Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T11:48:29.464Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - The Aging Mind and Brain: Implications of Enduring Plasticity for Behavioral and Cultural Change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 July 2009

Patricia A. Reuter-lorenz
Affiliation:
Professor of Psychology University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, USA
Joseph A. Mikels
Affiliation:
Postdoctoral Fellow Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Palo Alto, California, USA
Paul B. Baltes
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Bildungsforschung, Berlin
Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Frank Rösler
Affiliation:
Philipps-Universität Marburg, Germany
Get access

Summary

ABSTRACT

Although aging is broadly characterized by decline, the potential for new learning and plasticity persists well into the later decades of life. Scientific advances are yielding a deeper understanding of the limitations that biological aging imposes on cognitive function, as well as new insights into how the human mind and brain respond adaptively to the aging process. Neurocognitive investigations of the reciprocity between mind and brain reveal new avenues to influence and shape neural processes that underlie mental fitness, especially in the golden years. We explore these ideas to illustrate the co-constructivist framework in operation across neural, cognitive, behavioral, and cultural dimensions as they influence late-life development.

OVERVIEW

The persistence of behavioral adaptation and plasticity (i.e., modifiability) in later life has been recognized by the field of cognitive aging for several decades (e.g., Baltes, 1997). Training procedures of various sorts have been shown to enhance cognitive performance and produce long-term gains, even for older adults well into their seventies (e.g., Willis & Nesselroade, 1990). With the recent advances in genetics, in the basic neurosciences, and in brain imaging technologies, the scope and potential of age-related reorganizational processes have attained a new level of analysis and persuasion, especially for researchers whose theoretical orientation is closely linked to brain correlates of plasticity (Park, Polk, Mikels, Taylor, & Marshuetz, 2001; Reuter-Lorenz, 2002).

Type
Chapter
Information
Lifespan Development and the Brain
The Perspective of Biocultural Co-Constructivism
, pp. 255 - 276
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arbuckle, T. Y., Gold, D., & Andres, D. (1986). Cognitive functioning of older people in relation to social and personality variables. Psychology and Aging, 1 (1), 55–62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bäckman, L., & Dixon, R. A. (1992). Psychological compensation: A theoretical framework. Psychological Bulletin, 112 (2), 259–283CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52 (4), 366–380CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1988). On the range of cognitive plasticity in old age as a function of experience: 15 years of intervention research. Behavior Therapy, 19(3), 283–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19 (2), 290–303CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., & Park, D. (Eds.). (2005). Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Linking cognitive and cerebral aging. London:Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54 (3), 165–181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 14 (3), 117–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchill, J. D., Galvez, R., Colcombe, S., Swain, R. A., Kramer, A. F., & Greenough, W. T. (2002). Exercise, experience and the aging brain. Neurobiology of Aging, 23 (5), 941–955CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., Scalf, P., McAuley, E., Cohen, N. J.. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 101 (9), 3316–3321CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craik, F. I. M., & Salthouse, T. A. (Eds.). (2000). The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
DiGirolamo, G. J., Kramer, A. F., Barad, V., Cepeda, N. J., Weissman, D. H., Milham, M. P.. (2001). General and task-specific frontal lobe recruitment in older adults during executive processes: A fMRI investigation of task-switching. Neuroreport, 12 (9), 2065–2071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finch, C. E., & Cohen, D. M. (1997). Aging, metabolism, and Alzheimer's disease: Review and hypotheses. Experimental Neurology, 143, 82–102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gehlen, A. (1988). Man, his nature and place in the world. New York: Columbia University PressGoogle Scholar
Grady, C. L., & Kapur, S. (1999). The use of neuroimaging in neurorehabilitative research. In D. T. Stuss, G. Winocur, & I. H. Robertson (Eds.), Cognitive neurorehabilitation (pp. 47–58). New York: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108 (3), 356–388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J., Schumacher, E. H., Smith, E. E., Koeppe, R. A., Awh, E., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A.. (1998). The role of parietal cortex in verbal working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 18 (13), 5026–5034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, J. A., Shukitt-Hale, B., Denisova, N. A., Bielinski, D., Martin, A., McEwen, J. J., & Bickford, P. C. (1999). Reversals of age-related declines in neuronal signal transduction, cognitive, and motor behavioral deficits with blueberry, spinach, or strawberry dietary supplementation. Journal of Neuroscience, 19 (18), 8114–8121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1990). On the locus and process of magnification of age differences during mnemonic training. Developmental Psychology, 26 (6), 894–904CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, A. F., & Willis, S. L. (2002). Enhancing the cognitive vitality of older adults. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11 (5), 173–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Walking while memorizing: Age-related differences in compensatory behavior. Psychological Science, 12 (3), 230–237CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., Hommel, B., Aschersleben, G., Prinz, W., & Baltes, P. B. (2004). Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychological Science, 15 (3), 155–163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, J. M., Sanders, A. L., Snyder, A. Z., Morris, J. C., & Buckner, R. L. (2002). Under-recruitment and nonselective recruitment: Dissociable neural mechanisms associated with aging. Neuron, 33 (5), 827–840CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, J. K., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Sylvester, C. Y., Jonides, J., & Smith, E. E. (2003). Dissociable neural mechanisms underlying response-based and familiarity-based conflict in working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 100 (19), 11171–11175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, D. C., & Gutchess, A. H. (2002). Aging, cognition, and culture: A neuroscientific perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26 (7), 859–867CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, D. C., Nisbett, R., & Hedden, T. (1999). Aging, culture, and cognition. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, 54B (2), P75–P84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, D. C., Polk, T. A., Mikels, J. A., Taylor, S. F., & Marshuetz, C. (2001). Cerebral aging: Integration of brain and behavioral models of cognitive function. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 3 (3), 151–165Google ScholarPubMed
Persson, J., Sylvester, C. Y., Nelson, J. K., Welsh, K. M., Jonides, J., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2004). Selection requirements during verb generation: Differential recruitment in older and younger adults. NeuroImage, 23, 1382–1390CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Postle, B. R., Berger, J. S., & D'Esposito, M. (1999). Functional neuroanatomical double dissociation of mnemonic and executive control processes contributing to working memory performance. Proceedings of the National Academy (USA), 96 (22), 12959–12964CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–90). Mahwah, NJ:ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2002). New visions of the aging mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6 (9), 394–400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Hartley, A., Miller, A., Marshuetz, C., & Koeppe, R. A. (2000). Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 (1), 174–187CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Lustig, C. (2005). Brain aging: Reorganizing discoveries about the aging mind. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15 (4), 245–251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Stanczak, L., & Miller, A. C. (1999). Neural recruitment and cognitive aging: Two hemispheres are better than one, especially as you age. Psychological Science, 10 (6), 494–500CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Sylvester, C.-Y. C. (2005). The cognitive neuroscience of working memory and aging. In R. Cabeza, L. Nyberg, & D. C. Park (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Linking cognitive and cerebral aging (pp. 186–217). London:Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Rogers, W. A. (2000). Attention and aging. In D. C. Park & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Cognitive aging: A primer (pp. 57–73). Philadelphia:Psychology PressGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103 (3), 403–428CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaie, K. W. (2005). Developmental influences on adult intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. London:Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seeman, T. E., Lusignolo, T. M., Albert, M., & Berkman, L. (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Health Psychology, 20 (4), 243–255CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283 (5408), 1657–1661CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, P., & Carstensen, L. L. (Eds.). (2000). The aging mind: Opportunities in cognitive aging. Washington, DC:The National Academies PressGoogle Scholar
Praag, H., Christie, B. R., Sejnowski, T. J., & Gage, F. H. (1999). Running enhances neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 96 (23), 13427–13431CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, N. S., Brown, M. M., Thompson, A. J., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (2003). Neural correlates of motor recovery after stroke: A longitudinal fMRI study. Brain, 126 (11), 2476–2496CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willis, S. L., & Nesselroade, C. S. (1990). Long-term effects of fluid ability training in old-old age. Developmental Psychology, 26 (6), 905–910CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winocur, G. (1998). Environmental influences on cognitive decline in aged rats. Neurobiology of Aging, 19 (6), 589–597CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×