Elsevier

Trends in Food Science & Technology

Volume 96, February 2020, Pages 268-270
Trends in Food Science & Technology

It is important to differentiate sensory property from the material property

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tifs.2019.12.014Get rights and content

Highlights

  • A sensory property is human perception of its corresponding material property.

  • Sensory property and material property are two different properties by nature.

  • Stevens' law only indicates a link but not an equivalence between a sensory property and its corresponding physical stimulus.

  • A sensory property is not most feasible to be measured in vitro by an instrument.

Abstract

A sensory property (or sensory perception) is human's perception or sensory response to a material stimulus (or stimuli). Despite of the intrinsic relationship between a sensory property and its corresponding material property, the two properties are fundamentally different. However, in sensory practice, the two different properties are very often mixed up and instrumental measured material property is often mistaken as sensory property. Correct differentiation of the two properties is critically important for fundamental understanding of human sensory and in particular for practical sensory analysis. While it is perfectly feasible using an instrument to measure material property, it is much more complicated when using an instrument for sensory perception prediction.

Section snippets

Sensory property is a human-perceived property of a material, but not a material property

Sensory property and material property are two properties fundamentally different. The delicate difference between the two can be demonstrated by their very different mechanisms of detection/sensation and the very different mathematical models used for data analysis. Sensory property is by nature human's physiological as well as psychological responses to external stimuli acting alone or in combination, via channels of vision, hearing, smelling, tasting, and touching, either independently or

Can a sensory property be instrumentally measured?

While it is perfectly feasible to measure a material property using various physical instruments or devices, it is in principle not feasible (at least with current technology) to instrumentally measure the sensory property of a material. There were many cases in literature reporting sensory perception based on instrumental measurement, in particular in the perception of food texture (Chen & Opara, 2013). Unfortunately, many of such cases mixed up sensory properties perceived by human with the

Derived sensory properties are more complicated

We all know that sensory perception and food preference are much more complicated than just five basic tastes. Instead of being dominated by one single material property, most sensory properties are originated or derived from two or more material stimuli in complicated combination (Chen, 2014). Analysis of derived sensory properties is simply beyond the prediction of Stevens' law. Examples of derived sensory property are extensive. A convenient example is the sweet-sour taste, a very common

Oral sensory perception is not from the food alone, but from the food-saliva mixture

Another very important fact which attracted a lot of attention recently is the critical role of saliva in sensory perception of food flavor and texture. It is now clear that what one perceives during an eating process are not the properties of the food itself but the properties of the mixture of food and saliva (Mosca & Chen, 2017). Saliva is a biofluid with individually varied properties (Neyraud & Morzel, 2019). Extensive studies have confirmed that complicated interactions occur during food

Acknowledgement

The author thanks financial support for this research work from the National Key Research and Development of the Ministry of Science and Technology of China (Grant number 1110KZ0117057) and Natural Science Funding Council (grant number 31871885) of China.

References (25)

  • L. Chen et al.

    Approaches to analysis and modeling texture in fresh and processed foods: A review

    Journal of Food Engineering

    (2013)
  • J.V. Verhagen et al.

    The neurocognitive bases of human multimodal food perception: Sensory integration

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2006)
  • M. Bourne

    Food texture and viscosity: Concept and measurement

    (2002)
  • N. Brossard et al.

    “Oral” tribological study on the astringency sensation of red wines

    Journal of Texture Studies

    (2016)
  • J. Chen

    Food oral processing: Some important underpinning principles of eating and sensory perception

    Food Structure

    (2014)
  • B. De Lima Lucas et al.

    Salivary alpha-amylase and hormones levels of young adults with different body composition

    Journal of Texture Studies

    (2019)
  • D. De Ridder et al.

    The brain, obesity and addiction: An EEG neuroimaging study

    Scientific Reports

    (2016)
  • M. Glumac et al.

    Saliva could act as an emulsifier during oral processing of oil/fat

    Journal of Texture Studies

    (2019)
  • E.B. Goldstein

    Sensation and perception

    (2010)
  • R.C. Hootman
    (1992)
  • M. Johnson

    Observations on Dr. Peleg's comments regarding texture profile analysis

    Journal of Texture Studies

    (2019)
  • R.S.J. Keast et al.

    An overview of binary taste-taste interactions

    Food Quality and Preference

    (2002)
  • Cited by (17)

    • Flavor release from traditional dry-cured pork during oral processing

      2022, Food Science and Human Wellness
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, the relationship between food oral processing and sensory perception are not yet fully understood [52]. Sensory perception is a human's perception or response to a material stimulus [8]. In fact, food flavor perception is not only a sensory perception, but also a complex relationship based on a variety of senses, including psychological, physical and chemical senses.

    • Chemical instrumental analysis versus human evaluation to measure sensory properties of dairy products: What is fit for purpose?

      2021, International Dairy Journal
      Citation Excerpt :

      What is perplexing here is that this is widely accepted, while at the same time many argue that the relationships between properties are good enough to allow instruments to indirectly measure sensory properties. For example, very few rheologists would consider it acceptable to use sensory evaluations to determine rheological properties, so, one would have to ask why anyone would think it is any more acceptable to use rheology to determine sensory properties (Chen, 2020). In both scenarios the same correlation is being used, the only difference is the direction of the interpolation.

    • The Stevens law and the derivation of sensory perception

      2021, Journal of Future Foods
      Citation Excerpt :

      An n value of as low as 0.22 has also been observed for thickness perception against the objective viscosity measurement [5]. Despite of wide acceptance of the Stevens law and its modified versions, limits and contradictories still exist when applying this theory for practical sensory analysis [1]. One obvious drawback is the self-inconsistency of the units of the equation.

    • Fine wine flavour perception and appreciation: Blending neuronal processes, tasting methods and expertise

      2021, Trends in Food Science and Technology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Smith (2017) claims that the flavour perception system tracks the presence of flavours in foods and liquids because flavours are properties of the foods or liquids. Then, flavour is the sensory property (or sensory perception) resulting from the human response to a flavourant that is its material property or stimulus (Chen, 2020) as odour and odourant are defined by Hudson (2000). Among the senses contributing to flavour, the singularity of olfaction (ortho- and retronasal pathways) lies in the straightforward transmission of stimuli from the receptor cells in the nose to the orbito-frontal cortex (OFC) where conscious processing takes place without passing through the thalamus (Shepherd, 2017).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text