Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-06T10:34:51.028Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Faecal glucocorticoid level is not correlated with stereotypic pacing in two captive margays (Leopardus wiedii)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

M Gusset*
Affiliation:
Physiology and Animal Husbandry Research Group, Institute of Animal Sciences, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, 8092 Zurich, Switzerland
*
Correspondence address: Applied Behaviour and Ecology Laboratory, Department of Zoology, University of Transkei, Private Bag X1, UNITRA 5117, South Africa; e-mail: mgusset@bluewin.ch

Abstract

The ‘coping hypothesis’ of stereotypic behaviour — that stereotypies are performed as a means of helping the animal to cope with its environment by reducing stress — was tested using two adult female margays (Leopardus wiedii), an endangered neotropical small cat species. Within-individual and between-individual comparisons of the duration of stereotypic pacing and glucocorticoid concentration, measured non-invasively in faeces, indicated that stereotypic pacing did not help the two margays to cope with their captive environment by reducing their physiological stress level. However, hiding appeared to serve as a coping function in the two margays, not by immediately reducing the faecal glucocorticoid concentration, but rather as a long-term effect.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Carlstead, K, Brown, JL and Seidensticker, J 1993a Behavioral and adrenocortical responses to environmental changes in leopard cats (Felis bengalensis). Zoo Biology 12: 321331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K, Brown, JL and Strawn, W 1993b Behavioral and physiological correlates of stress in laboratory cats. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 38: 143158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K, Brown, JL, Monfort, SL, Killens, R and Wildt, DE 1992 Urinary monitoring of adrenal responses to psychological stressors in domestic and nondomestic felids. Zoo Biology 11: 165176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dantzer, R 1991 Stress, stereotypies and welfare. Behavioural Processes 25: 95102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dantzer, R and Mittleman, G 1993 Functional consequences of behavioural stereotypy. In: Lawrence, AB and Rushen, J (eds) Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare pp 147172. CAB International: Wallingford, UKGoogle Scholar
Graham, LH and Brown, JL 1996 Cortisol metabolism in the domestic cat and implications for non-invasive monitoring of adrenocortical function in endangered felids. Zoo Biology 15: 71823.0.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gusset, M, Burgener, N and Schmid, H 2002 Wirkung einer aktiven Futterbeschaffung mittels Futterkisten auf das stereotype Gehen und den Glukokortikoidspiegel von Margays, Leopardus wiedii, im Zoo Zürich. Der Zoologische Garten N. F. 72: 245262 [Title translation: Effects of an active foraging device by means of feeding boxes on the stereotypic pacing and faecal glucocorticoid level in margays, Leopardus wiedii, in the Zurich Zoo]Google Scholar
Hartmann-Furter, M 2000 A species-specific feeding technique designed for European wildcats (Felis s. silvestris) in captivity. Säugetierkundliche Informationen 4: 567575Google Scholar
Jenny, S and Schmid, H 2002 Effect of feeding boxes on the behavior of stereotyping Amur tigers (Panthera tigris altaica) in the Zurich Zoo, Zurich, Switzerland. Zoo Biology 21: 573584CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladewig, J, De Passillé, AM, Rushen, J, Schouten, W, Terlouw, EMC and Von Borell, E 1993 Stress and the physiological correlates of stereotypic behaviour. In: Lawrence, AB and Rushen, J (eds) Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare pp 97118. CAB International: Wallingford, UKGoogle Scholar
Martin, P and Bateson, P 1993 Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide, 2nd Edition. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, GJ 1991 Stereotypies: a critical review. Animal Behaviour 41: 10151037CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, K and Jackson, P 1996 Wild Cats: Status Survey and Conservation Action Plan. IUCN: Gland, SwitzerlandGoogle Scholar
Palme, R and Möstl, E 1997 Measurement of cortisol metabolites in faeces of sheep as a parameter of cortisol concentration in blood. International Journal of Mammalian Biology 62(Suppl 2): 192197Google Scholar
Rushen, J 1993 The ‘coping’ hypothesis of stereotypic behaviour. Animal Behaviour 45: 613615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schatz, S and Palme, R 2001 Measurement of faecal cortisol metabolites in cats and dogs: a non-invasive method for evaluating adrenocortical function. Veterinary Research Communications 25: 271287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, S and Castellan, NJ Jr 1988 Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edition. McGraw-Hill: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, B 1995 Coping and coping strategies: a behavioural view. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 43: 123134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wielebnowski, NC, Fletchall, N, Carlstead, K, Busso, JM and Brown, JL 2002 Noninvasive assessment of adrenal activity associated with husbandry and behavioral factors in the North American clouded leopard population. Zoo Biology 21: 7798CrossRefGoogle Scholar