Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T07:46:30.391Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Childhood trauma is associated with reduced frontal gray matter volume: a large transdiagnostic structural MRI study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 June 2021

Marieke J. H. Begemann*
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells & Systems, Section Cognitive Neurosciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands
Maya J. L. Schutte
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells & Systems, Section Cognitive Neurosciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands
Edwin van Dellen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UMCU Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands
Lucija Abramovic
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UMCU Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands
Marco P. Boks
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UMCU Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands
Neeltje E. M. van Haren
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UMCU Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychology, Erasmus University Medical Center-Sophia Children's Hospital, Rotterdam, the Netherlands
Rene C. W. Mandl
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UMCU Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands
Christiaan H. Vinkers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC (location VUmc), Amsterdam, the Netherlands Department of Anatomy and Neurosciences, Amsterdam UMC (location VUmc), Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Marc M. Bohlken
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, UMCU Brain Center, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands
Iris E. C. Sommer
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical Sciences of Cells & Systems, Section Cognitive Neurosciences, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands
*
Author for correspondence: Marieke J. H. Begemann, E-mail: m.j.h.begemann@umcg.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Childhood trauma increases risk for psychopathology and cognitive impairment. Prior research mainly focused on the hippocampus and amygdala in single diagnostic categories. However, other brain regions may be impacted by trauma as well, and effects may be independent of diagnosis. This cross-sectional study investigated cortical and subcortical gray matter volume in relation to childhood trauma severity.

Methods

We included 554 participants: 250 bipolar-I patients, 84 schizophrenia-spectrum patients and 220 healthy individuals without a psychiatric history. Participants filled in the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Anatomical T1 MRI scans were acquired at 3T, regional brain morphology was assessed using Freesurfer.

Results

In the total sample, trauma-related gray matter reductions were found in the frontal lobe (β = −0.049, p = 0.008; q = 0.048), this effect was driven by the right medial orbitofrontal, paracentral, superior frontal regions and the left precentral region. No trauma-related volume reductions were observed in any other (sub)cortical lobes nor the hippocampus or amygdala, trauma-by-group (i.e. both patient groups and healthy subjects) interaction effects were absent. A categorical approach confirmed a pattern of more pronounced frontal gray matter reductions in individuals reporting multiple forms of trauma and across quartiles of cumulative trauma scores. Similar dose−response patterns were revealed within the bipolar and healthy subgroups, but did not reach significance in schizophrenia-spectrum patients.

Conclusions

Findings show that childhood trauma is linked to frontal gray matter reductions, independent of psychiatric morbidity. Our results indicate that childhood trauma importantly contributes to the neurobiological changes commonly observed across psychiatric disorders. Frontal volume alterations may underpin affective and cognitive disturbances observed in trauma-exposed individuals.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re- use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Exposure to childhood trauma, encompassing different forms of maltreatment early in life, is an important risk factor for most psychiatric disorders (Green et al., Reference Green, McLaughlin, Berglund, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2010; Varese et al., Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster, Viechtbauer and Bentall2012). Childhood trauma may affect brain development, leading to aberrant cognitive functioning and emotional regulation and subsequent behavioral dysfunction (Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009; Sánchez, Ladd, & Plotsky, Reference Sánchez, Ladd and Plotsky2001; Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2016). Patients with a history of trauma may therefore form a specific group in terms of symptom presentation, response to treatment and clinical needs. As this subgroup is fairly large, roughly varying between 40% and 60% across psychiatric disorders, identification of trauma-related features of psychopathology, including their underlying neurobiological correlates, may inform individually tailored mental health care (Álvarez et al., Reference Álvarez, Roura, Osés, Foguet, Solà and Arrufat2011; Devi et al., Reference Devi, Shahwan, Teh, Sambasivam, Zhang, Lau and Subramaniam2019; Green et al., Reference Green, McLaughlin, Berglund, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2010; Porter, Branitsky, Mansell, & Warwick, Reference Porter, Branitsky, Mansell and Warwick2020; Schäfer & Fisher, Reference Schäfer and Fisher2011; Wiersma et al., Reference Wiersma, Hovens, van Oppen, Giltay, van Schaik, Beekman and Penninx2009).

Evidence on trauma-related gray matter alterations is now accumulating from structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI) investigations. Prior literature has largely discussed the effect of childhood trauma on the hippocampus and amygdala (Lim, Radua, & Rubia, Reference Lim, Radua and Rubia2014; Paquola, Bennett, & Lagopoulos, Reference Paquola, Bennett and Lagopoulos2016). This primary focus was guided by early findings in animal studies on the effect of early life stress impacting these regions, and clinical observations that linked childhood trauma to reduced memory performance and anxiety/mood regulation (Myers, McKlveen, & Herman, Reference Myers, McKlveen and Herman2014). Early MRI studies investigating the effect of trauma in patient samples have often used a region-of-interest (ROI) approach with hippocampus and amygdala as only ROIs (Lim et al., Reference Lim, Radua and Rubia2014; Paquola et al., Reference Paquola, Bennett and Lagopoulos2016), overlooking the role of trauma in other areas. The gray matter correlates of childhood trauma may well extend beyond the hippocampus and amygdala, as suggested by (pre)clinical childhood trauma models that implicate more widespread corticostriatal-limbic involvement (Edmiston et al., Reference Edmiston, Wang, Mazure, Guiney, Sinha, Mayes and Blumberg2011; Monroy, Hernández-Torres, & Flores, Reference Monroy, Hernández-Torres and Flores2010). Clinical studies also indicate a more extensive negative impact of childhood maltreatment involving a wide range of affective and cognitive functions, such as reward processing, attention and executive functioning, which are not primarily supported by hippocampus and amygdala function (Hart & Rubia, Reference Hart and Rubia2012; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2011). Such complex, higher-order functions are rather represented by networks that encompass both subcortical and cortical brain structures (Menon, Reference Menon2011). Thus, childhood trauma is hypothesized to lead to broader gray matter volume reductions in distributed cortical and subcortical areas, which is thought to impact cognition and emotion regulation and could be an independent risk factor for the development of psychopathology.

Despite the hypothesized general impact of childhood trauma, most of our knowledge on its neurobiological correlates stems from studies investigating individual diagnostic categories, including post-traumatic stress disorder, mood and anxiety disorders such as bipolar-I disorder, and schizophrenia (Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2016). As childhood trauma may affect patients with different diagnoses in a similar vein prior to the development of psychopathology, it is important to investigate its neurobiological consequences using a transdiagnostic approach (Bennett & Lagopoulos, Reference Bennett and Lagopoulos2018; Paquola et al., Reference Paquola, Bennett and Lagopoulos2016). Moreover, as only a minority of maltreated children will develop a psychiatric illness later in life, assessing the impact of childhood trauma both in the presence and absence of psychopathology will help disentangle the neurobiological effects of trauma from diagnostic classification (Begemann, Schutte, & Sommer, Reference Begemann, Schutte and Sommer2015b; Bennett & Lagopoulos, Reference Bennett and Lagopoulos2018; Paquola et al., Reference Paquola, Bennett and Lagopoulos2016).

In this study, we aimed to identify trauma-related gray matter alterations across the brain by investigating both cortical and subcortical regions in an exploratory manner. Moreover, we aimed to find correlates of trauma across diagnostic boundaries, by analyzing patients with disorders in two distinct DSM-V categories that share childhood adversity as an environmentally contributing factor. Relative to the general population, trauma-exposed individuals have a three-fold increased risk of developing bipolar disorder (Palmier-Claus, Berry, Bucci, Mansell, & Varese, Reference Palmier-Claus, Berry, Bucci, Mansell and Varese2016) or psychosis (Varese et al., Reference Varese, Smeets, Drukker, Lieverse, Lataster, Viechtbauer and Bentall2012). Therefore, we included 250 bipolar-I disorder patients and 84 patients with a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder. We also included a sample of healthy individuals (n = 220) to investigate possible correlates of trauma resilience.

Methods

Participants

Data were obtained from MRI studies conducted in the University Medical Center Utrecht (the Netherlands): Bipolar Genetics study (Vreeker et al., Reference Vreeker, Boks, Abramovic, Verkooijen, van Bergen, Hillegers and Riemersma-Van der Lek2016), Spectrum study (Sommer et al., Reference Sommer, Daalman, Rietkerk, Diederen, Bakker, Wijkstra and Boks2010) and Simvastatin for recent-onset psychosis (baseline data) (Begemann et al., Reference Begemann, Schutte, Slot, Doorduin, Bakker, van Haren and Sommer2015a). Participants were ⩾18 years of age. We included 554 participants: 220 healthy individuals without a current or past psychiatric diagnosis, 251 patients with type-I bipolar disorder, and 83 patients with a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder (DSM-IV-TR) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Presence or absence of psychopathology was established using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I) (First, Reference First1997), the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller and Dunbar1998) or the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History Interview (CASH) (Andreasen, Flaum, & Arndt, Reference Andreasen, Flaum and Arndt1992). Studies were approved by the medical ethical committee, participants gave written informed consent. Procedures are described in more detail in the eAppendix in Supplement.

Childhood trauma

Childhood maltreatment was assessed with the Dutch version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire–Short Form (CTQ-SF total score) (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia and Zule2003; Thombs, Bernstein, Lobbestael, & Arntz, Reference Thombs, Bernstein, Lobbestael and Arntz2009). This 25-item version evaluates five trauma subtypes (sexual, physical and emotional abuse, physical and emotional neglect), summing up to a total score. Good internal consistency reliability has been reported in both clinical and community samples (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia and Zule2003; Scher, Stein, Asmundson, McCreary, & Forde, Reference Scher, Stein, Asmundson, McCreary and Forde2001). Established cut-off scores for moderate to severe trauma were applied to classify the presence of a specific trauma subtype (emotional neglect ⩾15; physical neglect ⩾10; emotional abuse ⩾13; physical abuse ⩾10; sexual abuse ⩾8) (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia and Zule2003).

Structural magnetic resonance imaging

Structural MRI scans of the whole brain were acquired on the same 3T Philips Achieva medical scanner, equipped with an 8-channel SENSE headcoil (Philips, The Netherlands). Three-dimensional high-resolution T1-weighted images (high-res T1) were obtained with the following parameters; 200 contiguous sagittal slices (TE = 4.6 ms, TR = 10 ms, flip angle = 8°, FOV = 240 mm,0.75 × 0.75 × 0.80 mm3 voxels). T1-weighted images (T1) with a slightly lower resolution (160 contiguous sagittal slices, TE = 4.6 ms, TR = 10 ms, flip angle = 8°, FOV = 224 mm, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 voxels) were available for 43 healthy participants and 33 schizophrenia-spectrum disorder patients, yet no differences in total gray matter volume were observed when contrasting individuals with lower v. higher resolution T1 images within these groups (p = 0.651 in the healthy control group; p = 0.323 in the schizophrenia-spectrum group).

All 554 scans were checked for radiological abnormalities by a radiologist blinded to subject status (healthy individual or patient). Structural images were processed using the automated segmentation pipeline of FreeSurfer version 5.3.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) (Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, Van Der Kouwe, Destrieux, Halgren, Ségonne, Salat and Kennedy2004). By automatic parcellation of the cortical and subcortical structures, gray matter was divided into 82 distinct anatomical volumes of the Desikan−Killiany atlas (Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker and Hyman2006). Segmentations were manually checked for volumes deviating significantly from the population mean [i.e. more than two standard deviations (s.d.)] regarding total gray matter volume, thickness (left and right), surface area (left and right) and cerebral brain volume. When visual inspection of these images showed gross scan or segmentation errors, these segmentations were deemed unfit for further analysis. Images from two participants showed radiological abnormalities, two scans showed segmentation faults and seven scans were of insufficient quality, resulting in 543 scans suitable for analyses. The eleven excluded participants (four healthy controls, two bipolar-I patients and five schizophrenia-spectrum disorder patients) did not significantly differ (>2 s.d.) from the remaining sample across demographic variables or childhood trauma score. For each participant, total gray and white matter volumes were combined to calculate a general measure of cerebral brain volume.

We hierarchically grouped brain regions in order to obtain sufficient statistical power for analyzing both cortical and subcortical areas. The 68 individual cortical regions first were grouped into five cortical lobes: frontal (n = 22), parietal (n = 10), occipital (n = 8), temporal (n = 18) and insula/cingulate (n = 10) (Allen, Damasio, & Grabowski, Reference Allen, Damasio and Grabowski2002; Allen, Damasio, Grabowski, Bruss, & Zhang, Reference Allen, Damasio, Grabowski, Bruss and Zhang2003; Collin et al., Reference Collin, de Reus, Cahn, Hulshoff Pol, Kahn and van den Heuvel2013; Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker and Hyman2006; Long et al., Reference Long, Liao, Jiang, Liang, Qiu and Zhang2012). The 14 subcortical regions were grouped into one subcortical lobe, including the bilateral nucleus accumbens, amygdala, caudate, hippocampus, pallidum, putamen and thalamus. For significant lobes, individual regions were evaluated to investigate their contribution to the effect.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software, version 22.0. Group-differences in demographic characteristics were evaluated using chi-squared tests for categorical variables and F-tests for continuous variables. Differences in total cerebral brain volume were evaluated using ANCOVA (age and sex as covariates). Significant demographical findings were followed by post-hoc analyses to compare the three groups (Bonferroni corrected).

First, we used a linear approach by applying multivariate regression models to evaluate the association between cumulative childhood trauma severity (CTQ-SF total score) and total gray matter volume within the total sample, followed by the individual (sub)cortical lobes. All models were adjusted for group (dummy coding healthy controls, bipolar type-I and schizophrenia-spectrum disorder) and the covariates age, sex, cerebral brain volume, medication status (binary variable representing whether or not the patient was currently on an antipsychotic, antidepressant or lithium) and type of scan (dummy variable, high-res T1 v. T1). Analyses were Hochberg-adjusted (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995) for familywise multiple testing errors, using a false discovery rate (FDR) of 5% (q < 0.050). After identifying significant lobe(s), follow-up analyses were performed to specify which individual brain region(s) contributed to this effect (p < 0.050). Considering earlier sMRI findings focusing on the hippocampus and amygdala as single ROI's, we additionally defined these regions as specific outcomes of interest. Trauma-by-group interaction effects were examined by adding this interaction term in a separate set of analyses.

Second, a categorical approach was used to follow up on significant findings by means of the Jonckheere−Terpstra tests for ordered alternatives (J−T, one-tailed test) (Jonckheere, Reference Jonckheere1954). This, to address the general observations of low trauma scores in healthy control cohorts, in addition to a substantial number of patients not reporting any type of trauma. In the current sample, about 70% of the healthy control subgroup did not report any type of trauma according to CTQ cutoff-scores, in addition to more than 50% of both patient subgroups. For clinical relevance, individuals were first grouped according to the number of childhood trauma subtypes they had experienced using the established cut-off scores for moderate to severe trauma (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia and Zule2003). This resulted in four categories: no trauma; one trauma subtype, two trauma subtypes; and three or more trauma subtypes (three, four or five trauma subtypes were combined due to the low number of participants in these categories). Moreover, to address potential concerns on the use of predefined cut-off values, we created quartiles based on the total sample distribution of cumulative childhood trauma scores. After splitting the total sample by subgroup, J-T tests were repeated for the bipolar-I patients, schizophrenia-spectrum patients and healthy controls separately.

Results

Demographic characteristics

The subgroup of schizophrenia-spectrum disorder patients (n = 79) was on average younger and included more male participants than the other subgroups (Table 1). Healthy controls (n = 216) were significantly younger than bipolar patients (n = 248). Mean childhood trauma severity (CTQ-total score) was significantly higher for both patient groups as compared to healthy controls (Table 1). Figure 1 displays the prevalence of the five childhood trauma subtypes across the three subgroups. The number of individuals reporting to at least one form of trauma was similar in the bipolar patients (49.2%) and the schizophrenia-spectrum group (43%), this percentage was significantly lower in the healthy control group (29.6%). Total cerebral brain volume was significantly smaller in the bipolar and schizophrenia-spectrum subgroups, as compared to controls (Table 1).

Fig. 1. Number of individuals (%) exposed to different childhood trauma subtypes across the three subgroups.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the included participants

s.d., standard deviation; CTQ, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; BP, Bipolar-I patients; SSP, Schizophrenia-Spectrum Patients; HC, healthy controls.

a Individuals were categorized as having a history of childhood trauma when scoring above the cut-off for one or more trauma subtype(s). Bold values should be p < 0.001.

Linear trauma-related gray matter volume alterations across the brain

Considering the total sample (n = 512), the negative association between childhood trauma severity and total gray matter volume was not significant (β = −0.021, p = 0.124), correcting for group (p = 0.361) and including age, sex, cerebral brain volume, medication status and type of scan. Childhood trauma was significantly related to reduced gray matter volume of the frontal lobe (β = −0.049, p = 0.008; FDR-corrected, q = 0.048, scatter plot provided in eFigure 1 in Supplement), but not to the volume of other (sub)cortical lobes (Table 2). Childhood trauma severity was not associated with gray matter volume of the hippocampus (left: β = 0.005, p = 0.883; right: β = −0.009, p = 0.802) or amygdala (left: β = 0.002, p = 0.953; right: β = −0.054, p = 0.133). Analyses did not show any significant group effects on gray matter volume and there was no evidence for trauma-by-group interactions in a separate set of analyses.

Table 2. Linear associations between childhood trauma severity and gray matter volumes

s.e., standard error; FDR, False-Discovery Rate.

a Group, age, sex, cerebral brain volume, type of scan (high-resolution T1 v. T1), and medication status were included as covariates. Bold values should be p < 0.001.

Sensitivity analyses showed that the trauma-related gray matter volume reductions within the frontal lobe were localized in the right medialorbitofrontal region (β = −0.105, p = 0.002), right paracentral region (β = −0.088, p = 0.018), right superior frontal area (β = −0.058, p = 0.027) and left precentral region (β = −0.057, p = 0.0498) (eTable 1 in Supplement). Group had a significant effect on the left caudal middle frontal region (β = 0.129, p = 0.022) and bilateral rostral middle frontal regions (left: β = 0.143, p = 0.002; right: β = 0.087, p = 0.049). Post-hoc testing indicated larger gray matter volumes in the bipolar subgroup v. healthy controls (p = 0.013, p = 0.002 and p = 0.025, respectively). No trauma-by-group effects were observed.

Categorical trauma-related gray matter alterations in the frontal lobe

Including the total sample, more pronounced frontal gray matter volume reductions were found in individuals reporting an increasing number of childhood trauma subtypes (J-T = −3.86, p < 0.001, eTable 2, Fig. 2). Cumulative trauma scores, independent from trauma subtype, revealed a similar dose-response pattern: higher quartiles of overall trauma rates were linked to more pronounced gray matter volume reductions in the frontal lobe (J-T = −3.25, p = 0.001, eTable 2, Fig. 2). When repeating these analyses for the individual subgroups, similar significant dose-response patterns were observed in the bipolar-I patients and healthy controls, but did not reach significance for the schizophrenia-spectrum subgroup (eTable 2, Fig. 3).

Fig. 2. Violin plots depicting frontal gray matter volume across categories of reported trauma subtypes and cumulative trauma score in the total sample.

Fig. 3. Violin plots depicting frontal gray matter volume across categories of reported trauma subtypes (a) and cumulative trauma score (b) within the three subgroups.

Discussion

Exploring both cortical and subcortical brain regions in the largest transdiagnostic sample to date, we found that childhood trauma was associated with reduced gray matter volume in the frontal lobe, while no trauma-related volume reductions were observed in the hippocampus or amygdala. Sensitivity analyses showed that the right medial orbitofrontal, paracentral, superior frontal regions and the left precentral region specifically contributed to this effect. These frontal trauma-related reductions may be shared across diagnostic groups, as no trauma-by-group interaction effects were present. Ranked-analyses confirmed more pronounced frontal gray matter reduction in those reporting multiple forms of childhood trauma and across quartiles of cumulative trauma severity. Similar dose-response patterns were also significant within the subgroups of bipolar disorder patients and healthy controls.

The suggestion that the frontal regions are particularly sensitive to childhood trauma is supported by animal studies (Schubert, Porkess, & Dashdorj, Reference Schubert, Porkess and Dashdorj2009), showing structural changes in prefrontal dendrites after 1 week of induced stress (e.g. maternal separation or isolation rearing), or even a single traumatic incident, while changes in the hippocampus are observed only after enduring stress for several weeks (Arnsten, Reference Arnsten2009). Studies in maltreated children and adolescent samples have consistently shown frontal gray matter reductions, rather than hippocampal or amygdala reductions (De Brito et al., Reference De Brito, Viding, Sebastian, Kelly, Mechelli, Maris and McCrory2013; Edmiston et al., Reference Edmiston, Wang, Mazure, Guiney, Sinha, Mayes and Blumberg2011; Hanson et al., Reference Hanson, Chung, Avants, Shirtcliff, Gee, Davidson and Pollak2010; Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Viding, Wallace, Schaer, De Brito, Robustelli and Mccrory2013). Results from prior adult whole-brain studies in smaller samples, including patients as well as healthy subjects, also support a dose-response effect of trauma on the frontal lobe (Lim et al., Reference Lim, Radua and Rubia2014; Paquola et al., Reference Paquola, Bennett and Lagopoulos2016). Moreover, an ENIGMA study with the largest multi-center sample to date (n = 3036, healthy controls and patients with major depressive disorder) (Frodl et al., Reference Frodl, Janowitz, Schmaal, Tozzi, Dobrowolny, Stein and Jahanshad2017) investigated the link between childhood adversity and subcortical brain volume and did not replicate previous reports on trauma-related hippocampal or amygdala reductions either (Lim et al., Reference Lim, Radua and Rubia2014; Paquola et al., Reference Paquola, Bennett and Lagopoulos2016).

Although brain functioning related to cognition and social-emotional behavior is complex and distributed across remote brain regions, the frontal lobe is generally implicated to support top-down regulations of affect, motivational processing and social-emotional behaviors as well as higher-order cognition (Bonelli & Cummings, Reference Bonelli and Cummings2007; Carrion & Wong, Reference Carrion and Wong2012; Dolan, Reference Dolan2007; Lim et al., Reference Lim, Radua and Rubia2014; Menon, Reference Menon2011). Trauma-related cognitive and affective deficits may be mediated by structural brain alterations, predominantly in the frontal cortex. A link between early maltreatment and affective problems as well as cognitive deficits has consistently been shown in patient samples and healthy cohorts (Barzilay et al., Reference Barzilay, Calkins, Moore, Wolf, Satterthwaite, Cobb Scott and Gur2019; Gould et al., Reference Gould, Clarke, Heim, Harvey, Majer and Nemeroff2012; McCrory, De Brito, & Viding, Reference McCrory, De Brito and Viding2011; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2011; Wilson, Hansen, & Li, Reference Wilson, Hansen and Li2011). Our group previously reported that higher trauma severity is associated with reduced adaptive stress reactivity (Begemann, Stotijn, Schutte, Heringa, & Sommer, Reference Begemann, Stotijn, Schutte, Heringa and Sommer2017), high levels of neuroticism (So, Begemann, Gong, & Sommer, Reference So, Begemann, Gong and Sommer2016), and lower inhibitory control (Begemann, Daalman, Heringa, Schutte, & Sommer, Reference Begemann, Daalman, Heringa, Schutte and Sommer2016; Begemann, Heringa, & Sommer, Reference Begemann, Heringa and Sommer2016). Trauma-susceptible brain regions can be particularly impacted by adverse events during so called ‘sensitive periods’ (Anda et al., Reference Anda, Felitti, Bremner, Walker, Whitfield, Perry and Giles2006; Sánchez et al., Reference Sánchez, Ladd and Plotsky2001). Notably, the frontal lobe matures relatively late and its protracted development could make this region vulnerable to negative environmental influences for a longer time than other regions with more rapid maturation. Suboptimal functioning may not become apparent until full brain maturation in late adolescence or early adulthood, which is the time when bipolar disorder and schizophrenia-spectrum disorder are typically diagnosed (Bachevalier & Loveland, Reference Bachevalier and Loveland2006).

An important finding of the current study is that these trauma-related frontal gray matter reductions were observed in a transdiagnostic sample of healthy individuals, bipolar disorder patients and schizophrenia-spectrum disorder patients, without significant trauma-by-group interactions. This could indicate that trauma-related reductions in the frontal (sub)regions are shared across psychiatric patients, independent of diagnosis. In healthy individuals, the neurobiological effects of trauma have been described as being more subtle (Souza-Queiroz et al., Reference Souza-Queiroz, Boisgontier, Etain, Poupon, Duclap, D'Albis and Houenou2016), with lower trauma rates and smaller variance compared to patient cohorts, requiring large samples to demonstrate these effects. Both our ranked analyses revealed a clear pattern of more pronounced frontal gray matter reduction in healthy individuals reporting multiple forms of childhood trauma and across quartiles of cumulative trauma severity. While few studies that investigated healthy subjects extended their search beyond the hippocampal and amygdala regions, some previous studies have reported trauma-related frontal gray matter reductions in healthy adults (n = 145) and healthy adolescents (n = 42) (Dannlowski et al., Reference Dannlowski, Stuhrmann, Beutelmann, Zwanzger, Lenzen, Grotegerd and Kugel2012; Edmiston et al., Reference Edmiston, Wang, Mazure, Guiney, Sinha, Mayes and Blumberg2011).

The present data provide strong evidence that childhood trauma contributes to the neurobiological changes commonly observed in psychiatric disorders. The categorical dose-response patterns were clearly observed in the bipolar disorder patients. In the schizophrenia-spectrum disorder subgroup, an identical association was found between more pronounced frontal gray matter reductions in individuals reporting multiple forms of trauma, with a more subtle pattern for quartiles of overall trauma rates – yet our subgroup of 79 patients appeared underpowered to demonstrate these associations (May & Looney, Reference May and Looney2020). Previous neuroimaging studies have shown frontal gray matter alterations in bipolar disorder (Duarte et al., Reference Duarte, Neves, Albuquerque, De Souza-Duran, Busatto and Corrêa2016; Souza-Queiroz et al., Reference Souza-Queiroz, Boisgontier, Etain, Poupon, Duclap, D'Albis and Houenou2016), schizophrenia-spectrum disorder (Hoy et al., Reference Hoy, Barrett, Shannon, Campbell, Watson, Rushe and Mulholland2012; Sheffield, Williams, Woodward, & Heckers, Reference Sheffield, Williams, Woodward and Heckers2013), major depression disorder (Lu et al., Reference Lu, Xu, Cao, Yin, Gao, Wang and Li2019; Vythilingam et al., Reference Vythilingam, Heim, Newport, Miller, Anderson, Bronen and Charney2002), posttraumatic stress disorder (O'Doherty et al., Reference O'Doherty, Tickell, Ryder, Chan, Hermens, Bennett and Lagopoulos2017), borderline personality disorder (Brunner et al., Reference Brunner, Henze, Parzer, Kramer, Feigl, Lutz and Stieltjes2010), and substance use disorder (Bachi et al., Reference Bachi, Parvaz, Moeller, Gan, Zilverstand, Goldstein and Alia-Klein2018; Van Dam, Rando, Potenza, Tuit, & Sinha, Reference Van Dam, Rando, Potenza, Tuit and Sinha2014). An overlap in frontal gray matter loss has been demonstrated when directly comparing patients with bipolar disorder and those with a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder (Arnone et al., Reference Arnone, Cavanagh, Gerber, Lawrie, Ebmeier and McIntosh2009), but also when evaluating bipolar disorder v. depression (Wise et al., Reference Wise, Radua, Via, Cardoner, Abe, Adams and Périco2017), and depression v. borderline personality disorder (Depping et al., Reference Depping, Wolf, Vasic, Sambataro, Thomann and Wolf2016). Thus, the so-called p-factor (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014; Selzam, Coleman, Caspi, Moffitt, & Plomin, Reference Selzam, Coleman, Caspi, Moffitt and Plomin2018), reflecting the general effect of having a psychiatric illness including shared structural brain abnormalities across disorders (Opel et al., Reference Opel, Goltermann, Hermesdorf, Berger, Baune and Dannlowski2020; Parker et al., Reference Parker, Patel, Jackowski, Pan, Salum, Pausova and Paus2020), may overlap (perhaps for a large part) with the ‘t-factor’ reflecting the independent impact of childhood trauma.

Strengths and limitations

The current study has notable strengths, including its transdiagnostic design and extending the search for trauma-related gray matter alterations in brain regions other than the hippocampus and amygdala. Ideally, we would have implemented a fully data-driven approach based on individual brain regions. To ensure statistical power, we chose to conduct a lobe-wise analysis and follow up on significant results with explorative and uncorrected analyses to examine which individual regions would drive a particular result. Moreover, the three groups differed in age and gender distribution, in part because these are disease specific variables. Given the absence of trauma-by-group interactions in the total sample and the fact that we found a similar association between trauma and frontal brain volume across two out of three subgroups, suggests that age and gender effects do not to limit the interpretation of our current main findings. Moreover, we could not account for the plausible effect of cumulative medication use on gray matter volume, but instead evaluated current medication status. We also note that two different structured interviews were used to confirm clinical diagnosis in all included patients (SCID and CASH), their treating physician had already established diagnosis prior to enrollment. Furthermore, responses to the retrospective self-report questionnaire measuring childhood trauma may have been influenced by the inability of respondents to remember such experiences and subjective perception of the exposure to trauma. However, the CTQ-SF is currently indicated as one of the most reliable instruments for evaluating childhood trauma in healthy individuals as well as patients and our found trauma rates are in line with previous reports (Álvarez et al., Reference Álvarez, Roura, Osés, Foguet, Solà and Arrufat2011; Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia and Zule2003; Devi et al., Reference Devi, Shahwan, Teh, Sambasivam, Zhang, Lau and Subramaniam2019; Schäfer & Fisher, Reference Schäfer and Fisher2011; Wiersma et al., Reference Wiersma, Hovens, van Oppen, Giltay, van Schaik, Beekman and Penninx2009). Notably, the timing of maltreatment during childhood is not specifically assessed by the CTQ. The neurobiological effects on the brain, particularly limbic structures (Herzog et al., Reference Herzog, Thome, Demirakca, Koppe, Ende, Lis and Bohus2020), may depend on the timing of these experiences, which could be relevant for our null-findings regarding limbic structures. Important goals for future work should include setting up longitudinal cohort studies and analyzing large-scale transdiagnostic international consortium data sets.

In conclusion, our findings indicate that the frontal regions are most sensitive to the impact of childhood trauma and that these trauma-related frontal reductions are independent from psychiatric comorbidity. The frontal areas develop relatively late and the long window of high neuroplasticity may render these areas more vulnerable to adverse environmental circumstances during development. Emotional and cognitive deficits seen in individuals with a traumatic youth across diagnoses may at least partly stem from these frontal volume alterations and contribute to the development of psychopathology.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721002087

Trial registration

The study Simvastatin for recent-onset psychosis is registered in ClinicalTrails.gov (NCT01999309) and EudraCT (2013-000834-36).

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank all participants for their contribution and study team members for helping with data acquisition. We thank Dr Rachel Brouwer for her statistical input.

Financial support

The authors have no conflicts of interest relating to this article. The Spectrum study was funded by two personal grants for Professor Dr Sommer: De Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (NWO; Dutch Scientific Research Organization)/ZonMW Clinical Fellowship 2009 (grant number 4000703-97-270) and NWO/ZonMW Innovation Impulse (Vidi) 2009 (grant number 017.106.301). The Simvastatin for recent-onset psychosis is supported by the Stanley Medical Research Institute (to Prof. Dr Iris Sommer), grant number 12T-008 and is part of the research program TOP financed by NWO, grant number 40-00812-98-12154. The Bipolar Genetics study received funding from the National Institute of Mental Health, grant number R01MH090553 to Dr M. Boks.

Conflict of interest

The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.

References

Allen, J. S., Damasio, H., & Grabowski, T. J. (2002). Normal neuroanatomical variation in the human brain: An MRI-volumetric study. American Journal of Physical Anthropology: The Official Publication of the American Association of Physical Anthropologists, 118(4), 341358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, J. S., Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Bruss, J., & Zhang, W. (2003). Sexual dimorphism and asymmetries in the gray–white composition of the human cerebrum. Neuroimage, 18(4), 880894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Álvarez, M.-J., Roura, P., Osés, A., Foguet, Q., Solà, J., & Arrufat, F.-X. (2011). Prevalence and clinical impact of childhood trauma in patients with severe mental disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 199(3), 156161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic criteria from DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., Bremner, J. D., Walker, J. D., Whitfield, C. H., Perry, B. D., … Giles, W. H. (2006). The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 256(3), 174186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N. C., Flaum, M., & Arndt, S. (1992). The comprehensive assessment of symptoms and history (CASH): An instrument for assessing diagnosis and psychopathology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49(8), 615623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnone, D., Cavanagh, J., Gerber, D., Lawrie, S. M., Ebmeier, K. P., & McIntosh, A. M. (2009, September). Magnetic resonance imaging studies in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 195(3), 194201. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.108.059717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 410–422. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachevalier, J., & Loveland, K. A. (2006). The orbitofrontal–amygdala circuit and self-regulation of social–emotional behavior in autism. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(1), 97117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachi, K., Parvaz, M. A., Moeller, S. J., Gan, G., Zilverstand, A., Goldstein, R. Z., & Alia-Klein, N. (2018). Reduced orbitofrontal gray matter concentration as a marker of premorbid childhood trauma in cocaine use disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barzilay, R., Calkins, M. E., Moore, T. M., Wolf, D. H., Satterthwaite, T. D., Cobb Scott, J., … Gur, R. E. (2019). Association between traumatic stress load, psychopathology, and cognition in the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort. Psychological Medicine, 49(2), 325334. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718000880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begemann, M. J. H., Daalman, K., Heringa, S. M., Schutte, M. J. L., & Sommer, I. E. C. (2016). Letter to the editor: Childhood trauma as a risk factor for psychosis: The confounding role of cognitive functioning. Psychological Medicine, 46(5), 11151118. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171500255X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Begemann, M. J. H., Heringa, S. M., & Sommer, I. E. C. (2016). Childhood trauma as a neglected factor in psychotic experiences and cognitive functioning. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(8), 875876. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begemann, M. J. H., Schutte, M. J. L., Slot, M. I. E., Doorduin, J., Bakker, P. R., van Haren, N. E. M., … Sommer, I. E. C. (2015a). Simvastatin augmentation for recent-onset psychotic disorder: A study protocol. BBA Clinical, 4, 5258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbacli.2015.06.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begemann, M. J. H., Schutte, M. J., & Sommer, I. E. (2015b). Childhood trauma-specific reductions in limbic gray matter volume: Still in the dark. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(4), 398. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begemann, M. J. H., Stotijn, E., Schutte, M. J. L., Heringa, S. M., & Sommer, I. E. C. (2017). Letter to the editor: Beyond childhood trauma – stressful events early and later in life in relation to psychotic experiences. Psychological Medicine, 47(15), 27312736. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717000538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300.Google Scholar
Bennett, M., & Lagopoulos, J. (2018). Grey matter changes in the brain following stress and trauma. In Stress, trauma and synaptic plasticity. pp. 128. Cham: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-91116-8_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., … Zule, W. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse and Neglect, 27(2), 169190. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonelli, R. M., & Cummings, J. L. (2007). Frontal-subcortical circuitry and behavior. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 9(2), 141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunner, R., Henze, R., Parzer, P., Kramer, J., Feigl, N., Lutz, K., … Stieltjes, B. (2010). Reduced prefrontal and orbitofrontal gray matter in female adolescents with borderline personality disorder: Is it disorder specific? NeuroImage, 49(1), 114120. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrion, V. G., & Wong, S. S. (2012). Can traumatic stress alter the brain? Understanding the implications of early trauma on brain development and learning. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51(2 Suppl), S23-28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.04.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., … Poulton, R. (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collin, G., de Reus, M. A., Cahn, W., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Kahn, R. S., & van den Heuvel, M. P. (2013). Disturbed grey matter coupling in schizophrenia. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 23(1), 4654. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.09.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dannlowski, U., Stuhrmann, A., Beutelmann, V., Zwanzger, P., Lenzen, T., Grotegerd, D., … Kugel, H. (2012). Limbic scars: Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 71(4), 286293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Brito, S. A., Viding, E., Sebastian, C. L., Kelly, P. A., Mechelli, A., Maris, H., & McCrory, E. J. (2013). Reduced orbitofrontal and temporal grey matter in a community sample of maltreated children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 54(1), 105112. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02597.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Depping, M. S., Wolf, N. D., Vasic, N., Sambataro, F., Thomann, P. A., & Wolf, R. C. (2016). Common and distinct structural network abnormalities in major depressive disorder and borderline personality disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 65, 127133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., … Hyman, B. T. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage, 31(3), 968980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devi, F., Shahwan, S., Teh, W. L., Sambasivam, R., Zhang, Y. J., Lau, Y. W., … Subramaniam, M. (2019). The prevalence of childhood trauma in psychiatric outpatients. Annals of General Psychiatry, 18(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12991-019-0239-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolan, R. J. (2007). The human amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex in behavioural regulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 787799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duarte, D. G. G., Neves, M. D. C. L., Albuquerque, M. R., De Souza-Duran, F. L., Busatto, G., & Corrêa, H. (2016). Gray matter brain volumes in childhood-maltreated patients with bipolar disorder type I: A voxel-based morphometric study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 197, 7480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edmiston, E. E., Wang, F., Mazure, C. M., Guiney, J., Sinha, R., Mayes, L. C., & Blumberg, H. P. (2011). Corticostriatal-limbic gray matter morphology in adolescents with self-reported exposure to childhood maltreatment. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 165(12), 10691077. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B. (1997). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders SCID-I: Clinician version, scoresheet. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Fischl, B., Van Der Kouwe, A., Destrieux, C., Halgren, E., Ségonne, F., Salat, D. H., … Kennedy, D. (2004). Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14(1), 1122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frodl, T., Janowitz, D., Schmaal, L., Tozzi, L., Dobrowolny, H., Stein, D. J., … Jahanshad, N. (2017). Childhood adversity impacts on brain subcortical structures relevant to depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 86, 5865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, F., Clarke, J., Heim, C., Harvey, P. D., Majer, M., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2012). The effects of child abuse and neglect on cognitive functioning in adulthood. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46(4), 500506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, J. G., McLaughlin, K. A., Berglund, P. A., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(2), 113123. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanson, J. L., Chung, M. K., Avants, B. B., Shirtcliff, E. A., Gee, J. C., Davidson, R. J., & Pollak, S. D. (2010). Early stress is associated with alterations in the orbitofrontal cortex: A tensor-based morphometry investigation of brain structure and behavioral risk. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(22), 74667472. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0859-10.2010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, H., & Rubia, K. (2012). Neuroimaging of child abuse: A critical review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(March), 124. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herzog, J. I., Thome, J., Demirakca, T., Koppe, G., Ende, G., Lis, S., … Bohus, M., (2020). Influence of severity of type and timing of retrospectively reported childhood maltreatment on female amygdala and hippocampal volume. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoy, K., Barrett, S., Shannon, C., Campbell, C., Watson, D., Rushe, T., … Mulholland, C. (2012). Childhood trauma and hippocampal and amygdalar volumes in first-episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(6), 11621169. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbr085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonckheere, A. R. (1954). A distribution-free k-sample test against ordered alternatives. Biometrika, 41(1/2), 133145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, P. A., Viding, E., Wallace, G. L., Schaer, M., De Brito, S. A., Robustelli, B., & Mccrory, E. J. (2013). Cortical thickness, surface area, and gyrification abnormalities in children exposed to maltreatment: Neural markers of vulnerability? Biological Psychiatry, 74(11), 845852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.06.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, L., Radua, J., & Rubia, K. (2014). Gray matter abnormalities in childhood maltreatment: A voxelwise metaanalysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(8), 854863. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13101427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long, X., Liao, W., Jiang, C., Liang, D., Qiu, B., & Zhang, L. (2012). Healthy aging: An automatic analysis of global and regional morphological alterations of human brain. Academic Radiology, 19(7), 785793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, S., Xu, R., Cao, J., Yin, Y., Gao, W., Wang, D., … Li, L. (2019). The left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volume is reduced in adults reporting childhood trauma independent of depression diagnosis. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 112, 1217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
May, J. O., & Looney, S.W (2020). Sample size charts for Spearman and Kendall coefficients. Journal of Biometrics & Biostatistics, 11(6). https://doi.org/10.37421/jbmbs.2020.11.440.Google Scholar
McCrory, E., De Brito, S. A., & Viding, E. (2011). The impact of childhood maltreatment: A review of neurobiological and genetic factors. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2, 48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 483506. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2011.08.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroy, E., Hernández-Torres, E., & Flores, G. (2010). Maternal separation disrupts dendritic morphology of neurons in prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and nucleus accumbens in male rat offspring. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy, 40(2), 93101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jchemneu.2010.05.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, B., McKlveen, J. M., & Herman, J. P. (2014). Glucocorticoid actions on synapses, circuits, and behavior: Implications for the energetics of stress. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 35(2), 180196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.12.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Doherty, D. C. M., Tickell, A., Ryder, W., Chan, C., Hermens, D. F., Bennett, M. R., & Lagopoulos, J. (2017). Frontal and subcortical grey matter reductions in PTSD. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 266, 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.05.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opel, N., Goltermann, J., Hermesdorf, M., Berger, K., Baune, B. T., & Dannlowski, U. (2020). Cross-disorder analysis of brain structural abnormalities in six major psychiatric disorders: A secondary analysis of mega-and meta-analytical findings from the ENIGMA consortium. Biological Psychiatry, 88(9), 678686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmier-Claus, J.E., Berry, K., Bucci, S., Mansell, W., & Varese, F. (2016). Relationship between childhood adversity and bipolar affective disorder: Systematic review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(6), 454459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paquola, C., Bennett, M. R., & Lagopoulos, J. (2016). Understanding heterogeneity in grey matter research of adults with childhood maltreatment – A meta-analysis and review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 299312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, N., Patel, Y., Jackowski, A. P., Pan, P. M., Salum, G. A., Pausova, Z., & Paus, T. (2020). Assessment of neurobiological mechanisms of cortical thinning during childhood and adolescence and their implications for psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(11), 11271136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pechtel, P., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2011). Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: An integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology, 214(1), 5570. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-2009-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, C., Branitsky, A., Mansell, W., & Warwick, H. (2020). Childhood adversity and borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis. Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica 141(1), 6–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sánchez, M. M., Ladd, C. O., & Plotsky, P. M. (2001). Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: Evidence from rodent and primate models. Development and Psychopathology, 13(3), 419449. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579401003029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schäfer, I., & Fisher, H. L. (2011). Childhood trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with psychosis: Clinical challenges and emerging treatments. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 24(6), 514518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scher, C. D., Stein, M. B., Asmundson, G. J. G., McCreary, D. R., & Forde, D. R. (2001). The childhood trauma questionnaire in a community sample: Psychometric properties and normative data. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14(4), 843857.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schubert, M. I., Porkess, M. V, & Dashdorj, N. (2009). Effects of social isolation rearing on the limbic brain: A combined behavioral and magnetic resonance imaging volumetry study in rats. NSC, 159(1), 2130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.12.019.Google Scholar
Selzam, S., Coleman, J. R. I., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2018). A polygenic p factor for major psychiatric disorders. Translational Psychiatry, 8(1), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D. V, Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., … Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 2233.Google ScholarPubMed
Sheffield, J. M., Williams, L. E., Woodward, N. D., & Heckers, S. (2013). Reduced gray matter volume in psychotic disorder patients with a history of childhood sexual abuse. Schizophrenia Research, 143(1), 185191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
So, S. H.-W., Begemann, M. J. H., Gong, X., & Sommer, I. E. (2016). Relationship between neuroticism, childhood trauma and cognitive-affective responses to auditory verbal hallucinations. Scientific Reports, 6, 34401. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep34401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommer, I. E., Daalman, K., Rietkerk, T., Diederen, K. M., Bakker, S., Wijkstra, J., & Boks, M. P. M. (2010). Healthy individuals with auditory verbal hallucinations; Who are they? Psychiatric assessments of a selected sample of 103 subjects. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(3), 633641. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Souza-Queiroz, J., Boisgontier, J., Etain, B., Poupon, C., Duclap, D., D'Albis, M. A., … Houenou, J. (2016). Childhood trauma and the limbic network: A multimodal MRI study in patients with bipolar disorder and controls. Journal of Affective Disorders, 200, 159164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.04.038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., & Samson, J. A. (2016). Annual research review: Enduring neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 57(3), 241266. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thombs, B. D., Bernstein, D. P., Lobbestael, J., & Arntz, A. (2009). A validation study of the Dutch childhood trauma questionnaire-short form: Factor structure, reliability, and known-groups validity. Child Abuse and Neglect, 33(8), 518523. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2009.03.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dam, N. T., Rando, K., Potenza, M. N., Tuit, K., & Sinha, R. (2014). Childhood maltreatment, altered limbic neurobiology, and substance use relapse severity via trauma-specific reductions in limbic gray matter volume. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(8), 917925. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varese, F., Smeets, F., Drukker, M., Lieverse, R., Lataster, T., Viechtbauer, W., … Bentall, R. P. (2012). Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: A meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(4), 661671. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vreeker, A., Boks, M. P. M., Abramovic, L., Verkooijen, S., van Bergen, A. H., Hillegers, M. H. J., … Riemersma-Van der Lek, R. F. (2016). High educational performance is a distinctive feature of bipolar disorder: A study on cognition in bipolar disorder, schizophrenia patients, relatives and controls. Psychological Medicine, 46(4), 807818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vythilingam, M., Heim, C., Newport, J., Miller, A. H., Anderson, E., Bronen, R., … Charney, D. S. (2002). Childhood trauma associated with smaller hippocampal volume in women with major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(12), 20722080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiersma, J. E., Hovens, J. G. F. M., van Oppen, P., Giltay, E. J., van Schaik, D. J. F., Beekman, A. T. F., & Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2009). The importance of childhood trauma and childhood life events for chronicity of depression in adults. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70(7), 983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, K. R., Hansen, D. J., & Li, M. (2011). The traumatic stress response in child maltreatment and resultant neuropsychological effects. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(2), 8797. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2010.12.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wise, T., Radua, J., Via, E., Cardoner, N., Abe, O., Adams, T. M., … Périco, C. de A. M. (2017). Common and distinct patterns of grey-matter volume alteration in major depression and bipolar disorder: Evidence from voxel-based meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(10), 14551463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Number of individuals (%) exposed to different childhood trauma subtypes across the three subgroups.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the included participants

Figure 2

Table 2. Linear associations between childhood trauma severity and gray matter volumes

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Violin plots depicting frontal gray matter volume across categories of reported trauma subtypes and cumulative trauma score in the total sample.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Violin plots depicting frontal gray matter volume across categories of reported trauma subtypes (a) and cumulative trauma score (b) within the three subgroups.

Supplementary material: File

Begemann et al. supplementary material

Begemann et al. supplementary material 1

Download Begemann et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Begemann et al. supplementary material

Begemann et al. supplementary material 2

Download Begemann et al. supplementary material(File)
File 20.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Begemann et al. supplementary material

Begemann et al. supplementary material 3

Download Begemann et al. supplementary material(File)
File 94 KB
Supplementary material: File

Begemann et al. supplementary material

Begemann et al. supplementary material 4

Download Begemann et al. supplementary material(File)
File 21.5 KB