Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T11:50:56.063Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Person in the language of singletons, siblings, and twins

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 January 2010

Werner Deutsch
Affiliation:
Technische Universität Braunschweig
Angela Wagner
Affiliation:
Technische Universität Braunschweig
Renate Burchardt
Affiliation:
Technische Universität Braunschweig
Nina Schulz
Affiliation:
Technische Universität Braunschweig
Jörg Nakath
Affiliation:
Technische Universität Braunschweig
Melissa Bowerman
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Psycholinguistik, The Netherlands
Stephen Levinson
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Psycholinguistik, The Netherlands
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Many of the chapters in this book are concerned with the child's grappling with concepts of the nature of countable objects, or the very mechanisms of counting themselves or of the logical and spatial relations between objects: naive physics, geometry, logic and maths. But in this chapter we turn to an area of much greater concern to the child him- or herself: the realm of person identification. Clearly person identification is the precondition to socio-emotional attachment, and indeed meaningful human social life.

What can we learn about children's conceptions of the person by looking at early language? At first sight perhaps not much. Person identification is in place long before the beginnings of language. Moreover, there are some particular, intrinsically linguistic difficulties with person reference - namely the fact that proper names are replaced by pronouns in first and second persons (at least in the familiar European languages). The tendency for children's first references to self to use their own names or nicknames has been well documented (see e.g. Tanz 1980: ch. 4 for review; Chiat 1986), and their difficulties with pronouns are natural given the way in which their reference shifts according to who is speaking. But the data in this chapter suggest that more can be at stake than mere linguistic difficulty: this becomes especially evident in a special kind of self-reference to be found occasionally among twins.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2001

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×