermans, D., Spruyt, A., Eelen, P. (2003). Automatic affective priming of recently acquired stimulus valence: Priming at SOA 300 but not at SOA 1000. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 83–99. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ito, T.A., Larsen, J.T., Smith, N.K., Cacioppo, J.T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 887–900. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kensinger, E.A. (2004). Remembering emotional experiences: The contribution of valence and arousal. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15, 241–251. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Keselman, H.J., Keselman, J.C. (1998). Comparing repeated measures means in factorial designs. Psychophysiology, 25, 612–618. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kramer, A., Spinks, J. (1991). Capacity views of human information processing. In J.R. Jennings, M.G.H. Coles (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive psychophysiology. Central and autonomic nervous system approaches (pp. 179–249). Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., Cuthbert, B.N. (1999). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report A-4, Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P.J., Greenwald, M.K., Bradley, M.M., Hamm, A.O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lazarus, R. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotion and cognition. American Psychologist, 37, 1019–1024. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lazarus, R. (1999). The cognition-emotion debate: A bit of history. In T. Dalgleish, M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 3–19). Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leventhal, H., Scherer, K.R. (1987). The relationship of emotion to cognition: A functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 3–28. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mather, M., Carstensen, L.L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychological Science, 14, 409–415. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meinhardt, J., Pekrun, R. (2003). Attentional resource allocation to emotional events: An ERP study. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 477–500. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Murphy, S., Zajonc, R. (1993). Affect, cognition and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–729. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neumann, D.L., Lipp, O.V., Siddle, D.A.T. (2002). Discriminating between task-relevant and task-irrelevant stimuli: The effects on autonomic orienting and secondary task reaction time. Journal of Psychophysiology, 16, 191–200. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Öhman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives and information processing mechanism. In M. Lewis, J.M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (pp. 511–536). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Öhman, A., Esteves, F., Flykt, A., Soares, J.J.F. (1993). Gateways to consciousness: Emotion, attention, and electrodermal activity. In J.-C. Roy, W. Boucsein, D.C. Fowles, J.H. Gruzelier (Eds.), Progress in electrodermal research (pp. 137–158). New York: Plenum. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pashler, H. (1998). The psychology of attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pashler, H., Johnston, J., Ruthruff, E. (2001). Attention and performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 629–651. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Posner, M.I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Russell, J.A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sabatinelli, D., Bradley, M.M., Lang, P.J. (2001). Affective startle modulation in anticipation and perception. Psychophysiology, 38, 719–722. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scherer, K.R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In K.R. Scherer, P. Ekman (Eds), Approaches to emotion (pp. 100–123). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Scherer, K.R. (1986). Studying emotions empirically: Issues and a paradigm for research. In K.R. Scherer, H.G. Wallbott, A.B. Summerfield (Eds.), Experiencing emotion: A cross-cultural study (pp. 3–27). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Scherer, K.R. (1999). Appraisal theories. In T. Dalgleish, M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 637–667). Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scherer, K.R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multi-level sequential checking. In K.R. Scherer, A. Schorr, T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 92–120). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Scherer, K.R., Dan, E.S., Flykt, A. (2006). What determines a feeling’s position in affective space? A case for appraisal. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 92–113. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scherer, K.R., Schorr, A., Johnstone, T. (2001). Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research. New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schupp, H.T., Cuthbert, B.N., Bradley, M.M., Cacioppo, J.T., Ito, T.A., Lang, P.J. (2000). Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology, 37, 257–261. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, C.A., Ellsworth, P.C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stone, A., Valentine, T. (2005). Orientation of attention to nonconsciously recognized famous faces. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 537–558. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Reekum, C.M., Banse, R., Johnstone, T., Etter, A., Wehrle, T., Scherer, K.R. (2004). Psychophysiological responses to appraisal responses in a computer game. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 663–688. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Reekum, C.M., Scherer, K.R. (1997). Levels of processing for emotion-antecedent appraisal. In G. Matthews (Ed.), Cognitive science perspectives on personality and emotion (pp. 259–300). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Whalen, P.J., Rauch, S.L., Etcoff, N.L., McInerney, S.C., Lee, M.B., Jenike, M.A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 411–418. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zajonc, R.B. (1998). Emotions. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 591–632). New York: McGraw-Hill. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar