Main

Schizophrenia patients demonstrate abnormalities in information processing as measured by deficits in specific behavioral measures. Among these abnormalities, schizophrenia patients exhibit deficient prepulse inhibition (Braff et al. 1978, 1992; Swerdlow et al. 1994). Prepulse inhibition (PPI) is the reduction in startle magnitude that occurs when the startling stimulus is preceded 30–500 msec by a weak stimulus, or “prepulse.” A relationship between PPI and clinical symptoms in schizophrenia is demonstrated by the significant correlation between PPI and measures of thought disorder (Perry and Braff 1994) and positive and negative symptoms (Braff et al. 1995) in schizophrenia patients.

PPI can be studied across species and has been shown to be useful in animal studies of sensorimotor gating. In rats, PPI is disrupted by acute treatment with dopamine (DA) agonists (Swerdlow et al. 1986, 1990a,b; Mansbach et al. 1988) or N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) antagonists (Mansbach and Geyer 1989, 1991); the effects of DA agonists can be reversed by typical or atypical antipsychotics, while those of NMDA antagonists are reversed preferentially by clinically atypical antipsychotics (Keith et al. 1991; Swerdlow and Geyer 1993; Hoffman et al. 1993; Swerdlow et al. 1994; Bakshi et al. 1994). In addition, neuroanatomical and neurochemical manipulations in rats within brain circuitry implicated in the neuropathology of schizophrenia also produce these schizophrenia-like PPI deficits (cf. Swerdlow and Geyer 1998). These findings provide insight into neural circuit abnormalities that might contribute to the loss of PPI in schizophrenia patients.

PPI deficits in schizophrenia presumably reflect neuronal perturbations that are long-standing, and which may not be modelled optimally by acute and transient drug effects (Sams-Dodd 1998). Although no studies have examined the effects on PPI of sustained or repeated exposure to NMDA antagonists, lasting neuropathological and behavioral effects of prolonged exposure to NMDA antagonists have been reported. For example, neuropathological damage is observed after sustained phencyclidine exposure via subcutaneous minipumps (Ellison and Switzer 1993; Ellison 1994) and lasting behavioral deficits are reported after repeated systemic injections of PCP (Jentsch et al. 1997). In the present studies, we examined the effects of sustained or repeated exposure to PCP on PPI in rats, using the precise methodologies reported by different investigative groups to produce neuropathological damage (Ellison and Switzer 1993; Ellison 1994) and/or significant behavioral abnormalities in rats (Jentsch et al. 1997) that may parallel specific abnormalities in schizophrenia patients.

METHODS

Rats

A total of 115 female Sprague-Dawley rats, weighing 225–275 g and 32 male Sprague-Dawley rats (Harlan Laboratories, San Diego, CA), weighing 250–300 g were used in these experiments. Female rats were used for experiments utilizing minipump delivery of PCP, while male rats were used for experiments utilizing repeated PCP injections, according to the published literature for these two models of sustained and subchronic PCP exposure (Ellison and Switzer 1993; Ellison 1994; Jentsch et al. 1997). Rats were housed in same-sex groups of two or three, except for those with implanted minipumps, which were housed singly, to prevent excessive perturbation of the pumps. A reversed 12-hour light/dark cycle was used (lights on at 1900, off at 0700); surgery and all testing occurred between 1000 and 1700. Rats were handled prior to any procedures to minimize stress during behavioral testing, and were given ad libitum access to food and water except during surgery and behavioral testing.

Drugs

In studies examining acute drug effects on PPI, saline (n = 8) or phencyclidine (PCP HCl) (0.25, 0.75, 1.5 mg/kg, n = 8, 8, and 9, respectively) was administered subcutaneously (SC) to female rats, and saline or 10 mg/kg PCP (n = 16 per group) was administered intraperitoneally (IP) to male rats. An additional study was conducted to establish a dose-response curve for apomorphine in female rats; in this study, vehicle (0.01% ascorbate/saline solution, n = 9) or apomorphine (0.1, 0.2, 0.4 mg/kg, n = 9, 9, and 8, respectively) was administered subcutaneously to female rats. In studies examining acute drug effects on animals pre-exposed to sustained administration of PCP, PCP (0.75 mg/kg) or apomorphine (0.2 mg/kg) was administered SC to female rats, based on dose-response effects in naive rats. For sustained administration, osmotic minipumps (Alza Corporation, Palo Alto, CA) filled to deliver 2.73 (n = 15) or 5.45 (n = 14) mg/day PCP HCl were implanted SC under halothane anesthesia. Control rats (n = 18) received silastic polymer pellets of approximately the same size and shape as the minipumps (Lipton et al. 1991). For experiments with repeated daily injections, male rats received IP injections of saline or 10 mg/kg PCP HCl daily for 14 consecutive days.

Surgery

Implantation of the subcutaneous minipumps was performed under halothane anesthesia. Rats were initially anesthetized by exposing them to a bell jar containing vaporized halothane. After this, a small area on the back, directly behind the head, was shaved and the rat was placed into a stereotaxic apparatus with a nose cone that delivered a mixture of halothane and air. The area to be incised was cleaned with an alcohol swab, while tail pinch and visual observation of respiratory movements were used to assess sufficient levels of anesthesia. Once the animal was sufficiently and stably anesthetized, a small incision was made with a scalpel, a small subcutaneous pocket was created using blunt dissection, a pump or two pellets were inserted and the incision was closed with wound clips. Explantation involved a similar procedure, with an incision made below the previous incision through which the pump or pellets were removed.

Apparatus

All experiments utilized four startle chambers (SR-LAB; San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA) housed in a sound-attenuated room with a 60-dB ambient noise level. Each startle chamber consisted of a plexiglas cylinder 8.2 cm in diameter resting on a 12.5 × 25.5 cm plexiglas stand. Acoustic stimuli and background noise were presented via a Radioshack Supertweeter mounted 24 cm above the plexiglas cylinder. Startle magnitude was detected and recorded by a microcomputer and interface assembly (San Diego Instruments) as transduced cylinder movement via a piezoelectric device mounted below the plexiglas stand. Startle magnitude was defined as the average of 100 readings collected every 1 ms beginning at the onset of the acoustic noise burst. Acoustic stimulus intensities and response sensitivities were calibrated (using an SR-LAB Startle Calibration System) to be nearly identical in each of the four startle chambers (maximum variability <1% of stimulus range and <5% of response ranges). Chambers were also balanced across all experimental groups. Sound levels were measured and calibrated with a sound level meter (Quest Electronics, Oconomowoc, WI), A scale (relative to 20 μN/M2), with microphone placed inside the plexiglas cylinder. Airpuff stimuli (10 psi, 40 ms duration) were regulated at the air supply, and were directed 2.0 cm above the animal's back via a copper tube of 0.5 cm diameter. The SR-LAB system controlled delivery of acoustic and airpuff stimuli. Methodological details can be found in published material (Geyer and Swerdlow 1998).

Testing Procedures

Three to seven days prior to testing, all rats were exposed to a brief “matching” startle session. Rats were placed in a startle chamber, (a closed plexiglas cylinder) and exposed to 5 minutes of 70-dB background noise followed by 17 PULSE trials of 40 ms 120-dB noise bursts (PULSE ALONE) and 5 PREPULSE + PULSE trials consisting of an 82-dB (12 dB above background) prepulse followed 100 ms by a 120-dB pulse. Data from this session were then used to assign rats to balanced groups according to their average PULSE ALONE startle magnitude.

Behavioral testing began 8–14 days after rat shipment arrival. Rats were brought to the laboratory for 30 minutes and then either treated with vehicle or drug (PCP or apomorphine) 10 minutes prior to a test session (Acute Test) or simply placed into the startle chamber (Sustained Test). Each session was approximately 19 minutes long and consisted of 5 minutes of 70-dB background followed by four trial types: PULSE ALONE noise bursts; and PREPULSE trials which consisted of 20 ms noise bursts 3, 6, or 12 dB above 70-dB background noise followed 100 ms by a PULSE. The session consisted of four “blocks”: blocks 1 and 4 consisted of four PULSE ALONE trials, and blocks 2 and 3 included both PULSE ALONE (eight trials per block) and 3, 6, and 12 PREPULSE + PULSE trials (five trials each per block), presented in pseudorandom order with a variable inter-trial interval (average of 15 seconds). In addition, interspersed between each stimulus trial, 100 ms of response was recorded during periods where no stimulus was presented. These trials were called NOSTIM trials and were used to assess gross motor activity during the test session but were not included in the calculation of intertrial intervals. Importantly, while NOSTIM activity can be used as a gross measure of drug “bioactivity,” it is not comparable to more sophisticated measures of rat locomotion or stereotyped behaviors. For example, doses of amphetamine that reduce PPI and stimulate locomotor activity have no significant effect on NOSTIM values (Mansbach et al. 1988).

Blocks 1 and 4 were used to calculate acoustic startle reflex habituation (magnitude reduction with repeated stimulus presentations). Although schizophrenia patients exhibit deficits in acoustic startle habituation (Geyer and Braff 1982), tactile (airpuff) startle habituation is more reliably affected by drugs (Geyer and Braff 1982). Hence, immediately upon completion of acoustic startle testing, startle was measured in some rats during exposure to 100 tactile stimuli (10 psi 40 ms airpuffs; PUFF) with a fixed inter-trial interval of 10 seconds.

TREATMENT AND TEST SCHEDULE

For a summary of treatments and test schedules, see Table 1.

Table 1 Treatment and Testing Schedule

Experiment 1: Subcutaneous PCP Minipumps

No published studies have documented the effects of PCP on acoustic startle in female rats. To assess the effects of subcutaneous PCP administration on the critical behavioral variables, startle and PPI were measured in one group of female rats (Group 1) 10 minutes after SC injection of vehicle or one of three doses of PCP (0.25, 0.75, and 1.5 mg/kg). These doses were selected based on published dose-response effects in male rats (Mansbach and Geyer 1989). A second group of rats (Group 2) was tested, 3 days after implantation of subcutaneous osmotic minipumps, to assess the effects of sustained PCP administration on startle, at a time when blood levels of PCP are elevated. Minipumps were removed after the fifth day of PCP exposure. Ten days later, these rats were retested to assess any lasting effects of the 5-day PCP exposure on startle, at a time when neurotoxic effects of such exposure have been observed (Ellison and Switzer 1993; Ellison 1994). Finally, in two “challenge” experiments, PPI was assessed in these rats after treatment with “threshold” doses of either PCP (Group 2A) or the DA agonist apomorphine (Group 2B), to determine whether sustained exposure to PCP increased the sensitivity of the rats to the PPI-disruptive effects of these agents. Some reports suggest that schizophrenia patients exhibit an increased behavioral and neurochemical responsivity to acute challenge with NMDA antagonists (cf. Domino 1964; Lahti et al. 1995a,b).

Experiment 2: Repeated Intraperitoneal PCP Injections

Startle was assessed in one group (Group 3) of rats 10 minutes after a single IP injection of vehicle or PCP (10 mg/kg), to determine the acute effects of a single IP injection of this dose of PCP on startle. These rats then received daily injections of vehicle or PCP. Startle was reassessed in these rats 10 minutes after their third daily IP injection, to determine the effects of these repeated injections on startle at a time of elevated PCP blood levels. A separate group of rats (Group 4) was given identical daily injections of vehicle or PCP, and was tested 6 hours after their third daily injection, to assess any lasting effects of PCP on startle that might be evident at that time. These rats continued to receive daily injections of either vehicle or PCP (10 mg/kg) for 14 consecutive days, and startle was assessed 2, 10, and 17 days after cessation of these repeated injections, at times when lasting behavioral and neurochemical effects of repeated PCP injections have been observed (Jentsch et al. 1997).

Data Analysis

For all ACUTE studies, rats were tested only once. For the SUSTAINED studies, rats remained in only one drug group (e.g., vehicle or PCP) and were tested repeatedly. All data were analyzed using an analysis of variance (ANOVA) with drug treatment as a between-subject factor and block and trial type as within-subject repeated measures. All post hoc comparisons were conducted using the Tukey-Kramer test. Due to the multiple comparisons pursued in these experiements, alpha was set at 0.01.

RESULTS

Experiment 1: PCP Minipumps

Effects on Startle Magnitude

PCP had no significant effects on startle magnitude in any of the acute or sustained treatment phases (data not shown). For female rats treated acutely with PCP via SC injections (to determine whether PCP reduces PPI in female rats), ANOVA revealed no significant effect of PCP dose on startle magnitude (F[3,28] = 2.83, ns). On day 3 after minipump implantation, ANOVA revealed no significant effect of PCP dose on startle magnitude (F[2,45] < 1). Similarly, on day 15 (10 days after minipump removal), ANOVA of startle magnitude revealed no significant effect of PCP dose (F[2,45] < 1). Lastly, on day 16 (11 days after minipump removal), ANOVA of startle magnitude revealed no significant effect of prior PCP dose (i.e., minipump group assignment) in rats challenged with subthreshold doses of PCP (F[2,21] < 1), or apomorphine (F[2,21] = 2.40, ns).

Effects on PPI

Acute SC administration of PCP in female rats resulted in a dose-dependent reduction in PPI, at doses comparable to those reported to reduce PPI in male rats (Mansbach and Geyer 1989) (Figure 1 ). No analyses of PPI revealed significant interactions of drug dose × trial block. For ease of presentation, all data are presented collapsed across Blocks 2 and 3. ANOVA of PPI with dose of PCP as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor revealed a significant effect of dose (F[3,28] = 8.38, p < .0004) and a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,56] = 35.65, p < .0001), but no significant dose × intensity interaction. Tukey-Kramer comparisons revealed that, compared to vehicle and lower doses of PCP, 1.5 mg/kg PCP significantly reduced PPI (p < .01, all comparisons). Sustained PCP administration (Figure 2A ) via osmotic minipump significantly reduced PPI on day 3 after minipump implantation. ANOVA of PPI with dose of PCP as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor revealed a significant effect of PCP dose (F[2,45] = 26.32, p < .0001), a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,45] = 53.68, p < .0001), and a significant dose × intensity interaction (F[4,45] = 3.29, p < .02). Tukey-Kramer comparisons revealed that both “high”-dose (5.45 mg/day) and “low”-dose PCP rats exhibited significantly less PPI compared to saline rats (p < .01 all comparisons).

Figure 1
figure 1

Percent prepulse inhibition, collapsed across all three prepulse intensities (3, 6, and 12 dB), in drug naive female rats treated with PCP (vehicle, 0.25, 0.75, or 1.5 mg/kg SC, n = 8, 8, 8, and 9) and female rats exposed to 5 days of continuous administration of vehicle (n = 9) or PCP (2.73, 5.45 mg/day, n = 8, 7) administered a subthreshold dose of PCP (0.25 mg/kg SC). Error bars indicate SEM.

Figure 2A
figure 2

(A) Percent prepulse inhibition in female rats treated with PCP for 3 days continuously via implanted subcutaneous pellets (vehicle, n = 18) or minipumps (2.73, 5.45 mg/d, n = 15, 14). [* indicates statistically significant reduction of PPI by both low (2.73 mg/d) and high (5.45 mg/d) doses of sustained PCP compared to vehicle by Tukey post-hoc analysis (alpha = 0.01).] (B) Percent prepulse inhibition in female rats 10 days after treatment with PCP for 5 days continuously via implanted subcutaneous pellets (vehicle) or minipumps (2.73, 5.45 mg/d). Error bars indicate SEM.

Sustained PCP exposure did not produce lasting PPI deficits (Figure 2B). Rats were tested on day 10 after removal of the minipumps, at which time long-term neuropathological changes have been observed (Ellison and Switzer 1993; Ellison 1994). ANOVA of PPI with PCP dose as a between-subject factor, and prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor revealed no significant effect of PCP dose (F[2,45] = 1.08, ns), a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,45] = 81.87, p < .0001), and no significant dose × intensity interaction (F[4,45] < 1).

After sustained exposure to PCP, rats did not exhibit changes in sensitivity to PCP effects on PPI (Figure 1). Half of these rats (n = 24) were “challenged” on day 11 after removal of minipumps with a subthreshold dose of PCP (0.25 mg/kg; based on ACUTE SC administration, Figure 1) to assess whether or not sensitivity to PCP had been altered by sustained treatment. ANOVA of PPI, with previous sustained PCP dose as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor, revealed no significant effect of previous PCP dose (F[2,21] < 1), a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,21] = 72.05, p < .0001) and a significant dose × intensity interaction (F[4,21] = 3.09, p < .03). This significant interaction reflected the fact that, compared to vehicle and high-dose PCP groups, PPI in low-PCP-dose group rats was nonsignificantly reduced for 6- and 12-dB trials, but not for 3-dB trials (p > .05, all comparisons). The other half of the rats (n = 23) were challenged on day 11 after minipump removal with a subthreshold dose of apomorphine (0.2 mg/kg; based on ACUTE SC administration, Figure 3 ) to assess whether or not sensitivity to dopamine receptor-mediated effects of apomorphine on PPI had been altered by sustained treatment with PCP. ANOVA of PPI with previous PCP dose as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor, revealed no significant effect of previous PCP dose (F[2,21] < 1), a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,21] = 24.75, p < .0001) and no significant dose × intensity interaction (F[4,21] < 1).

Figure 3
figure 3

Percent prepulse inhibition, collapsed across all three prepulse intensities (3, 6, and 12 dB), in drug naive female rats treated with apomorphine (vehicle, 0.1, 0.2, or 0.4 mg/kg SC, n = 9, 9, 9, and 8) and female rats exposed to 5 days of continuous administration of vehicle (n = 9) or PCP (2.73, 5.45 mg/day, n = 7, 7) administered a subthreshold dose of apomorphine (0.2 mg/kg SC). Error bars indicate SEM.

Effects on Habituation

Lastly, 10 days after minipump removal, sustained exposure to PCP did not produce long-term effects on startle habituation to repeated acoustic PULSES or to repeated “airpuffs” (Table 2). For acoustic stimuli (Blocks 1 vs. 4), ANOVA of startle magnitude with previous PCP dose as a between-subject factor revealed no significant effect of dose (F[2,45] = 1.14, ns), a significant effect of trial block (F[2,45] = 68.24, p < .0001) and no significant dose × block interaction (F[4,45] < 1). For tactile stimuli (10 blocks of 10 trials), ANOVA of startle magnitude revealed no significant effect of previous PCP dose (F[2,45] < 1), a significant effect of block (F[9,45] = 19.13, p < .0001) and no significant dose × block interaction (F[4,45] = 1.62, ns). Lastly, ANOVA of percent change score revealed no treatment effects on reflex habituation (data not shown).

Table 2 Effects of Sustained PCP Exposure on Startle Magnitude and Habituation (Mean Startle Magnitude (SEM))

Experiment 2: Repeated PCP Injections

Effects on Startle Magnitude

To assess a model of subchronic PCP administration (Jentsch et al. 1997), rats were tested at specific time points during and after daily administration of vehicle or 10 mg/kg PCP IP. Via this route of administration, PCP had no significant effects on startle magnitude. ANOVA of startle magnitude with PCP dose as a between-subject factor revealed no significant effect of acute PCP (F[1,14] < 1). Similarly, ANOVA of startle magnitude with PCP dose as a between-subject factor revealed no significant effects of PCP dose on startle magnitude, when measured on day 3 out of the 14 days of repeated daily injections (either 10 minutes or 6 hours after PCP injection; 10 minutes: (F[1,6] < 1), 6 hours: (F[1,14] < 1), on day 2 after cessation of repeated injections (F[1,14] = 2.13, ns), on day 10 after cessation of repeated injections (F[1,14] < 1), or on day 17 after cessation of repeated injections (F[1,14] = 2.94, ns) (data not shown).

Effects on PPI

Acute IP administration of PCP significantly decreased PPI on day 1. ANOVA of PPI with PCP dose as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor, revealed a significant effect of PCP (F[1,14] = 141, p < .0001), a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,14] = 11.90, p < .0002) and no significant dose × intensity interaction (F[2,14] = 3.04, ns) (Figure 4 ). As in Experiment 1, no analyses of PPI revealed significant interactions of drug dose × trial block. For ease of presentation, all data are presented collapsed across Blocks 2 and 3.

Figure 4
figure 4

Percent prepulse inhibition in male rats treated with PCP (vehicle or 10 mg/kg IP). Error bars indicate SEM.

On day 3 of 14 days of repeated injections, PCP significantly decreased PPI 10 minutes, but not 6 hours after IP administration (Figure 5 ). For rats tested 10 minutes after IP PCP injection, ANOVA of PPI with PCP dose as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor, revealed a significant effect of PCP (F[1,6] = 33.59, p < .002), no significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,6] < 1) and no significant dose × intensity interaction (F[1,6] = 2.31, ns). For rats tested 6 hours after IP PCP injection on day 3 out of 14 days of repeated daily injections, ANOVA of PPI with PCP dose as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor, revealed no significant effect of PCP (F[1,14] < 1), a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,14] = 99.55, p < .0001) and no significant dose × intensity interaction (F[2,14] < 1).

Figure 5
figure 5

(A) Percent prepulse inhibition in male rats 10 minutes after treatment with PCP (vehicle or 10 mg/kg IP, n = 8 for both groups) on day 3 of repeated injections of PCP (vehicle or 10 mg/kg IP). (B) Percent prepulse inhibition in male rats 6 hours after treatment with PCP (vehicle or 10 mg/kg IP, n = 8 for both groups) on day 3 of repeated injections of PCP (vehicle or 10 mg/kg IP). Error bars indicate SEM.

Rats demonstrated no significant effects of PCP on PPI, when tested 2, 10, or 17 days after administration of the last of 14 daily injections of PCP, during a time period reported by others (Jentsch et al. 1997) to be characterized by both neurochemical and “cognitive” changes resulting from repeated PCP administration (Figure 6 ). For post-PCP day 2, ANOVA of PPI with PCP dose as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor, revealed no significant effect of PCP (F[1,14] = 3.32, ns), a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,14] = 39.99, p < .0001) and no significant dose × intensity interaction (F[2,14] = 1.61, ns) (Figure 6A). For post-PCP day 10, ANOVA of PPI with PCP dose as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor, revealed no significant effect of PCP (F[1,14] = 1.80, ns), a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,14] = 14.44, p < .0001) and no significant dose × intensity interaction (F[2,14] < 1). For post-PCP day 17, ANOVA with PCP dose as a between-subject factor, and with prepulse intensity as a within-subject factor, revealed no significant effect of PCP (F[1,14] < 1), a significant effect of prepulse intensity (F[2,14] = 68.89, p < .0001) and no significant dose × intensity interaction (F[2,14] < 1) (Figures 6 B and C).

Figure 6
figure 6

(A) Percent prepulse inhibition in male rats 2 days after 14 daily injections of PCP (vehicle or 10 mg/kg IP). (B) Percent prepulse inhibition in male rats 10 days after 14 daily injections of PCP (vehicle or 10 mg/kg IP). (C) Percent prepulse inhibition in male rats 17 days after 14 days of daily injections of PCP (vehicle or 10 mg/kg IP).n = 8 for each group. Error bars indicate SEM.

Effects on Habituation

Tested 10 or 17 days after exposure to 14 days of daily PCP injections, rats did not demonstrate long-term effects on habituation to repeated acoustic PULSES or to repeated “airpuffs” (day 10) (Table 2). For acoustic stimuli (Blocks 1 vs. 4), ANOVA of startle magnitude with PCP dose as a between-subject factor revealed no significant effects of PCP dose (day 10: F[1,14] = 1.04, ns; day 17: F[1,14] = 1.46, ns) and no significant dose × block interactions (days 10 and 17: F[1,14] < 1). For tactile stimuli (10 blocks of 10 trials), ANOVA of startle magnitude revealed no significant effect of PCP dose (day 10: F[1,14] = 2.14, ns; day 17: F[1,14] < 1) and no significant dose × block interactions (days 10 and 17: F[9,14] < 1) (data not shown). Lastly, as in Experiment 1, ANOVA using a percent change score revealed no significant treatment effects on reflex habituation (data not shown).

DISCUSSION

The “NMDA Hypothesis” of schizophrenia has been strengthened by reports of the effects of PCP and ketamine in normal subjects and schizophrenia patients, and of the effects of acute and chronic PCP administration in preclinical models. In humans, acute exposure to PCP or ketamine has been reported to produce both symptoms (e.g., hallucinations and sensory distortions) and cognitive deficits on formal neuropsychological testing, similar to those observed in patients with schizophrenia (cf. Pearlson 1981; Javitt 1987); occasionally, symptoms of PCP “psychosis” can become a lasting condition (Rainey and Crowder 1975; Burns and Lerner 1976; Allen and Young 1978; McCarron et al. 1981; Javitt and Zukin 1991). Acute administration of another NMDA antagonist (ketamine) can produce both “positive” and “negative” schizophrenia-like symptoms in control subjects (Krystal et al. 1994; Malhotra et al. 1996; Breier et al. 1997; Malhotra et al. 1997), and can increase glucose metabolism in prefrontal cortex (Breier et al. 1997); similar regional metabolic abnormalities are associated with both schizophrenia symptoms and neuropsychological performance deficits (cf. Frith 1996; Andreasen 1997). Sustained PCP exposure in non-schizophrenic humans who abuse PCP has been associated with lasting neurological and behavioral abnormalities (Fauman and Fauman 1980; Graeven and Sharp 1981) that have been compared to some of the abnormalities seen in schizophrenia patients.

Other observations suggest that schizophrenia patients may also be specifically clinically and neurochemically responsive to NMDA antagonists. Studies of schizophrenia patients exposed to NMDA antagonists have identified ketamine-induced increases in blood flow (rCBF) in anterior cingulate cortex (Lahti et al. 1995a), and an exacerbation of patient-specific symptoms with PCP (cf. Domino 1964) or ketamine (Lahti et al. 1995b).

In rats, acute administration of PCP produces changes in behavior (Mansbach and Geyer, 1989), neurochemistry (Snell et al. 1988), and brain metabolism (Tamminga et al. 1987; Weissman et al. 1989; Ellison and Keys 1996) that have been used to model pathological processes associated with schizophrenia. Long-standing changes in behavioral and brain measures also result from sustained PCP exposure in rats. Methods of sustained or subchronic PCP administration, via sustained-release osmotic minipumps or repeated daily injections, produce lasting changes in behavior, neurochemistry (Jentsch et al. 1997, 1998), brain metabolism and cell morphology (Ellison and Switzer 1993; Ellison 1994).

Neurochemical changes after repeated daily IP administration of PCP include decreased dopamine utilization in prefrontal cortex (Jentsch et al. 1997, 1998), and these changes are accompanied by behavioral deficits in T-maze alternation (Jentsch et al. 1997) and stress and psychostimulant-induced hyperlocomotion (Jentsch et al. 1998). The neuropathological changes associated with sustained PCP exposure include neuronal degeneration in retrosplenial cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, entorhinal cortex, hippocampus and olfactory regions (Ellison and Switzer 1993; Ellison 1994). Metabolic changes in rats after sustained PCP exposure include elevations in glucose utilization in some of those same areas (Ellison and Keys 1996).

Studies of the neuroanatomy and pharmacology of PPI suggest that it is regulated by brain circuitry that includes the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, basolateral amygdala, striatum, pallidum and pontine tegmentum (cf. Swerdlow and Geyer 1998). Investigations of the specific neuroanatomical substrates for NMDA antagonist effects on PPI have demonstrated that PPI is reduced after infusion of NMDA antagonists into the amygdala (Wan and Swerdlow 1997; Bakshi and Geyer 1998), the nucleus accumbens (Reijmers et al. 1995), and the dorsal hippocampus (Bakshi and Geyer 1998). It has been suggested that the loss of PPI after systemic administration of NMDA antagonists—as was observed in the present studies—may reflect the activity of these drugs within multiple brain regions (Bakshi and Geyer 1998).

The present studies were designed to determine whether sustained PCP exposure could reproduce in rats a specific deficit in sensorimotor gating that has been reported in schizophrenia patients (Braff et al. 1978, 1992; Grillon et al. 1992; Bolino et al. 1994). Rats implanted with osmotic minipumps releasing a continuous amount of PCP over a 5-day period exhibited PPI deficits while the minipumps were in place. However, 10 days after removal of the pumps, at a time when significant neuropathological and neurochemical changes have been reported (Ellison and Switzer 1993; Ellison 1994; Ellison and Keys 1996), no significant PCP-induced changes in PPI were evident. On day 3 of a 14 daily repeated-injection paradigm, PCP produced PPI deficits 10 minutes but not 6 hours after injection (i.e., an effect linked with acute but not subchronic PCP effects). In this paradigm, PCP-induced changes in PPI were not observed after cessation of 14 consecutive daily injections, at times when other significant behavioral and neurochemical effects of PCP have been reported (Jentsch et al. 1997, 1998).

While the neurochemical, metabolic, and neuropathological changes caused by sustained and/or subchronic exposure to PCP may certainly model brain processes that are critical to the pathophysiology and cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, the present studies demonstrate that these lasting PCP-induced changes do not cause a lasting functional disturbance in substrates regulating sensorimotor gating in rats. Certainly, this lack of sustained changes in PPI, at times when other behavioral effects of PCP are reported, may reflect a relative insensitivity of PPI to meaningful effects of sustained or subchronic PCP on the brain. To the degree that PPI is regulated by brain substrates responsible for cross-species changes in sensorimotor gating, the present results suggest that the relative loss of sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia does not reflect brain pathology modelled solely by sustained or subchronic PCP exposure.