Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T13:13:16.961Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Grammatical Comprehension in Spanish-Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2014

Juana Muñoz*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
Gloria Carballo
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
M. Dolores Fresneda
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
Elvira Mendoza
Affiliation:
Universidad de Granada (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Juana Muñoz López. Departamento de Personalidad. Evaluación y Tratamiento Psicológico. Facultad de Psicología. E-18071. Granada (Spain). Phone: +34–958244251. E-mail: jmunoz@ugr.es

Abstract

The Grammatical Structures Comprehension Test (CEG) was used to analyze grammatical comprehension problems in native Spanish-speaking children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). The test is divided into 20 blocks containing the most common grammatical structures in Spanish. Our objective was to establish whether the CEG was sensitive in detecting these problems and whether there were differential patterns in grammatical comprehension between children with SLI (14 participants) and two control groups: a chronological control group (CC) with 14 participants and a linguistic control group (LC) also with 14 participants. We found significant differences between the SLI group and the chronological control participants (Box’s M = 63.080, F = 1, 159, p = .238), with a correct classification rate of 85.7 % in the discriminant function analysis. These differences did not occur in all the blocks, in which we identified a range of different performance patterns that varied according to the structures being analyzed. This work helps to clarify certain questions about grammatical comprehension in children with SLI and contributes to the debate on delay vs. “delay within the delay”.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, R. T., & Souto, S. M. (2005). The use of articles by monolingual Puerto Rican Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 621647. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050332 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 675693. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13682820500442602 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (2001). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 905924. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/072) Google Scholar
Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (2005). Verb inflections and noun phrase morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 195225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050149 Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1979). Comprehension in developmental language disorders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 21, 225238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1979.tb01605.x Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1983). TROG. Test for Reception of Grammar. Oxford, UK: Medical Research Council. Chapel Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: Development and disorders of language comprehension. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., & Rosenbloom, L. (1987). Childhood language disorders: Classification and overview. In Yule, W. & Rutter, M. (Eds.), Language development and disorders (pp.1641). Oxford, UK: Mac Keith Press.Google Scholar
Bosch, L., & Serra, M. (1997). Grammatical morphology deficits of Spanish-speaking children with Specific Language Impairment. In Baker, A., Beers, A., Bal, M., de Jong, G., & Leemans, J. (Eds.), Child language disorders in a cross-linguistic perspective. Proceedings of the fourth symposium of the European Group of Child Language Disorders (pp. 3345). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Amsterdam series in Child Language Development.Google Scholar
Calet, N., Mendoza, E., Carballo, G., Fresneda, M. D., & Muñoz, J. (2010). CEG 2–4 (Test de comprensión de estructuras gramaticales de 2 a 4 años): Estudio Piloto. CEG 2–4. [Comprehension Test of Grammar Structures 2 to 4 years: A pilot study]. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 30, 6272.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. (1978). Comprehension strategies in children. In Kavanaugh, J. F. & Strange, W. (Eds.), Language and speech in the laboratory, school, and clinic (pp. 309327). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Hesketh, A. (2003). Risk markers for SLI: A study of young language-learning children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 251263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1368282031000092339 Google Scholar
Correa, L. M. S. (1995). An alternative assessment of children’s comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistics Research, 24, 183203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02145355 Google Scholar
Evans, J. L., & McWhinney, B. (1999). Sentence processing strategies in children with expressive and expressive–receptive specific language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 34, 117134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/136828299247469 Google Scholar
Felser, C., Marinis, T., & Clahsen, H. (2003). Children’s processing of ambiguous sentences: A study of relative clause attachment. Language Acquisition, 11, 127163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la1103_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, S., Plante, E., Vance, R., & Henrichsen, M. (1999). The diagnostic accuracy of four vocabulary tests administered to preschool-age children. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 196206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., Restrepo, M. A. Bedore, L., Peña, E. S., & Anderson, R. (2000). Language sample analysis in Spanish speaking-children: Methodological considerations. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 8898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hesketh, A. (2006). The use of relative clauses by children with language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 20, 539546. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699200500266398 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmann, L. M., & Gillam, R. B. (2004). Verbal and spatial processing constraints in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47, 114125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/011) CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horgan, D. (1978). The development of full passive. Journal of Child Language, 5, 6580. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S030500090000194X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, P. F., & Schwartz, R. G. (2002). Morphology in incipient bilingual Spanish-speaking pre-school children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 2341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716402000024 Google Scholar
Jakubowicz, C., Nash, L., Rigaut, C., & Gérard, C. L. (1998). Determiners and clitic pronouns in French-speaking children with SLI. Language Acquisition, 7, 113160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0702-4_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joanisse, M. F., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Specific language impairment in children: An impairment in grammar or processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 240246.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Magimairaj, B. M., & Montgomery, J. W. (2012). Children´s verbal working memory: Role of processing complexity in predicting spoken sentence comprehension. American Speech-Language- Hearing Association, 55, 669682. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11-0111) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marton, K., & Schwartz, R. (2003). Working memory capacity and language processes in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46, 11381153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/089) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marton, K., Schwartz, R. G., Farkas, L., & Katsnelson, V. (2006). Effects of sentence length and complexity on working memory performance in Hungarian children with specific language impairment (SLI): A cross-linguistic comparison. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 653673. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13682820500420418 Google Scholar
Mendoza, E., Carballo, G., Muñoz, J., & Fresneda, M. D. (2005a). CEG: Test de Comprensión de Estructuras Gramaticales. [Comprehension Test of Grammar Structures] Madrid, Spain: TEA.Google Scholar
Mendoza, E., Carballo, G., Muñoz, J., & Fresneda, M. D. (2005b). Evaluación de la comprensión gramatical: Un estudio translingüístico. [Assessment of grammatical comprensión: A cross-lingistic study] Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 25, 218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. F., & Paul, R. (1995). The clinical assessment of language comprehension. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes, Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (1995). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: The role of phonological working memory. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 187199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J. W. (2000a). Relation of working memory to off-line and real-time sentence processing in children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 117148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400001065 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (2000b). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 43, 293308.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (2002). Understanding the language difficulties of children with specific language impairment: Does verbal working memory matter? American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 7791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2002/009) CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (2004). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: Effects of input rate and phonological working memory. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 39, 115133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13682820310001616985 Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W. (2005). Effects of input rate and age on the real-time language processing of children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 171188. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13682820400011069 Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., & O’Malley, M. H. (2008). The role of working memory in typically developing children’s complex sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 37, 331354. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10936-008-9077-z Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., & Windsor, J. (2007). Examining the language performances of children with and without specific language impairment: contributions of phonological short-term memory and speed of processing. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50, 778797. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/054) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muñoz, J., Fresneda, M. D., Mendoza, E., & Carballo, G. (2008). Propiedades psicométricas de una prueba de comprensión gramatical. [Psycometric properties of a grammar comprehension test]. Revista de Neurología, 47, 2126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C. F., Bishop, D. V. M., & Briscoe, J. (2001). Production of English finite verb morphology: A comparison of SLI and mild-to-moderate hearing impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 44, 165178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/015) Google Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., & Genesse, F. (2006). Domain-general versus domain-specific accounts of specific language impairment. Evidence from bilingual children’s acquisition of object pronouns. Language acquisition, 13, 3362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la1301_3 Google Scholar
Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1994). Selection of preschool language tests: A data-based approach. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restrepo, M. A., & Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (2001). Article use in Spanish-speaking children with Specific. Language Impairment Journal of Child Language, 28, 433452. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000901004706 Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. (2003). A unified model of specific and general language delay: Grammatical tense as a clinical marker of unexpected variation. In Levy, Y. & Schaeffer, J. (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Toward a definition of specific language impairment (pp. 6395). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. (2004). Growth models of developmental language disorders. In Rice, M. L. & Warren, S. F. (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 12391257.Google Scholar
Robertson, E. K., & Joanisse, M. F. (2010). Spoken sentence comprehension in children with dyslexia and language impairment: The roles of syntax and working memory. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 141165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716409990208 Google Scholar
Sanz-Torrent, M., Aguilar, E., Serrat, E., & Serra, M. (2001). Verb type production in Catalan and Spanish children with SLI. In Almgren, M., Barreña, A., Ezeizabarrena, M. J., Idiazabal, I., & MacWhinney, B. (Eds.), Research on child language acquisition (pp. 909922). Massachussetts, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Sanz-Torrent, M., Llorenç, A., Badia, I., & Sidera, F. (2011). Argument omissions in preschool Catalan and Spanish speaking children with SLI. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 34, 4966. http://dx.doi.org/10.1174/021037011794390085 Google Scholar
Serra, M. (2002). Trastornos del lenguaje: Preguntas pendientes en investigación e intervención. [Language Disorders: Open issues in research and intervention]. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 22, 6376.Google Scholar
Simon-Cereijido, G., & Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (2007). Spontaneous language markers of Spanish language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 317339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070166 Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 175208). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Stavrakaki, S. (2003). Sentence comprehension in Williams syndrome and specific language impairment: A comparative approach. Annals of General Psychiatry, 2, 86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1475-2832-2-S1-S86 Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1994). Canonical linking rules: Forward versus reverse linking in normally developing and specifically language impaired children. Cognition, 51, 2972. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90008-6 Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1996). Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: Verbal passive vs adjectival passive sentence interpretation. Lingua, 98, 243272. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(95)00044-5 Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1998). SLI in children: Movement, economy and deficits in the computational syntactic system. Language Acquisition, 7, 161192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0702-4_4 Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (2003). Do heterogeneous SLI deficits need heterogeneous theories? SLI subgroups, G-SLI and the RDDR hypothesis. In Levy, Y. & Schaeffer, J. (Eds.), Towards a definition of specific language impairment (pp.109134). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. J. K. (2004). Evidence for an implication of a domain-specific grammatical deficit. In Jenkins, L.. (Ed.), The genetics of language (pp. 117144). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from grammatical-specific language impairment. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 5359. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002 Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Battell, J. (2003). Wh-movement in children with grammatical SLI: A test of the RDDR. Hypothesis. Language, 79, 153181.Google Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Dewart, H. (1986). Sentence comprehension strategies in specifically language impaired children. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 21, 291306. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/13682828609019843 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Harris, M. (1990). Comprehension of reversible sentences in specifically language impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 55, 101117.Google ScholarPubMed
Weighall, A., & Altmann, G. (2011). The role of working memory and contextual constraints in children’s processing of relative clauses. Journal of Child Language, 38, 579605. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000267 Google Scholar