Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T10:36:33.757Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 July 2023

Ning Song*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Medicine and Health, Leeds Institute of Health Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
Siobhan Hugh-Jones
Affiliation:
Faculty of Medicine and Health, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
Robert M. West
Affiliation:
Faculty of Medicine and Health, Leeds Institute of Health Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
John Pickavance
Affiliation:
Faculty of Medicine and Health, School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK Centre for Applied Education Research, Bradford Teaching Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Bradford, UK
Ghazala Mir
Affiliation:
Faculty of Medicine and Health, Leeds Institute of Health Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK
*
Corresponding author: Ning Song; Email: umnso@leeds.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Experiencing mental health stigma during adolescence can exacerbate mental health conditions, reduce quality of life and inhibit young people’s help-seeking for their mental health needs. For young people, education and contact have most often been viewed as suitable approaches for stigma reduction. However, evidence on the effectiveness of these anti-stigma interventions has not been consistent. This systematic review evaluated the effectiveness of interventions to reduce mental health stigma among youth aged 10–19 years. The review followed Cochrane and PRISMA guidelines. Eight databases were searched: PubMed, PsycINFO, MEDLINE, Web of Science, Scopus, EMBASE, British Education Index and CNKI. Hand searching from included studies was also conducted. Randomised controlled trials and experimental designs that included randomised allocation to interventions and control groups were included in the review. Narrative synthesis was employed to analyse the results. A meta-analysis was conducted to determine the effectiveness of included interventions. Twenty-two studies were included in the review. Eight studies reported positive effects, 11 studies found mixed effects and 3 studies reported no effect on indicators of mental health stigma among youth. Seven of the effective studies were education-based. Eleven studies were suitable for meta-analysis, and the multivariate meta-analytic model indicated a small, significant effect at post-intervention (d = .21, p < .001), but not at follow-up (d = .069, p = .347). Interventions to reduce stigma associated with mental health conditions showed small, short-term effects in young people. Education-based interventions showed relatively more significant effects than other types of interventions.

Type
Overview Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Impact statement

We review international evidence on the effectiveness of anti-stigma mental health interventions delivered in schools. Mental health stigma is a global issue, affecting help-seeking, treatment, quality of life and mental health outcomes. In particular, the experience of mental health stigma during adolescence can exacerbate mental health conditions and lead to significant negative life impacts. The evidence base for school-based mental health stigma reduction efforts needs to be widely communicated to researchers, education, healthcare providers and policy makers. Developing evidence in this field about ‘what works’ will support the growth of governmental policies to ensure anti-stigma interventions reach young people. A focus on anti-stigma interventions in young people has the potential to significantly improve the quality of life of people living with poor mental health as well as their education, employment and help-seeking trajectories. Our article identifies effective components of anti-stigma interventions among youth aged 10–19 years and highlights the lack of evidence from low-and middle-income countries and lack of previous meta-analysis. To the best of our knowledge, ours is the first review to produce an effect size for anti-stigma interventions which target young people. We show that these interventions showed small, short-term effects in young people. Education-based interventions showed relatively more significant effects than other approaches. Our review supports the use of education interventions in schools for reducing mental health stigma in young people and makes recommendations for improving the quality of future interventions and trials. Our article is likely to influence thinking about the adoption of the most appropriate school-delivered prevention strategies for reducing mental health stigma in young people.

Introduction

Adolescent mental health is a significant global issue. In the global population, of those aged 10–19 years, one in seven have a mental health disorder (WHO, 2021), and 75% of mental health disorders arise before the age of 18 (Patton et al., Reference Patton, Sawyer, Santelli, Ross, Afifi, Allen, Arora, Azzopardi, Baldwin, Bonell, Kakuma, Kennedy, Mahon, McGovern, Mokdad, Patel, Petroni, Reavley, Taiwo, Waldfogel, Wickremarathne, Barroso, Bhutta, Fatusi, Mattoo, Diers, Fang, Ferguson, Ssewamala and Viner2016). However, the proportion of undetected and untreated cases of mental-ill health is likely to be higher among young people (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Angermeyer, Anthony, de Graaf, Demyttenaere, Gasquet, de Girolamo, Gluzman, Gureje, Haro, Kawakami, Karam, Levinson, Medina Mora, Oakley Browne, Posada-Villa, Stein, Adley Tsang, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Lee, Heeringa, Pennell, Berglund, Gruber, Petukhova, Chatterji and Ustün2007). One reason for the low rate of mental health treatment in youth is stigma associated with having a mental health condition (Calear et al., Reference Calear, Batterham, Torok and McCallum2021). It has been argued that stigma of any kind is a global phenomenon functioning to ‘keep people in the group by enforcing social norms, keep people away from the group as a strategy to avoid disease and keep people down to exploit and dominate’ (Hartoga et al., Reference Hartoga, Hubbardc, Krouwera, Thornicroftd, Kohrte and Jordans2020, p. 2). Goffman’s (Reference Goffman1963) landmark publication described stigma as leading to a ‘spoiled identity’. He argued that just as physical marks such as burns or cuts historically identified in slaves or criminals, stigma is a form of social marks to indicate social difference and ostracisation. Contemporary work has begun to focus on culture-specific forms of stigma (of any kind). Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Kleinman, Link, Phelan, Lee and Good2007) argued that everyday life and interactions in a culture indicate ‘what matters most’ in that context. Termed ‘moral experience’, the position argues that if a person can engage with ‘what matters most’ in that context, then they have ‘full status’ (or personhood) within that cultural group (Kleinman, Reference Kleinman2006). Stigma can affect a person’s opportunity to participate in ‘what matters most’, and this may vary by cultural contexts (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Kleinman, Link, Phelan, Lee and Good2007). What become stigmatised, and the form and practice stigma, will also vary by culture and sub-culture (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Kleinman, Link, Phelan, Lee and Good2007).

Mental health stigma is defined as ‘negative thoughts, beliefs and discriminatory behaviours towards individuals with mental illness or those receiving mental health services’ (Pederson et al., Reference Pederson, Burnett-Zeigler, Fokuo, Wisner, Zumpf and Oshodi2020, p. 2). ‘Public stigma’ and ‘self-stigma’ are the most reported kinds of stigma associated with mental health conditions (Corrigan and Watson, Reference Corrigan and Watson2002). Societal discrimination or prejudice about mental health conditions has been termed public stigma (Link, Reference Link1987), social stigma or enacted stigma (Livingston and Boyed, Reference Livingston and Boyed2010). Self-stigma, which is also known as internalised stigma (Park et al., Reference Park, Lee and Seo2019), can be defined as the application of negative stereotypes to oneself, resulting in internalised devaluation (Corrigan, Reference Corrigan2002).

There is evidence that the experience of mental health stigma during adolescence exacerbates mental health conditions and leads to significant negative life impacts (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Yen, Ko, Cheng and Yen2010). Kaushik et al.’s (Reference Kaushik, Kostaki and Kyriakopoulos2016) systematic review on mental health stigma towards children and youth found that when young people hold a viewpoint blaming those with mental-ill health, they are more likely to keep a distance from a young person with a mental health condition. In Moses’s (Reference Moses2010) interview study with 56 American youth with mental health conditions, 25 reported experiences of being rejected by peers. American youth reported that they were viewed as lazy by their families when taking medication for a mental health need (Elkington et al., Reference Elkington, Hackler, McKinnon, Borges, Wright and Wainberg2012). Nearchou et al. (Reference Nearchou, Bird, Costello, Duggan, Gilroy, Long, McHugh and Hennessy2018) investigated mental health stigma in Irish youth and found that public stigma predicted lower intention to seek help.

Self-stigma has also been identified as a barrier to help-seeking. For example, Yap et al. (Reference Yap, Wright and Jorm2011) found that Australian youth were less likely to see psychology professionals in school-based mental health services when they considered mental health conditions as an individual weakness. Shechtmana et al. (Reference Shechtmana, Vogelb, Strassb and Heathb2018) investigated stigma and help-seeking in Israeli adolescents and found that self-stigma was negatively associated with attitudes towards seeking help. According to a systematic review on psychological outcomes of adolescents’ mental health stigma, self-stigma can aggravate a young person’s mental health conditions (Ferriea et al., Reference Ferriea, Millera and Hunter2020). Additionally, researchers found that, in American adolescents, internalised stigma mediates the relationship between psychosis and subjective quality of life (Akouri-Shan et al., Reference Akouri-Shan, DeLuca, Pitts, Jay, Redman, Petti, Bridgwater, Rakhshan Rouhakhtar, Klaunig, Chibani, Martin, Reeves and Schiffman2022). Mitten et al. (Reference Mitten, Preyde, Lewis, Vanderkooy and Heintzman2016) investigated the perceptions of self-stigma in Canadian youth with self-harm experience and reported that youth believed that others would avoid contacting them out of fear of their mental health conditions. In addition, the main reason for unwillingness to seek help was the worry among American youth about peers’ pejorative or stigmatising attitudes towards their help-seeking, as well as concerns with confidentiality in mental health services (Heflinger and Hinshaw, Reference Heflinger and Hinshaw2016).

The development and evaluation of interventions to challenge mental health stigma in many parts of the world has aimed to increase help-seeking, reduce self-stigma, improve social acceptance and engagement (e.g., in school) and reduce negative impacts of stigma on quality of life and suicide rates. Protest, education and contact have been identified as three main approaches to addressing stigma (Corrigan and Penn, Reference Corrigan and Penn1999). Protest refers to taking exception to situations where stigmatising experiences occur (Corrigan and Penn, Reference Corrigan and Penn1999), such as when advocacy and service groups organise events to protest against social stigma. School is a key environment where young people can socialise, obtain knowledge and shape their attitudes and beliefs. Many anti-stigma programmes in Western countries have been mainly designed and delivered in schools. Education interventions provide factual mental health information via teaching and workshops that challenge mental health stereotypes (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Reavley, Ross, Too and Jorm2018). Contact interventions work to lower fear of mental health conditions and develop empathy through the involvement of people who have experience of living with a mental health condition (Pettigrew and Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2008).

Several theoretical frameworks inform existing interventions; contact-based approaches are informed by Intergroup Contact Theory, coined by Allport (Reference Allport1954), which proposes that intergroup prejudice can be reduced when social groups have more social contact. Pinto-Foltz et al. (Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011) stated that their intervention was guided by Fisher’s Narrative Paradigm Theory (Fisher and Hood, Reference Fisher and Hood1987), which proposes that all meaningful communication is a form of storytelling or reporting of events.

According to a systematic review on anti-stigma interventions with secondary and primary students (Mellor, Reference Mellor2014), education and contact interventions are common choices. Yet outcomes from anti-stigma interventions based on these approaches are inconsistent. For example, Mulfinger et al. (Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018) and Pinto-Foltz et al. (Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011) evaluated contact-based interventions, respectively, but the former showed positive effects and the later reported no effect. The inconsistent results may link to specific components of the interventions, delivery, dosage and/or characteristics of participants. Studies also often have considerable methodological limitations (Sakellari et al., Reference Sakellari, Leino-Kilpi and Kalokerinou-Anagnostopoulou2011), meaning caution is needed when interpreting any reports of effectiveness (Mellor, Reference Mellor2014).

There is, therefore, a need to identify the effective components of interventions to reduce mental health stigma in young people. A meta-analysis of high-quality studies would also be valuable in determining the strength of evidence for particular types of interventions. Several systematic reviews have been conducted to investigate the effectiveness of interventions for stigma reduction in young people. However, previous reviews have focused on a specific intervention delivery platform, such as video (Janoušková et al., Reference Janoušková, Tušková, Weissová, Trančík, Pasz, Evans-Lacko and Winkler2017), have only paid attention to school-based interventions (Mellor, Reference Mellor2014) or have focused on other types of stigma, such as HIV stigma rather than mental health stigma (Hartoga et al., Reference Hartoga, Hubbardc, Krouwera, Thornicroftd, Kohrte and Jordans2020). There are no systematic reviews of mental health anti-stigma interventions conducted via randomised controlled trials for young people or meta-analyses synthesising their effectiveness. This study aimed to fill this evidence gap through a systematic review and meta-analysis to explore the effectiveness of mental health anti-stigma interventions for young people, and where possible, to identify effective interventions with global relevance.

Methods

This review followed Cochrane and PRISMA guidelines (Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow, Shamseer, Tetzlaff, Akl, Brennan, Chou, Glanville, Grimshaw, Hróbjartsson, Lalu, Li, Loder, Mayo-Wilson, McDonald, McGuinness, Stewart, Thomas, Tricco, Welch, Whiting and Moher2021).

Protocol and registration

A written protocol for the systematic review has been completed and registered on PROSPERO (registration number is CRD42021251932).

Eligibility criteria and exclusion criteria

The PICOS framework (Amir-Behghadami and Janati, Reference Amir-Behghadami and Janati2020) was employed to identify eligibility and exclusion criteria. Table 1 demonstrates the selection criteria.

Table 1. Eligibility and exclusion criteria

Information sources

Eight databases were searched to identify eligible studies: PubMed, PsycINFO (2002 to present), MEDLINE (1950 to present), Web of Science (1999 to present), Scopus (1823 to present), EMBASE (1996 to present), British Education Index (1975 to present) and the Chinese database CNKI (1999 to present). A Chinese database was included to address the lack of evidence from China in existing reviews because of the availability of language expertise within the research team. A manual search was conducted to identify further eligible studies by examining reference lists of included papers. Any potential papers were found via this search and then screened following the procedures described below.

The search was conducted from May 2021 to July 2021, and a manual trace back literature search was conducted in March 2022. No limits were applied to publication year or language for the database search. However, if studies were not written in English or Chinese, they were excluded from the review.

Search strategy

Search terms were determined around four domains: stigma, mental health conditions, young people and intervention. Appendix 1 of the Supplementary Material specifies the search terms.

Study selection

This review followed the PRISMA guidance (Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow, Shamseer, Tetzlaff, Akl, Brennan, Chou, Glanville, Grimshaw, Hróbjartsson, Lalu, Li, Loder, Mayo-Wilson, McDonald, McGuinness, Stewart, Thomas, Tricco, Welch, Whiting and Moher2021) for study selection. All documents and data from reviewed papers were stored in Rayyan (Ouzzani et al., Reference Ouzzani, Hammady, Fedorowicz and Elmagarmid2016). Following search returns, duplicate papers were removed, and the titles and abstracts of the remaining papers were screened. Double screening was also conducted. Eligible papers were then subjected to full-text review. Papers without full access were sought by contacting authors for copies. The full-text papers were screened according to the inclusion criteria. The second reviewer reviewed a random 50% of the full-text papers. Disagreements were resolved through discussion.

Data extraction

Data were extracted using the Cochrane Collaboration-recommended templates ‘Data Collection for Intervention Reviews for Randomised Controlled Trials Only’ (Cochrane, 2021). Tables 2 and 3 provide information on study characteristics of included studies.

Table 2. Characteristics of included studies

Note: CRT, cluster-randomised trial, NA, not available, NI, no information, QED, quasi-experimental design, RCT, randomised controlled trial.

Table 3. Outcomes and intervention sessions

Note: NA, not available; NI, no information.

Quality assessment

Given that the included studies were randomised controlled trials, cluster-randomised trials and quasi-experimental designs involving intervention and control groups with random allocation of participants, Risk of Bias 2 (Sterne et al., Reference Sterne, Savović, Page, Elbers, Blencowe, Boutron, Cates, Cheng, Corbett, Eldridge, Emberson, Hernán, Hopewell, Hróbjartsson, Junqueira, Jüni, Kirkham, Lasserson, Li, McAleenan, Reeves, Shepperd, Shrier, Stewart, Tilling, White, Whiting and Higgins2019) was employed for the quality assessment of randomised controlled trials and quasi-experimental designs (Cochrane, 2021), and Risk of Bias 2 Cluster-Randomised Trials (Sterne et al., Reference Sterne, Savović, Page, Elbers, Blencowe, Boutron, Cates, Cheng, Corbett, Eldridge, Emberson, Hernán, Hopewell, Hróbjartsson, Junqueira, Jüni, Kirkham, Lasserson, Li, McAleenan, Reeves, Shepperd, Shrier, Stewart, Tilling, White, Whiting and Higgins2019) was used for the quality of assessment of cluster-randomised trials. After assessing the quality of included studies, the second assessor conducted a double quality assessment. Disagreements were resolved by discussion.

Data synthesis

A narrative synthesis was used to analyse the findings of the eligible studies and the components of the reported interventions. This allowed results of included studies to be assessed systematically and comprehensively and significant features of the included studies to be highlighted (Ryan, Reference Ryan2013).

Meta-analysis was performed using R (v4.2.0). Models were fit using the metafor (v3.4.0) package with covariance imputed for robust estimation using clubSandwich (v0.5.6). The sample size, mean and standard deviation were obtained for control and intervention groups at up to three time points for all screened studies. They were Pre-Intervention, Post-Intervention and Follow-Up. All measures were transformed such that a positive mean difference indicated a reduction in stigma. Cohen’s d was then computed as the standardised mean difference at each time point available for each study (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). Firstly, a multivariate meta-analysis model on Pre-Intervention effect sizes was conducted as a control. In many studies, multiple measures of stigma were taken (see Tables 2 and 3). Therefore, a random effect was specified to better account for the variance both within and between the studies (Harrer et al., Reference Harrer, Cuijpers, Furukawa and Ebert2021). Moreover, robust variance estimation was used to account for the fact that measures within the studies were likely to be highly correlated having been generated from the same sample or intervention. The correlation coefficient was set to .6 a priori, but subsequent fits with coefficients ranging from .2 to .8 showed no discernible difference. The variance of the distribution of effect sizes (τ 2) was calculated using restricted maximum likelihood. Once it was confirmed that there were no effects at baseline, separate multivariate meta-analysis models were fit to estimate pooled effect sizes at Post-Intervention and Follow-Up using the same specifications as above. Finally, for completeness, Post-Intervention and Follow-Up effects were combined, and a multivariate meta-analysis model was fit with time as a moderating effect. This was coded as a dummy variable with Post-Intervention as the intercept and Follow-Up as the coefficient. This was to determine whether any decline in the efficacy of interventions was significant.

Results

Study selection

A PRISMA flow diagram (see Figure 1) shows the procedures for study selection. In total, 170 studies were identified for full-text screening. Of these, 21 studies were excluded as full texts were not accessible, leaving 149 for full-text reviews. The agreement between two reviewers for study selection was calculated at a full-text review using Cohen’s k coefficient, and the value was 0.9, suggesting excellent interrater reliability. Following this, 16 studies were assessed as eligible for the review. Other reasons for exclusion included studies that were protocols or not relevant to the review topic. A further six studies were identified via a hand searching from the included studies. A final total of 22 publications were included in the review.

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram.

Study characteristics

Population characteristics

The interventions were conducted in 14 countries and mainly distributed in Europe (n = 8), North America (n = 9), Australia (n = 4) and Asia (n = 1). The number of participants including control groups ranged from 98 to 6,025. The age range of study samples was 12–19 years. Seven studies indicated the mean age of participants ranged from 14.52 to 18.41, and six studies did not report participants’ age but stated that these were secondary/high school students. Most studies investigated the efficacy of interventions for both boys and girls with two exceptions: Staniland and Byrne (Reference Staniland and Byrne2013) only included boys and Pinto-Foltz et al. (Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011) recruited only girls. Except for Mulfinger et al. (Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018), who recruited participants with a diagnosed mental health condition, all other study samples were community groups. Table 2 shows the overall characteristics of included studies.

Intervention characteristics

Seven interventions aimed to reduce stigma linked to a specific mental health condition (such as depression); one targeted stigma about autism and the remaining interventions aimed to reduce stigma around general mental health conditions. One study (Mulfinger et al., Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018) was conducted in an inpatient setting, and the other 21 studies were implemented in secondary/high schools.

Eighteen studies used randomised controlled trials, including cluster-randomised trials to evaluate intervention efficacy, and the other four interventions used experiment designs involved random allocation of participants to intervention and control groups but did not use the term randomised controlled trials.

Four studies had comparator intervention(s) (Chisholm et al., Reference Chisholm, Patterson, Torgerson, Turner, Jenkinson and Birchwood2016; Painter et al., Reference Painter, Phelan, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro and Link2017; Ahmad et al., Reference Ahmad, Leventhal, Nielsen and Hinshaw2020; Link et al., Reference Link, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro, Phelan and Painter2020). Participants were grouped in no intervention control condition in seven studies (Pinto-Foltz et al., Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011; Staniland and Byrne, Reference Staniland and Byrne2013; Perry et al., Reference Perry, Petrie, Buckley, Cavanagh, Clarke, Winslade, Hadzi-Pavlovic, Manicavasagar and Christensen2014; Vila-Badia et al., Reference Vila-Badia, Martínez-Zambrano, Arenas, Casas-Anguera, García-Morales, Villellas, Martín, Pérez-Franco, Valduciel, Casellas, García-Franco, Miguel, Balsera, Pascual, Julia and Ochoa2016; Painter et al., Reference Painter, Phelan, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro and Link2017; Link et al., Reference Link, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro, Phelan and Painter2020; Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Dang, Bui, Phoeun and Weiss2020). Eight studies adopted active control groups, where participants received a leaflet (Winkler et al., Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017), a talk about nutrition and healthy living (Economou et al., Reference Economou, Louki, Peppou, Gramandani, Yotis and Stefanis2012), a presentation of parallel length on ‘careers in psychology’ (DeLuca, Reference DeLuca2020), treatment (not described) as usual (Mulfinger et al., Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018), a parallel 35-min educational presentation with content unrelated to mental health (Saporito et al., Reference Saporito, Ryan and Teachman2011), a 2-h discussion on immunisation (Economou et al., Reference Economou, Peppou, Geroulanou, Louki, Tsaliagkou, Kolostoumpis and Stefanis2014), neutral information on the symptoms of depression (Howard et al., Reference Howard, Griffiths, McKetin and Ma2018) and another video game unrelated to mental health (Cangas et al., Reference Cangas, Navarro, Parra, Ojeda, Cangas, Piedra and Gallego2017).

Thirteen studies reported some details on intervention compliance. Table 3 shows information on outcomes, intervention sessions, post-intervention time, indicators of intervention compliance, comparator interventions and control condition.

Included interventions were mainly based on education, contact or education plus contact. Education interventions provided factual mental health information via teaching and workshops that aimed to challenge mental health stereotypes (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Reavley, Ross, Too and Jorm2018). Contact interventions aimed to lower fear and develop empathy through contact with people living with mental health conditions (Pettigrew and Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2008). Sixteen interventions were education-based, and they were delivered by trained teachers/professionals/facilitators via the curriculum, lessons, discussions, lectures, activities, meetings and seminars. The delivery forms were interactive, which meant participants needed to interact with facilitators/peers, such as participating in a discussion, rather than passively receiving content from facilitators. Five studies were contact-related interventions, inviting people who had poor mental health to share their lived experience or personal story via video delivery or presentation (Pinto-Foltz et al., Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011; Vila-Badia et al., Reference Vila-Badia, Martínez-Zambrano, Arenas, Casas-Anguera, García-Morales, Villellas, Martín, Pérez-Franco, Valduciel, Casellas, García-Franco, Miguel, Balsera, Pascual, Julia and Ochoa2016; Painter et al., Reference Painter, Phelan, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro and Link2017; Winkler et al., Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017; Mulfinger et al., Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018). Three interventions included both education and contact elements (Saporito et al., Reference Saporito, Ryan and Teachman2011; Staniland and Byrne, Reference Staniland and Byrne2013; DeLuca, Reference DeLuca2020).

Interventions were commonly delivered via an educational curriculum approach; this introduced symptoms and basic information on mental health conditions to increase mental health knowledge, interaction and discussion on how to reduce stigma and videos that shared the experience of people living with mental conditions. Cangas et al. (Reference Cangas, Navarro, Parra, Ojeda, Cangas, Piedra and Gallego2017) delivered the intervention using a video game that featured characters with various mental health conditions (e.g., schizophrenia and depression), presenting knowledge on mental health conditions to correct participants’ misunderstanding or stereotypes. Guidebooks, leaflets, booklets, printed materials and some supplementary resources on stigma reduction were also incorporated into education, contact or education plus contact interventions. Participants could access these materials online through a website designed for the intervention. Three studies (Staniland and Byrne, Reference Staniland and Byrne2013; Painter et al., Reference Painter, Phelan, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro and Link2017; Link et al., Reference Link, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro, Phelan and Painter2020) also introduced homework exercises.

Fifteen studies reported the intervention delivery agent (see Table 2). Teachers who were trained to deliver the intervention were reported in six studies (Perry et al., Reference Perry, Petrie, Buckley, Cavanagh, Clarke, Winslade, Hadzi-Pavlovic, Manicavasagar and Christensen2014; Milin et al., Reference Milin, Kutcher, Lewis, Walker, Wei, Ferrill and Armstrong2016; Painter et al., Reference Painter, Phelan, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro and Link2017; Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Musci, Stuart, Heley, Beaudry, Schweizer, Ruble, Swartz and Wilcox2019; Link et al., Reference Link, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro, Phelan and Painter2020; Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Dang, Bui, Phoeun and Weiss2020). Four studies showed that their delivery agents were mental health professionals, psychologists or psychiatrists (Economou et al., Reference Economou, Louki, Peppou, Gramandani, Yotis and Stefanis2012; Economou et al., Reference Economou, Peppou, Geroulanou, Louki, Tsaliagkou, Kolostoumpis and Stefanis2014; Winkler et al., Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017; Mulfinger et al., Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018). Other delivery agents included the researcher (Staniland and Byrne, Reference Staniland and Byrne2013), trained peer facilitator (O’Mara et al., Reference O’Mara, Akhtar-Danesh, Browne, Mueller, Grypstra, Vrkljan and Powell2013) and trained graduate and undergraduate psychology students (Saporito et al., Reference Saporito, Ryan and Teachman2011). Contact elements were delivered by healthcare staff (Vila-Badia et al., Reference Vila-Badia, Martínez-Zambrano, Arenas, Casas-Anguera, García-Morales, Villellas, Martín, Pérez-Franco, Valduciel, Casellas, García-Franco, Miguel, Balsera, Pascual, Julia and Ochoa2016), and young people and adults who had a mental health condition (Pinto-Foltz et al., Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011; Painter et al., Reference Painter, Phelan, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro and Link2017; Mulfinger et al., Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018).

Thirteen studies reported how many sessions the intervention involved. Ten interventions were single session only. The longest intervention was 12 × 60 min sessions (Cangas et al., Reference Cangas, Navarro, Parra, Ojeda, Cangas, Piedra and Gallego2017). Among those who reported intervention dose (k = 17), the shortest and longest interventions lasted 2 min (Winkler et al., Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017) and 120 min (Economou et al., Reference Economou, Peppou, Geroulanou, Louki, Tsaliagkou, Kolostoumpis and Stefanis2014), respectively.

All studies collected post-intervention data. Nine studies collected data immediately after the intervention was completed. Other studies assessed their outcomes at different time points. Twelve studies collected follow-up data. Pinto-Foltz et al. (Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011) and DeLuca (Reference DeLuca2020) had two follow-up points, and Cheetham et al. (Reference Cheetham, Sandral and Lubman2020) had three. The longest follow-up point was 24 months (Link et al., Reference Link, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro, Phelan and Painter2020), and the shortest one was 3 weeks (Mulfinger et al., Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018).

The intervention outcomes examined in the studies included: self-stigma, stigmatising beliefs/attitudes, stigma stress, mental health knowledge, help-seeking intentions, social distance, disclosure worries, suicidal ideations, alcohol use, resilience, quality of life, acceptability, confidence, implicit bias, empowerment and recovery. Some studies investigated outcomes related to specific mental health conditions, such as stigmatising attitudes towards schizophrenia.

Most studies did not report a primary outcome or distinguish between the primary and secondary outcomes; only five studies distinguished and reported these. Two of these studies investigated stigma-related outcomes as the primary outcomes, whereas these were secondary outcomes in the other two studies. Researchers used different standardised measures to measure outcomes. Our review focused on stigma-related outcomes for which results are reported in Table 3.

Intervention effectiveness

In total, eight interventions showed positive effects, including six education-only intervention, one contact-only intervention and one intervention that included education and contact. Six education-based interventions reported significant stigma reduction at immediately after the intervention (Perry et al., Reference Perry, Petrie, Buckley, Cavanagh, Clarke, Winslade, Hadzi-Pavlovic, Manicavasagar and Christensen2014; Milin et al., Reference Milin, Kutcher, Lewis, Walker, Wei, Ferrill and Armstrong2016; Cangas et al., Reference Cangas, Navarro, Parra, Ojeda, Cangas, Piedra and Gallego2017; Winkler et al., Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017; Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Dang, Bui, Phoeun and Weiss2020), and 2 weeks afterwards (Economou et al., Reference Economou, Peppou, Geroulanou, Louki, Tsaliagkou, Kolostoumpis and Stefanis2014). Of these, the following five interventions were conducted in classrooms and aimed to both deliver mental health literacy and correct misconceptions about mental health conditions. Perry et al. (Reference Perry, Petrie, Buckley, Cavanagh, Clarke, Winslade, Hadzi-Pavlovic, Manicavasagar and Christensen2014) delivered an anti-stigma intervention to Australian youth and found significant effects on stigma reduction at different test time points (p < .05) compared with the control group. Intervention delivery was via a booklet, slideshow and various appendices in class. Nguyen et al. (Reference Nguyen, Dang, Bui, Phoeun and Weiss2020) evaluated an intervention for Vietnamese and Cambodian youth and reported significant effects on stigma reduction in both Vietnamese (p < .0001) and Cambodian youth (p < .0001) at post-intervention compared to the control group. This intervention involved six modules that were on mental health-related knowledge and responses to mental health conditions. Link et al. (Reference Link, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro, Phelan and Painter2020) evaluated the efficacy of a curriculum intervention compared with two comparator interventions (contact and printed materials) and a control group. The curriculum intervention involved a didactic component group discussion and homework exercises in each module. The curriculum intervention significantly increased knowledge and improved attitudes towards mental health conditions (p < .001) and reduced social distance (p < .05), with these effects being maintained over 2 years. Only the curriculum aspect of the intervention was effective, and neither contact nor printed materials showed significant effects on measured outcomes. Economou et al. (Reference Economou, Peppou, Geroulanou, Louki, Tsaliagkou, Kolostoumpis and Stefanis2014) obtained significant reductions in the intervention group in negative attitudes towards schizophrenia at post-intervention (p < .001) and social distance (p < .001). This intervention was delivered through an educational talk in class. Milin et al. (Reference Milin, Kutcher, Lewis, Walker, Wei, Ferrill and Armstrong2016) intervention for stigma reduction in youth also obtained positive findings showing an increase of positive attitudes towards mental health conditions compared to the control group (p < .01). Researchers delivered this intervention via six mental health stigma-related modules, which were embedded in classroom activities.

Winkler et al. (Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017) evaluated two interventions (seminar and short video) and an active control group (leaflets). At post-intervention, there were small effects in the flyer arm, medium in the seminar arm and medium in the video arm. At a 3-month follow-up, there were medium effects in the seminar arm and small effects in the video arm but no effect in the flyer arm. The seminar showed the strongest and relatively most stable effect on outcomes, which suggested that the role of facilitators could be of importance in changing attitudes. One study (Cangas et al., Reference Cangas, Navarro, Parra, Ojeda, Cangas, Piedra and Gallego2017) designed a video game to deliver the education intervention. They reported a statistically significant stigma reduction towards schizophrenia (dangerousness: p = .000; stereotypes: p = .001). Finally, Mulfinger et al. (Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018) conducted a contact-based intervention in an inpatient setting. This peer-led programme covered five themes aiming to increase disclosure of mental health conditions. The intervention was delivered by a young adult with experience of a mental health condition and a young mental health professional. At post-intervention, there was a significant improvement in stigma stress (i.e., person feels stigma-related harms outweigh the coping resources; the level of stigma stress will be high if that person feels less confident to cope with stigma; p < .001) as well as at a 3-week follow-up (p < .001).

Eleven interventions reported mixed effects. Of those, the following six studies evaluated education-based interventions. O’Mara et al.’s (Reference O’Mara, Akhtar-Danesh, Browne, Mueller, Grypstra, Vrkljan and Powell2013) intervention involved videos followed by researcher-facilitated discussion and focus groups. It did not significantly reduce stigma overall but showed significant decrease stigma in the low-needs schools (i.e., less needy in the Learning Opportunity Index), resulting in lower rates of depression and more uses of healthy coping strategies after the intervention (p < .001). Gonçalves et al. (Reference Gonçalves, Moleiro and Cook2015) reported significantly higher intervention effects on self-stigma (p < .05), social stigma (p < .05) and attribution (p < .05) for the intervention group than the control group, but at a 1-month follow-up, the effects diminished for these three outcomes to become non-significant. Economou et al.’s (Reference Economou, Louki, Peppou, Gramandani, Yotis and Stefanis2012) intervention involved discussions delivered by an educational psychologist and a psychiatrist. They found a significant change in participants’ beliefs and attitudes towards people with schizophrenia (p < .05), and this effect was retained at a 12-month follow-up (p < .05). The effect on social distance (p < .05) was shown at post-intervention but not at the follow-up point.

Ahmad et al. (Reference Ahmad, Leventhal, Nielsen and Hinshaw2020) evaluated a school club where students engaged in club activities and meetings and reported that the intervention had overall effects on attitudes towards mental health conditions (intervention group: p = .010; delayed group [i.e., received the intervention later]: p = .004), but from the second to third test time point, the effects were non-significant. A significant overall improvement in positive actions was also found (intervention group: p < .001; delayed group: p < .001). For social distance outcome, significant effects over time were found in the delayed group (p = .037) but did not in the intervention group. Cheetham et al. (Reference Cheetham, Sandral and Lubman2020) reported an intervention providing information on mental health condition and help-seeking did not demonstrate its efficacy over 12 months in reducing stigmatising attitudes towards mental health for both ‘weak not sick’ (p = .171) and ‘dangerousness’ (p = .242). However, compared with the control group, more stigma reduction was reported (p < .001). One study (Vila-Badia et al., Reference Vila-Badia, Martínez-Zambrano, Arenas, Casas-Anguera, García-Morales, Villellas, Martín, Pérez-Franco, Valduciel, Casellas, García-Franco, Miguel, Balsera, Pascual, Julia and Ochoa2016) was contact-only content. They found that the intervention had positive effects on authoritarianism (i.e., a viewpoint that people who are living with mental health conditions are inferior; p = .000) and social restrictiveness (i.e., the attitude that individuals with mental health conditions are a danger to society and should be restricted during or after hospitalisation; p = .019), two factors in The Community Attitudes towards Mental Illness (Taylor and Dear, Reference Taylor and Dear1981), but had no effect on benevolence (i.e., attitudes that include encouragement and paternalism towards people living mental health conditions; p = .742) and community mental health ideology (i.e., beliefs that people with mental health conditions should integrate into society in general; p = .117). Participants in this intervention were shown a documentary film that was related to mental health conditions. Three studies assessed interventions combining education and contact content and obtained mixed effects. Staniland and Byrne (Reference Staniland and Byrne2013) evaluated an intervention to reduce stigmatising attitudes and behaviours towards autistic people. This covered education with both direct and video contact with autistic people. For attitudes, more positive attitudes were found in the intervention group and were maintained at the follow-up point (p = .01). As for behaviours, this intervention failed to work in the intervention group (p = .22), nor did it work when compared to the control group (p = .37). In addition, researchers examined the online activity usage effects and reported no pre-post differences between online activity users’ attitudes and behaviours towards their autistic peers. DeLuca (Reference DeLuca2020) found that their intervention did significantly reduce anticipated stigma (p = .020) across test time points but not self-stigma. This intervention consisted of an educational presentation and a presentation of the personal story of the presenter.

Saporito et al. (Reference Saporito, Ryan and Teachman2011) evaluated the efficacy of an intervention to decrease explicit and implicit stigma around adolescents’ mental health, consisting of a presentation with slides on mental health conditions in young people and a video presentation of a youth currently suffering from a mental health condition. The findings indicated that the intervention had effects on reducing explicit stigma (attitudes to mental health: p = .03; attitudes to treatment: p = .001) but not implicit stigma that was assessed by using automatic associations in memory related to help-seeking and people with mental health conditions. One study’ intervention was education-based, and it had education plus contact as the comparator intervention (Chisholm et al., Reference Chisholm, Patterson, Torgerson, Turner, Jenkinson and Birchwood2016). Those two were allocated to the education and contact condition received educational curriculum-based modules and a contact session working with a young person with experience of a mental health condition. The authors reported that their primary outcome, attitudinal stigma, was significantly decreased in both interventions (education-only condition or education plus contact intervention), but there was no significant effect of either intervention at a 2-week follow-up (p = .5). At a 6-month follow-up, a significant effect was shown in the education-only intervention compared with the education plus contact condition (p = .03).

One study had three interventions: education, contact and printed material (Painter et al., Reference Painter, Phelan, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro and Link2017). A curriculum with active learning and encouragement of empathy was delivered by teachers who introduced stigma-related knowledge and concepts of and coping with specific mental disorders. College students with histories of bipolar disorder were invited to do presentations in the contact section. Printed material consisted of posters focusing on individuals’ personal traits and abilities as opposed to language that labels a person as ‘mentally ill’. Finding showed that there was no effect from using posters (p > .05). Also, compared with the control group, the curriculum-only group had significantly more positive outcomes for 8 of 13 outcomes, and the contact-only group reported less effects than the curriculum-only group.

Two education-only interventions reported no effects. Townsend et al. (Reference Townsend, Musci, Stuart, Heley, Beaudry, Schweizer, Ruble, Swartz and Wilcox2019) implemented an intervention delivered by high school teachers and explored efficacy for increasing depression knowledge and reducing stigma. No main effect of the intervention on stigma scores (p = .08) were found. Howard et al. (Reference Howard, Griffiths, McKetin and Ma2018) evaluated whether education information that described biological or psychological causes of mental-ill health could reduce stigma; neither information on biological nor psychological causes had significant effects on anticipated self-stigma or personal stigma. One study was a contact-related intervention that did not produce a positive effect. Pinto-Foltz et al.’s (Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011) knowledge-contact programme involved narrative story, discussion, and video presentation. They found that stigma did not show reduction after the intervention (p = .33), and at 4- and 8-week follow-ups, there was no significant difference between adolescents in the intervention and control groups.

Intersectional analysis showed that four studies reported findings of gender difference in measured outcomes. Except for Townsend et al. (Reference Townsend, Musci, Stuart, Heley, Beaudry, Schweizer, Ruble, Swartz and Wilcox2019) who did not find a gender difference in stigma reduction, other studies consistently suggested that their interventions had more efficacy in females. Cheetham et al. (Reference Cheetham, Sandral and Lubman2020) reported that female stigma scores decreased over time, and females showed more positive attitudes towards stigma and less social distance than males in Economou et al.’s (Reference Economou, Louki, Peppou, Gramandani, Yotis and Stefanis2012) study. From baseline to follow-up, O’Mara et al. (Reference O’Mara, Akhtar-Danesh, Browne, Mueller, Grypstra, Vrkljan and Powell2013) found that increased stigma scores were found in both males and females, but females increased less than males. No interventions reported details of any analysis or findings with regard to ethnic difference.

Overview

According to these findings, education-based interventions were most likely to have positive effects on stigma reduction in young people, even though some education interventions showed mixed and no effect. Four studies (including Winkler et al., Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017) adopted contact interventions; two reported positive effects (Winkler et al., Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017; Mulfinger et al., Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018) and another two reported mixed (Vila-Badia et al., Reference Vila-Badia, Martínez-Zambrano, Arenas, Casas-Anguera, García-Morales, Villellas, Martín, Pérez-Franco, Valduciel, Casellas, García-Franco, Miguel, Balsera, Pascual, Julia and Ochoa2016) and no effect (Pinto-Foltz et al., Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011) respectively, making it hard to assess effectiveness of contact-only approaches for stigma reduction in this review. Education plus contact interventions could have positive effects on stigma reduction, but these were not significant or stable for long-term effects (e.g., Staniland and Byrne, Reference Staniland and Byrne2013).

Effective intervention components were educational approaches, including lessons, curriculum that consisted of modules explaining stigma-related concepts and strategies, activities such as video games and facilitated discussion, which could be effective in reducing stigma through correcting misinformation on mental health. Also, contact with people with mental health conditions could potentially work for stigma reduction. Apart from one intervention, those with positive effects invited trained teachers or psychologists to deliver interventions, which may have contributed to the efficacy of these anti-stigma interventions.

Meta-analysis

In total, 11 of the 22 studies did not report adequate statistics to be included in the formal analysis. Accordingly, a total of 11 studies were included in our meta-analytic models. Of these, six were education-only (Perry et al., Reference Perry, Petrie, Buckley, Cavanagh, Clarke, Winslade, Hadzi-Pavlovic, Manicavasagar and Christensen2014; Chisholm et al., Reference Chisholm, Patterson, Torgerson, Turner, Jenkinson and Birchwood2016; Milin et al., Reference Milin, Kutcher, Lewis, Walker, Wei, Ferrill and Armstrong2016; Cangas et al., Reference Cangas, Navarro, Parra, Ojeda, Cangas, Piedra and Gallego2017; Howard et al., Reference Howard, Griffiths, McKetin and Ma2018; Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Dang, Bui, Phoeun and Weiss2020), two were contact-only (Pinto-Foltz et al., Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011; Vila-Badia et al., Reference Vila-Badia, Martínez-Zambrano, Arenas, Casas-Anguera, García-Morales, Villellas, Martín, Pérez-Franco, Valduciel, Casellas, García-Franco, Miguel, Balsera, Pascual, Julia and Ochoa2016), two were education plus contact (Staniland and Byrne, Reference Staniland and Byrne2013; DeLuca, Reference DeLuca2020) and one (Winkler et al., Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017) had two anti-stigma interventions (seminar and short video) and an active control group (leaflets). Since some studies had more than one outcome, sample or intervention, these studies were further comprised of studies nested within them. For example, at Post-Intervention, Nguyen et al. (Reference Nguyen, Dang, Bui, Phoeun and Weiss2020) reported results from a Vietnamese sample and a Cambodian sample, thus comprised two nested studies. In total, at Post-Intervention, there were 22 studies nested within the 11 parent studies selected, and at Follow-Up, there were 11 studies nested within the 6 parent studies.

At Pre-Intervention, the pooled effect size based on the three-level meta-analytic model was not significant (d = .008, p = .856). In other words, as expected, there was no discernible difference between control and intervention groups when tested Pre-Intervention. The multivariate model at Post-Intervention revealed a small, significant effect (d = .21, p < .001). Overall, heterogeneity was high (Q (21) = 76.63, p < .001). More specifically, the estimated variance components were τ 2Level 3 = 0.020 and τ 2Level 2 = 0.017, with I 2Level 3 = 40.2% of the total variation attributed to between-cluster, and I 2Level 2 = 33.9% to within-cluster heterogeneity. In other words, approximately a third of variance can be explained by differences within parent studies, with a slightly larger proportion of the variance accounted for by differences between parent studies. At Follow-Up, the multi-variate model revealed that interventions were no longer effective at reducing stigma (d = .069, p = .347). Funnel plots for both models were symmetrical (see Appendix 3 of the Supplementary Material), showing no evidence of publication bias, thus indicating no need for adjustment (Duval and Tweedie, Reference Duval and Tweedie2000). Finally, pooled effect sizes for both Post-Intervention and Follow-Up were considered in a model with time as a moderating factor. We found that the intercept (i.e., Post-Intervention) was significantly different from zero, with a small pooled effect size (d = .212, p < .001). Moreover, the coefficient (i.e., Follow-Up) was negative and significantly different from zero (d = −.128, p = .046), suggesting that there was a significant decline in the efficacy of intervention at Follow-Up. Once again, heterogeneity was high (QE (31) = 99.37, p < .001). More specifically, the estimated variance components were τ 2Level 3 = 0.021 and τ 2Level 2 = 0.015, with I 2Level 3 = 41.26% of the total variation attributed to between-cluster, and I 2Level 2 = 29.3% to within-cluster heterogeneity. Overall, the included interventions had a small effect reducing mental health stigma, but this decayed to no effect in the weeks following intervention delivery (see Figure 2).

Figure 2. Effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions.

Note: Standardised mean differences are presented for each measure of stigma for each study at Post-Intervention (Top) and Follow-Up (Middle). Dots represent the overall effect size, with error bars showing 95% confidence intervals. The colours are common between studies nested within the same parent study. Pooled effect sizes calculated for each model (Separate: Top and Middle; As Moderating Effects: Bottom) are shown as diamonds, which are centred over the pooled effect size, extending as far as the 95% confidence interval.

Risk assessment

Following the Cochrane guidance, the Risk of Bias 2 risk assessment tool was employed to assess risk of bias in randomised controlled trials and cluster-randomised trials (Sterne et al., Reference Sterne, Savović, Page, Elbers, Blencowe, Boutron, Cates, Cheng, Corbett, Eldridge, Emberson, Hernán, Hopewell, Hróbjartsson, Junqueira, Jüni, Kirkham, Lasserson, Li, McAleenan, Reeves, Shepperd, Shrier, Stewart, Tilling, White, Whiting and Higgins2019). The studies that were cluster-randomised trials design were assessed by Risk of Bias 2 for cluster-randomised trials and those randomised controlled trials and experiment designs with randomisation were evaluated by Risk of Bias 2. Overall, the included studies in this review indicated poor quality. No studies were rated in the low-risk category, few studies showed some concerns and others were evaluated as high risk of bias (see Appendix 2 of the Supplementary Material).

Discussion

The study fills a gap in the current evidence on the effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for young people and is the first to conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis exploring the global literature. Of the 22 studies included in this review, more than half of the interventions adopted education-based interventions. Review findings indicated that education-based interventions were most likely to have positive effects compared with contact-based or education plus contact interventions.

It has been argued that it is often ineffective to reduce stigma in the general public using educational programmes alone and that contact-based interventions are more successful than education-based interventions for adults (Corrigan et al., Reference Corrigan, Morris, Michaels, Rafacz and Rüsch2012). Our review showed the opposite findings for young people. A possible reason for this inconsistency is the target population: compared with adults, educational interventions might be more effective for youth because school is the place where young people easily have access to mental health knowledge, and it is routine for young people to have classroom-based learning (Mcluckie et al., Reference Mcluckie, Kutcher, Wei and Weaver2014). There might also be cohort effects in school, where young people influence each other. Additionally, education-based interventions are relatively more economical and easier to deliver than contact-based interventions, for which it can be difficult and time-consuming to prepare people with mental health conditions to participate (Malachowski and Kirsh, Reference Malachowski and Kirsh2013). This is evidence that young people could be suitable audiences for education-based interventions because these could help lay a solid foundation of having positive attitudes towards people with mental health conditions and prevent stigmatising behaviours in adulthood (Corrigan et al., Reference Corrigan, Lurie, Goldman, Slopen, Medasani and Phelan2005).

However, it is worth noting that six education-based interventions showed mixed effects and two were ineffective. This could be related to intervention intensity and the role of trained facilitators. Some education-based interventions with mixed effects had low intensity with only one session (e.g., O’Mara et al., Reference O’Mara, Akhtar-Danesh, Browne, Mueller, Grypstra, Vrkljan and Powell2013). In education-based interventions with mixed/no effect(s), some were delivered by teachers who did not receive training (e.g., Painter et al., Reference Painter, Phelan, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro and Link2017), which could have affected intervention fidelity and efficacy. In contrast, trained teachers or psychology-related professionals participated in interventions with more efficacy (e.g., Winkler et al., Reference Winkler, Janoušková, Kožený, Pasz, Mladá, Weissová, Tušková and Evans-Lacko2017). Moreover, for interventions with positive effects, some interventions included several modules to improve the understanding of relevant concepts of mental health and coping strategies in youth. Students not only received basic knowledge on stigma from facilitators, but also engaged in facilitated discussions improving interactivity. These components that were interactively delivered could have contributed to efficacy (e.g., Milin et al., Reference Milin, Kutcher, Lewis, Walker, Wei, Ferrill and Armstrong2016). Although the previous findings showed the advantages of education-based interventions for stigma reduction by correcting stigmatising attitudes, there is no robust evidence supporting that specific non-stigmatising behaviours can be predicted from attitudes (Crano and Prislin, Reference Crano and Prislin2011). Thus, it is essential to explore other approaches to achieve destigmatising behaviours via further research.

Contact-based interventions did not demonstrate as much effectiveness as education-based interventions. The limited number of contact-based interventions that were included in this review could have made it difficult to evidence the same level of efficacy as for educational-based interventions. Included studies suggest that contact time could be a factor that affects efficacy (Gronholm et al., Reference Gronholm, Henderson, Deb and Thornicroft2017), and longer contact time could be associated with more significant effects. For example, Mulfinger et al. (Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018) had longer intervention contact time than Vila-Badia et al. (Reference Vila-Badia, Martínez-Zambrano, Arenas, Casas-Anguera, García-Morales, Villellas, Martín, Pérez-Franco, Valduciel, Casellas, García-Franco, Miguel, Balsera, Pascual, Julia and Ochoa2016) did, and the former showed more positive effect than the latter. A possible explanation for the inconsistent findings in contact-based interventions could be the differences between measures for mental health stigma which assess different aspects of stigma. Although the included studies all aimed to evaluate interventions for reducing stigma, the measures used varied. For instance, Mulfinger et al. (Reference Mulfinger, Müller, Böge, Sakar, Corrigan, Evans-Lacko, Nehf, Djamali, Samarelli, Kempter, Ruckes, Libal, Oexle, Noterdaeme and Rüsch2018) adopted the Stigma Stress Scale, Vila-Badia et al. (Reference Vila-Badia, Martínez-Zambrano, Arenas, Casas-Anguera, García-Morales, Villellas, Martín, Pérez-Franco, Valduciel, Casellas, García-Franco, Miguel, Balsera, Pascual, Julia and Ochoa2016) used The Community Attitudes towards Mental Illness and Pinto-Foltz et al. (Reference Pinto-Foltz, Logsdon and Myer2011) employed a five-item subscale of the Revised Attribution Questionnaire. Also, interventions were not all delivered in a context with equivalent levels of stigma and it might be easier to show an effect when the levels of stigma are high at baseline. The education plus contact interventions showed mixed effects in this systematic review. This is inconsistent with previous studies showing that it is more effective to use a combination of education and contact interventions instead of employing each of these interventions alone (e.g., Chan et al., Reference Chan, Mak and Law2009). In Chan, Mak and Law’s intervention, they had only one intervention group with three conditions but no control group, which could be a reason for the inconsistency.

Intersectional analysis within included studies focused only one gender, showing that interventions had more efficacy in females. Owing to a lack of detail on ethnicity and differences in stigma reduction within included studies, our review was not able to cover this aspect of diversity among young people. Further research is needed that includes more intersectional analysis of interventions for reducing stigma in young people, including any differences in interventions outcomes for people of different ethnicities.

Stigma reduction, it is argued, needs to be delivered in culturally specific ways, in order to align with ‘what matters most’ in given cultures, and the subsequent impact of stigma on people’s ability to engage in ‘what matters’ in their society (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Kleinman, Link, Phelan, Lee and Good2007). With this in mind, the following three agendas (Corrigan, Reference Corrigan2015; Corrigan and Al-Khouja, Reference Corrigan and Al-Khouja2018) could be considered in future anti-stigma efforts to prevent young people from being excluded from ‘what matters most’ in society. A ‘services agenda’ is associated with promotion of mental health literacy and care-seeking; a ‘rights agenda’ achieves stigma reduction by affirming attitudes and behaviours to replace discrimination; a ‘self-worth’ agenda helps reduce self-stigma by promoting self-affirming attitudes in place of shame. Our reviewed findings indicate that interventions focused on these agendas included promoting mental health literacy, non-discriminatory attitudes towards mental health and reducing stigmatising behaviours, and reported positive effects in some studies (e.g., Milin et al., Reference Milin, Kutcher, Lewis, Walker, Wei, Ferrill and Armstrong2016; Cangas et al., Reference Cangas, Navarro, Parra, Ojeda, Cangas, Piedra and Gallego2017; Link et al., Reference Link, DuPont-Reyes, Barkin, Villatoro, Phelan and Painter2020). In particular, interventions based on a rights agenda and a self-worth agenda demonstrated more efficacy in this review. This suggests that such interventions would support young people with mental health conditions would be more able to take part in ‘what matters most’ in their culture. Intervention content would, however, need to be tailored to the form and causes of mental health stigma in any specific cultural context.

Risk assessment

The overall quality of included studies was poor, and the primary concern was randomisation as only few of studies reported how allocation to intervention and control groups took place. Other concerns were ‘Deviations from the intended interventions’, which could be ‘the administration of additional interventions that are inconsistent with the trial protocol, failure to implement the protocol interventions as intended or non-adherence by trial participants to their assigned intervention’ (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins, Sterne, Savović, Page, Hróbjartsson, Boutron, Reeves, Eldridge, Chandler, McKenzie, Boutron and Welch2016). In other assessment categories including report of missing data, measurement of outcome and results report, most studies were rated as having some concerns as there was a lack of information demonstrating how authors dealt with these issues. These omissions were possibly due to limited word counts for the publications, which adversely affected the risk assessment.

Limitations

This is the first systematic review to look at effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions among young people. However, it has some limitations related to capacity within the research team. The grey literature was not searched, and aside from English and Chinese publications, studies written in other foreign languages were excluded for the review. This could have led to publication bias in terms of studies with null or negative results. The quality assessment was conducted by two assessors but may have been limited by only the first author being involved data extraction. This review did not differentiate between types of stigma, and further research could usefully investigate the effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for different types of stigma. Moreover, the high heterogeneity means estimates of the pooled effect sizes from our models are less reliable than if study designs had been more similar. Finally, the overall poor quality of the included studies may mean that conclusions from the review are not generalisable and definitive recommendations on the effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for young people require additional research.

Implications

This review has provided evidence that anti-stigma interventions were effective overall but yielded small improvements that did not endure in the long term. Findings indicate that education-based interventions showed advantages for reducing stigma in youth compared with other interventions. Thus, incorporating education-related approaches, such as having interactive discussions, workshops and seminars, is recommended when developing anti-stigma interventions. Our results suggest the importance of intervention intensity for education-based interventions. More sessions could contribute to positive and stable effects on reducing stigma in youth. Findings from the meta-analysis show that the intervention conducted by Cangas et al. (Reference Cangas, Navarro, Parra, Ojeda, Cangas, Piedra and Gallego2017) had a larger effect than other approaches. This suggests that educational components could usefully include video games as an effective and innovative approach to reducing mental health stigma perpetuated by young people.

Schools have been identified as important sites to deliver mental health and well-being campaigns (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Stapley, Hayes, Town and Deighton2022). Findings from this review confirm that schools are potentially effective sites from which to reach young people to reduce mental health stigma, however, the complexity of school settings needs to be considered. Prioritising other tasks, such as academic achievements, over mental health and well-being interventions, has been identified as an obstacle to effectiveness (Nadeem and Ringle, Reference Nadeem and Ringle2016; Dijkman et al., Reference Dijkman, Harting, van Tol and van der Wal2017; Crane et al., Reference Crane, Phillips, Maxwell, Norris, Rifkin, Blank, Sorid, Read, Swan, Kendall and Frank2021). Staff turnover is another barrier to the sustainability of school-based interventions (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Stapley, Hayes, Town and Deighton2022). Low intervention fidelity has been reported from school-based mental health and well-being interventions when an intervention was delivered partially by teachers who had not been trained or received materials (Friend et al., Reference Friend, Flattum, Simpson, Nederhoff, Neumark-Sztainer and Neumark-Sztaine2014). This is consistent with our review findings that those anti-stigma interventions that were delivered by trained teachers or psychology-related professionals were more effective. Additionally, school staff capacity to deliver interventions and the impact of this on intervention sustainability has been questioned (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Stapley, Hayes, Town and Deighton2022). For example, in one study, adequate supervision was not provided by coordinators to teachers (Dijkman et al., Reference Dijkman, Harting, van Tol and van der Wal2017). There is evidence that engaged school leaders who were inclined to provide support and encouragement for school staff in the use of an intervention could facilitate its implementation (Hudson et al., Reference Hudson, Lawton and Hugh-Jones2020). This suggests that, to develop and deliver anti-stigma interventions in school settings, it is important to have support from school-based needs and decision-makers as a key factor.

The evidence base for anti-stigma interventions is itself a further issue highlighted by the review. With regard to the countries in which intervention studies were conducted in this review, only one study (Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Dang, Bui, Phoeun and Weiss2020) was conducted in an Asian country and all other intervention studies were in Western countries. Thus, this review provides most evidence for anti-stigma interventions targeting youth in a Western context. To determine the components of effective anti-sigma interventions for youth in other contexts, and particularly in low- and middle-income countries, more studies on mental health stigma in young people need to be conducted in those settings.

Open peer review

To view the open peer review materials for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/gmh.2023.34.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/gmh.2023.34.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, N.S., upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by Amy Palmer who was the second reviewer for screening full-text publications and Kier Harris who was the second assessor for a double quality assessment.

Author contribution

N. S. led the development of the protocol for this review, conducted the database searches, screened all studies, extracted data and drafted this article. G. M. contributed to the protocol development, double-screened 50% of included studies and contributed to the development of each draft of the manuscript. S. H.-J. contributed to the protocol development, double-screened the remaining 50% of included studies and contributed to the development of each draft of the manuscript. J. P. conducted the statistical analysis and visualisation and contributed to the interpretation of the results. R. M. W. gave guidance on the statistical analysis and made final edits to the manuscript. All authors have approved the final article.

Financial support

This review did not receive any funding.

Competing interest

None of the authors has a competing interest related to the present study.

References

Ahmad, SI, Leventhal, BL, Nielsen, BN and Hinshaw, SP (2020) Reducing mental-illness stigma via high school clubs: A matched-pair, cluster-randomized trial. Stigma and Health 5(2), 230239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akouri-Shan, LA, DeLuca, JS, Pitts, SC, Jay, SY, Redman, SL, Petti, E, Bridgwater, MA, Rakhshan Rouhakhtar, PJ, Klaunig, MJ, Chibani, D, Martin, EA, Reeves, GM and Schiffman, J (2022) Internalized stigma mediates the relation between psychosis-risk symptoms and subjective quality of life in a help-seeking sample. Schizophrenia Research 241, 298305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, GW (1954) The Nature of Prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Amir-Behghadami, M and Janati, A (2020) Population, intervention, comparison, outcomes and study (PICOS) design as a framework to formulate eligibility criteria in systematic reviews. Emergency Medicine Journal 37, 387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calear, AL, Batterham, PJ, Torok, M and McCallum, S (2021) Help-seeking attitudes and intentions for generalised anxiety disorder in adolescents: The role of anxiety literacy and stigma. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 30, 243251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cangas, AJ, Navarro, N, Parra, JMA, Ojeda, JJ, Cangas, D, Piedra, JA and Gallego, J (2017) Stigma-stop: A serious game against the stigma toward mental health in educational settings. Frontiers in Psychology 8, 1385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, JN, Mak, WS and Law, LC (2009) Combining education and video-based contact to reduce stigma of mental illness: ‘The same or not the same’ anti-stigma program for secondary schools in Hong Kong. Social Science and Medicine 68, 15211526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheetham, A, Sandral, E and Lubman, DI (2020) Outcomes of reducing stigma towards alcohol misuse during adolescence: Results of a randomized controlled trial of the MAKINGtheLINK intervention. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 14, 11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chisholm, K, Patterson, P, Torgerson, C, Turner, E, Jenkinson, D and Birchwood, M (2016) Impact of contact on adolescents’ mental health literacy and stigma: The SchoolSpace cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ 6(2), e009435.Google ScholarPubMed
Cochrane (2021) Developmental, Psychological and Learning Problems. Available at https://dplp.cochrane.org/data-extraction-forms (accessed: 15 May 2021).Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Corrigan, PW (2002) Empowerment and serious mental illness: Treatment partnerships and community opportunities. Psychiatric Quarterly 73, 217228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, PW (2015) Challenging the stigma of mental illness: Different agendas. Different Goals Psychiatric Services 66, 13471349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, PW and Al-Khouja, MA (2018) Three agendas for changing the public stigma of mental illness. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal 41(1), 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, PW, Lurie, BD, Goldman, HH, Slopen, N, Medasani, K and Phelan, S (2005) How adolescents perceive the stigma of mental illness and alcohol abuse. Psychiatric Services 56, 544550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, PW, Morris, SB, Michaels, PJ, Rafacz, JD and Rüsch, N (2012) Challenging the public stigma of mental illness: A meta-analysis of outcome studies. Psychiatric Services 63, 963973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, PW and Penn, DL (1999) Lessons from social psychology on discrediting psychiatric stigma. American Psychologist 54, 765776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, PW and Watson, AC (2002) Understanding the impact of stigma on people with mental illness. World Psychiatry 1(1), 1620.Google ScholarPubMed
Crane, ME, Phillips, KE, Maxwell, CA, Norris, LA, Rifkin, LS, Blank, JM, Sorid, SD, Read, KL, Swan, AJ, Kendall, PC and Frank, HE (2021) A qualitative examination of a school-based implementation of computer-assisted cognitive-behavioral therapy for child anxiety. School Mental Health 13, 347361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crano, WD and Prislin, R (2011) Attitudes and Attitude Change, 1st Edn. New York: Taylor and Francis.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLuca, JS (2020) Developmental Predictors of Adolescent Mental Health Stigma and a Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial of ‘Ending the Silence’ in New York City, Vol. 2020. New York: CUNY Academic Works.Google Scholar
Dijkman, MAM, Harting, J, van Tol, L and van der Wal, MF (2017) Sustainability of the good behaviour game in Dutch primary schools. Health Promotion International 32, 7990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duval, SJ and Tweedie, RL (2000) Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 56(2), 455463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Economou, M, Louki, E, Peppou, LE, Gramandani, C, Yotis, L and Stefanis, CN (2012) Fighting psychiatric stigma in the classroom: The impact of an educational intervention on secondary school students’ attitudes to schizophrenia. The International Journal of Social Psychiatry 58(5), 544551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Economou, M, Peppou, LE, Geroulanou, K, Louki, E, Tsaliagkou, I, Kolostoumpis, D and Stefanis, CN (2014) The influence of an anti-stigma intervention on adolescents’ attitudes to schizophrenia: A mixed methodology approach. Child and Adolescent Mental Health 19(1), 1623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elkington, KS, Hackler, D, McKinnon, K, Borges, C, Wright, ER and Wainberg, ML (2012) Perceived mental illness stigma among youth in psychiatric outpatient treatment. Journal of Adolescent Research 27(2), 290317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferriea, J, Millera, H and Hunter, SC (2020) Psychosocial outcomes of mental illness stigma in children and adolescents: A mixed-methods systematic review. Children and Youth Services Review 113, 104961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, S and Hood, BM (1987) The stress of the transition to university: A longitudinal study of psychological disturbance, absent-mindedness and vulnerability to homesickness. British Journal of Psychology 78(4), 425441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friend, S, Flattum, CF, Simpson, D, Nederhoff, D, Neumark-Sztainer, D and Neumark-Sztaine, D (2014) The researchers have left the building: What contributes to sustaining school-based interventions following the conclusion of formal research support? Journal of School Health 84, 326333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E (1963) Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Gonçalves, M, Moleiro, C and Cook, B (2015) The use of a video to reduce mental health stigma among adolescents. Adolescent Psychiatry 5, 11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gronholm, PC, Henderson, C, Deb, T and Thornicroft, G (2017) Interventions to reduce discrimination and stigma: The state of the art. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 52, 249258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrer, M, Cuijpers, P, Furukawa, TA and Ebert, DD (2021) Doing Meta-Analysis with R: A Hands-On Guide. Boca Raton, FL and London: Chapman & Hall/CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartoga, K, Hubbardc, CD, Krouwera, AF, Thornicroftd, G, Kohrte, BA and Jordans, MJD (2020) Stigma reduction interventions for children and adolescents in low- and middle-income countries: Systematic review of intervention strategies. Social Science & Medicine 246, 112749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heflinger, CA and Hinshaw, SP (2016) Stigma in child and adolescent mental health services research: Understanding professional and institutional stigmatization of youth with mental health problems and their families. Administration and Policy in Mental Health 37, 6170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, JPT, Sterne, JAC, Savović, J, Page, MJ, Hróbjartsson, A, Boutron, I, Reeves, B and Eldridge, S (2016) A revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomized trials. In Chandler, J, McKenzie, J, Boutron, I and Welch, V (eds.), Cochrane Methods. Chichester, UK: Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, pp. 2931.Google Scholar
Howard, KA, Griffiths, KM, McKetin, R and Ma, J (2018) Can a brief biologically based psychoeducational intervention reduce stigma and increase help-seeking intentions for depression in young people? A randomised controlled trial. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health 30(1), 2739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudson, KG, Lawton, R and Hugh-Jones, S (2020) Factors affecting the implementation of a whole school mindfulness program: A qualitative study using the consolidated framework for implementation research. BMC Health Services Research 20, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janoušková, M, Tušková, E, Weissová, A, Trančík, P, Pasz, J, Evans-Lacko, S and Winkler, P (2017) Can video interventions be used to effectively destigmatize mental illness among young people? A systematic review. European Psychiatry 41, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaushik, A, Kostaki, E and Kyriakopoulos, M (2016) The stigma of mental illness in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Psychiatry Research 243, 469494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Angermeyer, M, Anthony, JC, de Graaf, R, Demyttenaere, K, Gasquet, I, de Girolamo, G, Gluzman, S, Gureje, O, Haro, JM, Kawakami, N, Karam, A, Levinson, D, Medina Mora, ME, Oakley Browne, MA, Posada-Villa, J, Stein, DJ, Adley Tsang, CH, Aguilar-Gaxiola, S, Alonso, J, Lee, S, Heeringa, S, Pennell, BE, Berglund, P, Gruber, MJ, Petukhova, M, Chatterji, S and Ustün, TB (2007) Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of mental disorders in the World Health Organization’s world mental health survey initiative. World Psychiatry 6, 168176.Google ScholarPubMed
Kleinman, A (2006) What Really Matters: Living a Moral Life Amidst Uncertainty and Danger. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Link, BG (1987) Understanding labeling effects in the area of mental disorders: An assessment of the effects of expectations of rejection. American Sociological Review 52, 96112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Link, BG, DuPont-Reyes, MJ, Barkin, K, Villatoro, AP, Phelan, JC and Painter, K (2020) A school-based intervention for mental illness stigma: A cluster randomized trial. Pediatrics 145(6), e20190780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, JD and Boyed, JE (2010) Correlates and consequences of internalized stigma for people living with mental illness: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Social Science & Medicine 75(12), 21502161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malachowski, C and Kirsh, B (2013) Workplace antistigma initiatives: A scoping study. Psychiatric Services 64, 694702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mcluckie, A, Kutcher, S, Wei, Y and Weaver, C (2014) Sustained improvements in students’ mental health literacy with use of a mental health curriculum in Canadian schools. BMC Psychiatry 14, 379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellor, C (2014) School-based interventions targeting stigma of mental illness: Systematic review. Psychiatric Bulletin 38, 164171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milin, R, Kutcher, S, Lewis, SP, Walker, S, Wei, Y, Ferrill, N and Armstrong, MA (2016). Impact of a mental health curriculum on knowledge and stigma among high school students: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 55(5), 383391.e1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitten, N, Preyde, M, Lewis, S, Vanderkooy, J and Heintzman, J (2016) The perceptions of adolescents who self-harm on stigma and care following inpatient psychiatric treatment. Social Work in Mental Health 14(1), 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, A, Stapley, E, Hayes, D, Town, R and Deighton, J (2022) Barriers and facilitators to sustaining school-based mental health and wellbeing interventions: A systematic review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19(6), 3587.Google ScholarPubMed
Morgan, AJ, Reavley, NJ, Ross, A, Too, LS and Jorm, AF (2018) Interventions to reduce stigma towards people with severe mental illness: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Psychiatric Research 103, 120133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moses, T (2010) Being treated differently: Stigma experiences with family, peers, and school staff among adolescents with mental health disorders. Social Science & Medicine 70, 985993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulfinger, N, Müller, S, Böge, I, Sakar, V, Corrigan, PW, Evans-Lacko, S, Nehf, L, Djamali, J, Samarelli, A, Kempter, M, Ruckes, C, Libal, G, Oexle, N, Noterdaeme, M and Rüsch, N (2018) Honest, open, proud for adolescents with mental illness: Pilot randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 59(6), 684691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadeem, E and Ringle, VA (2016) De-adoption of an evidence-based trauma intervention in schools: A retrospective report from an urban school district. School Mental Health 8, 132143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nearchou, FA, Bird, N, Costello, A, Duggan, S, Gilroy, J, Long, R, McHugh, L and Hennessy, E (2018) Personal and perceived public mental-health stigma as predictors of help-seeking intentions in adolescents. Journal of Adolescence 66, 8390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, AJ, Dang, HM, Bui, D, Phoeun, B and Weiss, B (2020) Experimental evaluation of a school-based mental health literacy program in two southeast Asian nations. School Mental Health 12, 716731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Mara, L, Akhtar-Danesh, N, Browne, G, Mueller, D, Grypstra, L, Vrkljan, C and Powell, M (2013) Does youth net decrease mental illness stigma in high school students. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health 32, 922.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ouzzani, M, Hammady, H, Fedorowicz, Z and Elmagarmid, A (2016) Rayyan – a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Systematic Reviews 5, 210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Page, MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, Shamseer, L, Tetzlaff, JM, Akl, EA, Brennan, SE, Chou, R, Glanville, J, Grimshaw, JM, Hróbjartsson, A, Lalu, MM, Li, T, Loder, EW, Mayo-Wilson, E, McDonald, S, McGuinness, LA, Stewart, LA, Thomas, J, Tricco, AC, Welch, VA, Whiting, P and Moher, D (2021) The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372, n71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Painter, K, Phelan, JC, DuPont-Reyes, MJ, Barkin, KF, Villatoro, AP and Link, BG (2017) Evaluation of antistigma interventions with sixth-grade students: A school-based field experiment. Psychiatric Services (Washington, DC) 68(4), 345352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, K, Lee, MH and Seo, M (2019) The impact of self-stigma on self-esteem among persons with different mental disorders. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 65(7–8), 558565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patton, GC, Sawyer, SM, Santelli, JS, Ross, DA, Afifi, R, Allen, NB, Arora, M, Azzopardi, P, Baldwin, W, Bonell, C, Kakuma, R, Kennedy, E, Mahon, J, McGovern, T, Mokdad, AH, Patel, V, Petroni, S, Reavley, N, Taiwo, K, Waldfogel, J, Wickremarathne, D, Barroso, C, Bhutta, Z, Fatusi, AO, Mattoo, A, Diers, J, Fang, J, Ferguson, J, Ssewamala, F and Viner, RM (2016) Our future: A Lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing. The Lancet 387(10036), 24232478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pederson, AB, Burnett-Zeigler, I, Fokuo, JK, Wisner, KL, Zumpf, K and Oshodi, Y (2020) Mental health stigma among university health care students in Nigeria: A cross-sectional observational study. The Pan African Medical Journal 37, 5.Google ScholarPubMed
Perry, Y, Petrie, K, Buckley, H, Cavanagh, L, Clarke, D, Winslade, M, Hadzi-Pavlovic, D, Manicavasagar, V and Christensen, H (2014) Effects of a classroom-based educational resource on adolescent mental health literacy: A cluster randomized controlled trial. Journal of Adolescence 37(7), 11431151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, TF and Tropp, LR (2008) How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta-analytic tests of three mediators. European Journal of Social Psychology 38(6), 922934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinto-Foltz, MD, Logsdon, MC and Myer, JA (2011) Feasibility, acceptability, and initial efficacy of a knowledge-contact program to reduce mental illness stigma and improve mental health literacy in adolescents. Social Science & Medicine 72(12), 20112019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, R (2013) Cochrane Consumers and Communication Review Group. ‘Cochrane Consumers and Communication Review Group: Data Synthesis and Analysis’. Available at http://cccrg.cochrane.org (accessed 16 May 2022).Google Scholar
Sakellari, E, Leino-Kilpi, H and Kalokerinou-Anagnostopoulou, A (2011) Educational interventions in secondary education aiming to affect pupils’ attitudes towards mental illness: A review of the literature. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 18, 166176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saporito, JM, Ryan, C and Teachman, BA (2011) Reducing stigma toward seeking mental health treatment among adolescents. Stigma Research and Action 1(2), 921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shechtmana, Z, Vogelb, DL, Strassb, HA and Heathb, PJ (2018) Stigma in help-seeking: The case of adolescents. British Journal of Guidance & Counselling 46(1), 104119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staniland, JJ and Byrne, MK (2013) The effects of a multi-component higher-functioning autism anti-stigma program on adolescent boys. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43, 28162829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterne, JAC, Savović, J, Page, MJ, Elbers, RG, Blencowe, NS, Boutron, I, Cates, CJ, Cheng, HY, Corbett, MS, Eldridge, SM, Emberson, JR, Hernán, MA, Hopewell, S, Hróbjartsson, A, Junqueira, DR, Jüni, P, Kirkham, JJ, Lasserson, T, Li, T, McAleenan, A, Reeves, BC, Shepperd, S, Shrier, I, Stewart, LA, Tilling, K, White, IR, Whiting, PF and Higgins, JPT (2019) RoB 2: A revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 366, l4898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, SM and Dear, MJ (1981) Scaling community attitudes toward the mentally ill. Schizophrenia Bulletin 7(2), 225240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, L, Musci, R, Stuart, E, Heley, K, Beaudry, MB, Schweizer, B, Ruble, A, Swartz, K and Wilcox, H (2019) Gender differences in depression literacy and stigma following a randomized controlled evaluation of a universal depression education program. The Journal of Adolescent Health 64(4), 472477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vila-Badia, R, Martínez-Zambrano, F, Arenas, O, Casas-Anguera, E, García-Morales, E, Villellas, R, Martín, JR, Pérez-Franco, MB, Valduciel, T, Casellas, D, García-Franco, M, Miguel, J, Balsera, J, Pascual, G, Julia, E and Ochoa, S (2016) Effectiveness of an intervention for reducing social stigma towards mental illness in adolescents. World Journal of Psychiatry 6(2), 239247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winkler, P, Janoušková, M, Kožený, J, Pasz, J, Mladá, K, Weissová, A, Tušková, E and Evans-Lacko, S (2017) Short video interventions to reduce mental health stigma: A multi-centre randomised controlled trial in nursing high schools. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 52(12), 15491557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organisation (2021) Mental Health of Adolescents. Available at https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/adolescent-mental-health (accessed 11 May 2022).Google Scholar
Yang, LH, Kleinman, A, Link, BG, Phelan, JC, Lee, S and Good, B (2007) Culture and stigma: Adding moral experience to stigma theory. Social Science & Medicine 64, 15241535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, YS, Yen, JY, Ko, CH, Cheng, CP and Yen, CF (2010) The association between problematic cellular phone use and risky behaviors and low self-esteem among Taiwanese adolescents. BMC Public Health 10(1), 217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yap, MBH, Wright, A and Jorm, AF (2011) The influence of stigma on young people’s help-seeking intentions and beliefs about the helpfulness of various sources of help. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 46, 12571265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Eligibility and exclusion criteria

Figure 1

Table 2. Characteristics of included studies

Figure 2

Table 3. Outcomes and intervention sessions

Figure 3

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram.

Figure 4

Figure 2. Effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions.Note: Standardised mean differences are presented for each measure of stigma for each study at Post-Intervention (Top) and Follow-Up (Middle). Dots represent the overall effect size, with error bars showing 95% confidence intervals. The colours are common between studies nested within the same parent study. Pooled effect sizes calculated for each model (Separate: Top and Middle; As Moderating Effects: Bottom) are shown as diamonds, which are centred over the pooled effect size, extending as far as the 95% confidence interval.

Supplementary material: Image

Song et al. supplementary material

Song et al. supplementary material 1

Download Song et al. supplementary material(Image)
Image 195.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Song et al. supplementary material

Song et al. supplementary material 2

Download Song et al. supplementary material(File)
File 17.1 KB
Supplementary material: Image

Song et al. supplementary material

Song et al. supplementary material 3

Download Song et al. supplementary material(Image)
Image 280.7 KB
Supplementary material: Image

Song et al. supplementary material

Song et al. supplementary material 4

Download Song et al. supplementary material(Image)
Image 2.1 MB

Author comment: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R0/PR1

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Review: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R0/PR2

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

Dear authors, your article is of great scientific interest and relevance. The abstract and article are clearly written, giving a complete description and perspective.

However, the following comments and suggestions should be made explicit and incorporated:

1. Methods.

- You must explain and include the characteristics and specifications of the manual search performed.

- It is necessary to indicate the dates during which the search for articles was carried out, since this will provide information for the replicability of the study. Likewise, it is relevant to indicate the dates on which a manual search of the articles was carried out.

-Was a manual trace back literature search performed after the date of the search to identify new literature that may have been published?

-Since the search and selection of articles was carried out between 2 authors, it is relevant to indicate the degree of agreement between them, through the measurement of interrater reliability (e.g. using the Cohen κ coefficient).

- It is highly recommended to estimate the measure of publication bias (e.g. through the trim-and-fill method for publication bias).

2. Limitations:

- It is relevant to note that gray literature was not included in the search and selection of studies, which generates an increased risk of publication bias (because studies with null or negative results tend not to be published or submitted to scientific journals).

-It is not at all clear to me whether quasi-experimental studies (non-randomized but with control and experimental groups) were included, as this may bias the effect size and effectiveness of these studies.

Review: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R0/PR3

Conflict of interest statement

No competing interests

Comments

I appreciate the opportunity to review this timely and important paper on the effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people. Thank you for allowing me to provide feedback and suggestions for improving the clarity and effectiveness of the paper.

Overall, the abstract provides a clear and well-written overview of the study, highlighting the issue of mental health stigma among young people and the effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions. The introduction effectively sets the stage for the paper, citing relevant studies and research to support the prevalence of mental health disorders and the impact of stigma on seeking help and receiving treatment.

The methods section clearly explain how the paper follows established guidelines for systematic reviews, and the results section provides sufficient detail on the interventions and their effectiveness in reducing mental health stigma.

Some things to consider:

Some concepts introduced in the results section may be confusing and require further explanation. Some examples: “Stigma stress” (l 334), “benevolence” (l 366), “community mental health ideology” (l 366), etc. These concepts come from the reviewed articles but should be explained to ensure everything is clear.

In the discussion, lines 559-562, it is indicated that it is more challenging to reduce stigma in the USA and Australia compared to other countries in the review. This claim should be substantiated.

The study found that education-based interventions were most likely to have positive effects compared to contact-based or education plus contact interventions. It is argued that this is due to the target population, young people, may be more receptive to education-based interventions because school is the place where they can easily access mental health knowledge. The discussion section could benefit from further elaboration on the implications of working more closely with schools for awareness raising in stigma. The complexity of schools as over-interveened spaces with different normative agendas, and with staff working in precarious conditions should also be recognized.

The existence of stigmatizing thoughts and attitudes towards persons with mental illness responds to each context’s cultural and social features. Link’s work around “what matters most” in different contexts points to this fundamental fact. It reveals that mental illness does not necessarily mean stigma: what is stigmatized depends on what is valuable about being a human in different settings and what is expected from fellow humans. This also means that public action against stigma has different forms and is moved by different agendas depending on each context. Sheehan and Corrigan have identified a) A services agenda, which seeks to decrease stigma so people better engage in care, b) a rights agenda, which seeks to stem the injustice of stigma so people are able to meet life goals and aspirations, and c) self-worth agenda, which strives to replace shame with self-affirming attitudes in people impacted by stigma.

I ask the authors to engage with these aspects of stigma, recognizing that what stigma is, how it should be addressed, and the effectiveness of different strategies in the long and short run is all context-dependent.

Overall, the paper is well-written and informative, but could benefit from further elaboration and explanation on some concepts and implications.

Recommendation: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R0/PR4

Comments

Dear authors,

Thank you for submitting this manuscript.

It addresses a relevant issue in the field as there is a need to learn more about what works to reduce stigma in diverse contexts.

The reviewers have noted, however, that your manuscript could be benefit from further elaboration and clarification of key concepts, methods, and findings.

Looking forward to reading the revised manuscript.

Best,

Franco Mascayano

Decision: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R0/PR5

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Author comment: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R1/PR6

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Review: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R1/PR7

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

Dear authors, I appreciate the effort made and the inclusion of the reviewers' comments in your article.

Review: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R1/PR8

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

I am pleased to say that the authors have thoroughly addressed the concerns raised in my initial review. The revisions and amendments made have significantly improved the overall quality of the manuscript. Consequently, I believe it is now in a suitable state for publication.

This piece does an excellent job synthesizing a wide array of research in an accessible manner, making it a significant contribution to our current understanding of school interventions for reducing stigma. The manuscript’s focus on young people’s mental health stigma - a topic of undeniable importance - further enhances its value.

I would like to express my gratitude for the opportunity to review this piece. It has been a privilege to be a part of the process and to witness the development of such a critical piece of scholarship.

Thank you once again for the opportunity. I look forward to seeing this work shared with the wider academic community.

Recommendation: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R1/PR9

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Decision: The effectiveness of anti-stigma interventions for reducing mental health stigma in young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis — R1/PR10

Comments

No accompanying comment.