Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T07:23:34.227Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Verbal and non-verbal recall by depressed and euthymic affective patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Avraham Calev*
Affiliation:
Jerusalem Mental Health Center–Ezrath Nashim, Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel
Yaacov Korin
Affiliation:
Jerusalem Mental Health Center–Ezrath Nashim, Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel
Baruch Shapira
Affiliation:
Jerusalem Mental Health Center–Ezrath Nashim, Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel
Sol Kugelmass
Affiliation:
Jerusalem Mental Health Center–Ezrath Nashim, Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel
Bernard Lerer
Affiliation:
Jerusalem Mental Health Center–Ezrath Nashim, Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr A. Calev, Jerusalem Mental Health Center–Ezrath Nashim, P.O. Box 140, Jerusalem, Israel

Synopsis

This study uses matched-tasks methodology in order to test memory function in depressed and euthymic patients with major affective disorder. Neither drug-free depressed patients nor lithium-treated euthymic patients show a differential deficit in verbal versus non-verbal recall. However, while euthymic patients show no memory impairment, drug-free depressives do show poor memory functioning. The results support the view that memory deficits observed in affective patients in the depressed state are transient, secondary manifestations of depression and are neither indicative of underlying organic pathology, nor of abnormal hemispheric laterality. This suggests that memory impairment in depression can be treated by treating depressive symptoms, both chemically and behaviourally. The results also support the view that prophylactic lithium treatment has no adverse effects on these memory tasks.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1986

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn). APA: Washington, D.C.Google Scholar
Ammons, R. B. & Ammons, C. H. (1962). Quick Test. Psychological Tests Specialists: Missoula, MT.Google Scholar
Benton, A. L. (1974). Revised Visual Retention Test: Clinical and Experimental Applications (4th edn). Psychological Corporation: New York.Google Scholar
Breslow, R., Kocksis, J. & Belkin, B. (1980). Memory deficits in depression: evidence utilizing the Wechsler Memory Scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills 51, 541542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calev, A. (1984 a). Recall and recognition in chronic nondemented schizophrenics: use of matched tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 93, 172177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calev, A. (1984 b). Recall and recognition in mildly disturbed schizophrenics: the use of matched-tasks. Psychological Medicine 14, 425429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calev, A. & Erwin, P. G. (1985). Recall and recognition in depressives: use of matched tasks. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 24, 127128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calev, A. & Monk, A. F. (1982). Verbal memory tasks showing no deficit in schizophrenia – fact or artefact? British Journal of Psychiatry 141, 528530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calev, A., Korin, Y., Kugelmass, S. & Lerer, B. (1986). Performance of chronic schizophrenics on matched verbal and nonverbal recall tasks. (Submitted for publication.)Google Scholar
Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J. P. (1973). Disordered Thought in Schizophrenia. Appleton Century Crafts: New York.Google Scholar
Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J. P. (1978). The measurement of differential deficits. Journal of Psychiatric Research 14, 303311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, R. M., Weingartner, H., Smallberg, S. A., Pickar, D. & Murphy, D. L. (1982). Effort and cognition in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 39, 593597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronholm, B. & Ottosson, J. (1961). Memory functions in endogenous depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 5, 101107.Google ScholarPubMed
Davis, H. & Unruh, W. R. (1980). Word memory in non-psychotic depression. Perceptual and Motor Skills 51, 699705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deptula, D. & Yozawitz, A. (1984). Lateralized brain function in depression: Analysis of memory.Paper presented at the 19th Annual Meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society,Houston, Texas.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., Stevens, M., Johnstone, E. C., Deakin, J. F. W., Lawler, P. & Crow, T. J. (1983). Effects of ECT and depression on various aspects of memory. British Journal of Psychiatry 142, 610617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. H. (1981). Self-reinforcement and recall: differential deficits in depressed and nondepressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 90, 521530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruzelier, J. M. & Venables, P. H. (1974). Bimodality and laterality of skin conductance orienting activity in schizophrenics: replication and evidence of lateral asymmetry in patients with depression and disorders of personality. Biological Psychiatry 8, 5573.Google ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M. & Krap, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behaviour: a cognitive loop? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joynt, R. J. & Shoulton, I. (1985). Dementia. In Clinical Neuropsychology (ed. Heilman, K. M. and Valenstein, E.), pp. 453480. Oxford University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Judd, L. L., Hubbard, B., Janowsky, D. S., Huey, L. Y. & Takahashi, K. I. (1977). The effect of lithium carbonate on the cognitive functions of normal subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry 34, 355357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koh, S. D. & Wolpert, E. A. (1983). Memory scanning and retrieval in affective disorders. Psychiatry Research 8, 289297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kropf, D. & Muller-Oerlinghausen, B. (1979). Changes in learning, memory and mood during lithium treatment: approach to a research strategy. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 59, 97124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, R. J. & Frith, C. D. (1981). Learning and reminiscence in the pursuit–motor performance of normal and depressed subjects. Personality and Individual Differences 2, 207213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lishman, W. A. (1972). Selective factors in memory. Part 2: Affective disorder. Psychological Medicine 2, 248273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, G. G. & Lishman, W. A. (1975). Effect of depression on the speed of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Psychological Medicine 5, 173180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, R. E. & Craighead, W. E. (1977). Selective recall of positive and negative feedback, self-control behaviors and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 86, 379388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellegrino, J. W. (1971). A general measure of organization in free recall for variable unit size and internal sequential consistency. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation 3, 241246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapp, M. S. & Thomas, M. R. (1979). Lithium and memory loss. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 24, 700701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rattenbury, F. R., Silverstein, M. L., Dewolf, A. S., Kaufman, C. F. & Harrow, M. (1983). Associative disturbance in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorders, and major affective disorders: comparisons between hospitalization and 1-year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 51, 621623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartzburg, M. (1983). Hemispheric laterality and EEG correlates of depression. Research Communications in Psychology, Psychiatry and Behavior 8, 187205.Google Scholar
Shima, S., Shikano, T., Kitamura, T., Masuda, Y., Tsukumo, T., Kanba, S. & Asai, M. (1984). Depression and ventricular enlargement. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 70, 275277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silberman, E. K., Weingartner, H., Stillman, R., Chen, H. J. & Post, R. M. (1983). Altered lateralization of cognitive processes in depressed women. American Journal of Psychiatry 140, 13401344.Google Scholar
Smigan, L. & Perris, C. (1983). Memory functions and prophylactic treatment with lithium. Psychological Medicine 13, 529536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J. & Robins, E. (1977). Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders (3rd edn). Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Squire, L R. (1984). ECT and memory dysfunction. In ECT Basic Mechanisms (ed. Lerer, B., Weiner, R. D. and Belmaker, R. H.), pp. 156163. Libby: London.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R., Judd, L. L., Janowsky, D. S. & Huey, L. Y. (1980). Effects of lithium carbonate on memory and other cognitive functions. American Journal of Psychiatry 137, 10421046.Google ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, D. E. & Jarvik, M. E. (1976). Memory functions in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 33, 219224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, M. E. & Allred, L. J. (1986). Measurement of differential cognitive deficits after head injury. In Neurobehavioral Recovery From Head Injury (ed. Levin, H. S., Eisenberg, H. M. and Grafman, J.). Oxford University Press: Oxford. (In the press.)Google Scholar
Teasdale, J. D. & Russell, M. L. (1983). Differential effects of induced mood on the recall of positive, negative and neutral words. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 22, 163171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich, G., Zeller, G. & Muhlbauer, H. D. (1983). Functional asymmetry in endogeneous depressive inpatients. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 233, 457469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wexler, B. E. (1980). Cerebral laterality and psychiatry: a review of the literature. American Journal of Psychiatry 137, 279291.Google Scholar